Neuroendocrine–immune relationships between emotion regulation and health in patients with rheumatoid arthritis

H. van Middendorp, R. Geenen, M. J. Sorbi, L. J. P. van Doornen and J. W. J. Bijlsma1

Department of Health Psychology, Utrecht University and 1 Department of Rheumatology and Clinical Immunology, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands.

Correspondence to: H. van Middendorp, Department of Health Psychology, Utrecht University, PO Box 80140, 3508 TC Utrecht, The Netherlands. E-mail: H.vanMiddendorp{at}fss.uu.nl


    Abstract
 Top
 Abstract
 Introduction
 Methods
 Results
 Discussion
 References
 
Objectives. Emotion regulation is hypothesized to be related to health through neuroendocrine–immune changes. This study examined the role of the neuroendocrine variables 24-h urinary cortisol and noradrenaline, and the immune variable serum interleukin 6 as mediators between emotion regulation styles and health (perceived health and disease activity: erythrocyte sedimentation rate (ESR) and Thompson joint score).

Methods. Sixty patients with rheumatoid arthritis (mean age 59.0±11.2 yr; 38 female) participated.

Results. Emotion regulation was not associated with immune functioning or disease activity, but it was somewhat related to neuroendocrine functioning: one of the emotion regulation styles, ambiguity, was related to noradrenaline in women (r = 0.39) but not in men. The indicators of neuroendocrine functioning (cortisol and noradrenaline) were correlated (r = 0.40), as were indicators of immune functioning (interleukin 6) and inflammatory activity (ESR; r = 0.53), but analyses did not indicate a role of these physiological variables in mediating between emotion regulation and health: neuroendocrine variables were not related to interleukin 6 or ESR, and none of the physiological parameters was correlated with joint score or perceived health.

Conclusions. To examine whether the proposed mediational processes apply to individual patients, a longitudinal within-subjects design is needed. In our cross-sectional study, emotion regulation was somewhat related to neuroendocrine functioning, but our study did not uncover a potential mediational role of cortisol, noradrenaline or interleukin 6 in the relationship between emotion regulation and health in rheumatoid arthritis.

KEY WORDS: Emotions, Psychological adaptation, Alexithymia, Stress, Erythrocyte sedimentation rate, Interleukin-6, Noradrenaline, Cortisol, Psychoneuroimmunology, Rheumatoid arthritis


    Introduction
 Top
 Abstract
 Introduction
 Methods
 Results
 Discussion
 References
 
Activity of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and the sympathetic–adrenal–medullary (SAM) system, which are critically involved in both psychological and inflammatory processes, may mediate bidirectional relationships between emotion regulation and the immune response and health in rheumatoid arthritis (RA) [1–3] (Fig. 1).



View larger version (13K):
[in this window]
[in a new window]
 
FIG. 1. Model of potential physiological mediators between emotion regulation and health. Solid lines represent positive relationships, dotted lines represent negative relationships. The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and the sympathetic–adrenal–medullary (SAM) system are critically involved in the inflammatory process, but can also be activated by stress reactions or be down-regulated after persistent stress. Cortisol and catecholamines, including noradrenaline, are end-products of the HPA and SAM systems, respectively. Cortisol and catecholamines have shown both immunosuppressive and immunostimulating effects, depending on their concentration and receptor-binding capacity. During inflammation, proinflammatory cytokines such as IL-6 activate the neuroendocrine stress system. Proinflammatory cytokines that signal the brain may also be involved in changing how individuals perceive their health, by reducing psychological, social and physical well-being and functioning (so-called sickness behaviour).

 
Emotion regulation refers to the processes by which individuals influence which emotions they have, when they have them, and how they experience and express these emotions [3]. Examples of emotion regulation strategies are the ability to recognize one's emotions, the intensity of emotions, and the expression or suppression of emotions. Experiencing emotions intensely and suppressing emotional experiences are suggested to be physiologically arousing [3, 4]. The resulting chronically elevated sympathetic nervous system activity may affect health, especially in a physiologically compromised system such as RA. Emotion regulation styles that are physiologically relaxing or neutral may counteract adverse health consequences [5]. A few studies suggested that emotion regulation affects neuroendocrine and immune functioning [3, 6].

Our group demonstrated relationships between emotion regulation and health in patients with RA [7]. The purpose of the present cross-sectional study was to examine the role of the physiological variables 24-h urinary cortisol, and noradrenaline and serum interleukin 6 (IL-6) as mediators between emotion regulation styles on the one hand and inflammatory activity (erythrocyte sedimentation rate, ESR), joint score and perceived health on the other.


    Methods
 Top
 Abstract
 Introduction
 Methods
 Results
 Discussion
 References
 
Participants and procedure
Participants were recruited by rheumatologists and rheumatology nurses of the rheumatology divisions of seven hospitals in the Utrecht area, The Netherlands, participating in the Utrecht Rheumatoid Arthritis Cohort study group. A letter with information on the study and a questionnaire booklet were handed out to patients during their regular check-ups. Emotion regulation questionnaires were filled in by 345 patients. On average, 13 (S.D. = 5) months later, physiological variables, inflammatory activity, joint score and perceived health were measured in 72 volunteers from among these patients at the baseline assessment before the start of a randomized controlled trial. This trial compared the effects of home-based emotional disclosure (experimental intervention) and discussion of time management (control condition). The effects of this trial will be reported in a separate paper. Patients had indicated which medication they used in the 4 weeks preceding the assessments. Twelve patients who used glucocorticoids were excluded. Data on the remaining 60 participants are analysed in the present study. The characteristics of the participants are reported in Table 1. Compared with the remainder of the 345 patients, they were characterized by less frequent use of analgesics, more frequent use of non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs), lower scores on negative affect, and higher scores on positive affect and physical functioning. Written consent was obtained from all participants and both studies were approved by the ethical committee of the University Medical Center Utrecht.


View this table:
[in this window]
[in a new window]
 
TABLE 1. Patient characteristics of 60 patients with rheumatoid arthritis

 
Patients collected a 24-h urine sample, from the second void of the first day up to and including the first void of the second day, and filled in questionnaires on perceived health. On the second day in the afternoon (between 13:30 and 15:30 h), patients came to the University Medical Center Utrecht for a joint examination and to have blood drawn for cytokine and ESR assessment.

Assessments
Emotion regulation
Four psychometrically sound questionnaires were administered to assess how individuals typically respond to emotional situations: the Toronto Alexithymia Scale 20 [8], the Ambivalence over Emotional Expressiveness Questionnaire [9], the Self-Assessment Questionnaire Nijmegen [10] and the Five Expressivity Facet Scales [11]. Principal component analysis was used to summarize the 14 scales of these questionnaires into four aspects of emotion regulation [7]. ‘Ambiguity’ is a combination of alexithymia (difficulty identifying and describing emotions) and ambivalence on expressing emotions. ‘Control’ reflects keeping feelings inside and trying to restrain feelings and be rational. ‘Orientation’ encompasses attending to and valuing emotions, and experiencing emotions intensely. ‘Expression’ is the expression of both negative and positive emotions towards others. The internal consistency of the styles of emotion regulation varied between {alpha} = 0.63 for orientation and {alpha} = 0.79 for ambiguity [7].

Neuroendocrine variables
Urine samples were processed and stored according to guidelines, with HCl added after 24-h collection for noradrenaline analyses [12]. Cortisol concentration was measured after dichloromethane extraction using an immunometric technique on an Advantage Chemiluminescense System (Nichols Institute Diagnostics, San Juan Capistrano, CA, USA). Noradrenaline excretion was measured by standard high-performance liquid chromatography with electrochemical detection (HPLC-ECD). Output (in nmol/24 h) was corrected for urinary volume.

Immune variable
Serum levels of IL-6 were determined using a high-sensitivity enzyme-linked immunoabsorbent sandwich assay (R & D Systems, Minneapolis, MN, USA). One value of IL-6 was lower than the detection limit (0.06 pg/ml). This missing value was set to the detection limit value.

Disease activity
ESR was assessed by the method of Westergren. The Thompson joint score [13] was calculated from tender and swollen joint counts.

Perceived health
Four psychometrically sound self-report questionnaires were administered: the shortened Profile of Mood States [14], the Impact of Rheumatic diseases on General health and Lifestyle [15], the Health Assessment Questionnaire [16] and the Rheumatoid Arthritis Disease Activity Index [17]. Principal component analysis on the 16 scales of these instruments indicated five aspects of perceived health [7]: ‘negative affect’ (depressed and tense mood), ‘positive affect’ (energetic and cheerful mood), ‘social functioning’ (actual and perceived social support), ‘physical functioning’ [self-care, disability (reversed sign) and mobility] and ‘disease activity’ (pain and self-assessed disease activity). The internal consistency of these components varied from {alpha} = 0.59 for social functioning to {alpha} = 0.91 for negative affect and disease activity [7].

Statistical analysis
One instance of incomplete urine collection was treated as a missing value. A single extreme outlier on cortisol output and one on joint score were replaced by the value 2.5 S.D. above the mean. All variables showed normal to nearly normal distributions (highest skewness value for Thompson joint score, 1.63). Parametric analyses were performed. Transforming the variables logarithmically or using non-parametric statistics did not significantly change the results.

Pearson correlation coefficients between the variables of emotion regulation, physiology and health were calculated. For significant associations between emotion regulation and health (disease activity and perceived health), we used common methods to test whether these associations were mediated by physiological variables. To take account of multiple testing, the Bonferroni criterion was used: the standard {alpha} level of 0.05 was divided by the number of correlations per research question.

Independent sample t-tests (for dichotomous variables) or correlations were used to find which demographic or disease-related variables (sex, age, disease duration, comorbidity and medication use) were related to emotion regulation, physiology and health. In post hoc covariance analysis, partial correlations adjusted for potentially confounding covariates were computed. SPSS 11.5 was used.


    Results
 Top
 Abstract
 Introduction
 Methods
 Results
 Discussion
 References
 
Relationships of emotion regulation with physiology
Ambiguity (r = 0.40, P = 0.002), orientation (r = –0.44, P<0.001) and expression (r = –0.30, P = 0.023) were correlated with noradrenaline output. After Bonferroni correction, the association between expression and noradrenaline was no longer significant. Emotion regulation was not significantly correlated with cortisol or IL-6.

Mutual relationships between physiological variables
The two neuroendocrine measures cortisol and noradrenaline were significantly correlated (r = 0.40, P = 0.002), but did not correlate with IL-6 (r = –0.01 and r = –0.05, respectively).

Relationships of physiology with health
Cortisol and noradrenaline were not significantly correlated with ESR (r = –0.04 and r = –0.19, respectively), but IL-6 was (r = 0.53, P<0.001). None of the physiological parameters was related to the joint score (r = –0.02, –0.08 and 0.02, respectively). A significant correlation between noradrenaline and social functioning (r = –0.32, P = 0.014) became non-significant after Bonferroni correction. All other correlations were not significant.

Physiological mediation
Because emotion regulation was not correlated with ESR and joint score, and the physiological parameters were not related to perceived health, it was not applicable to examine potential physiological mediators between emotion regulation and health.

Post hoc covariance analyses
After adjusting for the covariates sex, age and use of NSAIDs, correlations between emotion regulation and physiology remained in the same direction but became lower. The correlation between ambiguity and noradrenaline became r = 0.26 (P = 0.057) and that between orientation and noradrenaline became r = –0.21 (P = 0.119). Sex was predominantly responsible for this weakening effect. Men were higher on ambiguity, lower on orientation and higher on noradrenaline than women. Ambiguity was correlated with noradrenaline in women (r = 0.39) but not in men (r = 0.03), while men and women did not differ in correlations of orientation and noradrenaline (r = –0.21 and –0.19, respectively).


    Discussion
 Top
 Abstract
 Introduction
 Methods
 Results
 Discussion
 References
 
Although the joint regulatory activation of the HPA and SAM systems by corticotrophin releasing hormone was reflected by a mutual association of noradrenaline and cortisol output, emotion regulation showed a moderate association with noradrenaline only. The association between ambiguity and noradrenaline is in agreement with inhibition theories stating that keeping emotions inside requires physiological work [4], and with empirical findings showing associations of the related constructs suppression and alexithymia with higher basal neuroendocrine levels and blood pressure [18, 19]. Ambiguity and noradrenaline were only related in women. Prospective research should examine the suggestion that frequent use of ambiguity as the emotion regulation style could be unhealthy in the long run in women with RA.

The moderate association of orientation with lower noradrenaline does not support the hypothesis that experiencing emotions intensely is associated with physiological hyperarousal [4]. The association largely reflects the higher noradrenaline and lower orientation scores in men than in women. Although not significant after Bonferroni adjustment, the association of expression with lower noradrenaline output is in agreement with physiological relaxation theories proposing that expressing feelings has a physiological effect through resolution of inhibition or through stress reduction [4, 5].

The proinflammatory cytokine IL-6 and the inflammation indicator ESR were highly correlated. IL-6 is a potent activator of the HPA axis [20], but in our study cortisol and noradrenaline output levels were not related to serum IL-6 or ESR, perhaps because we used 24-h measures of neuroendocrine functioning while IL-6 was tested at a single time point. Although neuroendocrine–immune correlations have been reported in RA [e.g. 21], our results agree with studies finding no relation between neuroendocrine functioning and IL-6 [22] or ESR [23]. Perhaps in established RA, the SAM system loses control of the immune system and inflammation [2] and HPA system activity is too low for the degree of inflammation [24]. Acknowledging the methodological limitations of our study, deficient neuroendocrine–immune communication might explain our observation of limited association between emotion regulation and the disease process in RA.

Proinflammatory cytokines such as IL-6 are suggested to trigger a reduction in psychological, social and physical well-being [25], but our study did not find a correlation between IL-6 and perceived health. Because of individual differences in adaptation to the disease, perceived health may have become independent from physiological changes associated with repeated inflammation.

Styles of emotion regulation were related to perceived health but not to disease activity. Since cortisol, noradrenaline and IL-6 were not related to perceived health, the examination of a mediational model made no sense. Our study did not provide a single indication for physiological mediation of the relationship between emotion regulation and health.

A limitation of our study is the relatively small sample size. Consequently, we were unable to test a full model at once, controlling for the other variables and directions of relationships. Although emotion regulation styles are considered and proven stable characteristics of individuals [11], we cannot fully rule out the possibility that the long time interval between the self-report and physiological measures hampered our test of the mediational hypothesis. Another restriction is that our cross-sectional study examined individual differences. Beside the present condition and medication, past inflammatory and psychosocial stress will have determined whether specific physiological, and perhaps psychological, response systems have become up-regulated or down-regulated. We cannot generalize beyond our cross-sectional design to processes in individual patients. For this, our results need confirmation in a longitudinal within-subjects design. A final restriction is that our conclusion relates to patients with RA who receive common medication that might affect physiological responding.

To conclude, our cross-sectional study did not uncover a potential mediational role of cortisol, noradrenaline or IL-6 in the relationship between emotion regulation and health in RA. We found a moderate association between emotion regulation and neuroendocrine functioning, as well as an association between immune activation and inflammatory activity, but the psychological–neuroendocrine and the immune–inflammatory systems were not related, and were mostly unrelated to health.


    Acknowledgments
 
This study was financially supported by the Dutch Arthritis Association. We thank the rheumatologists and rheumatology nurses of the Arthritis Research Foundation Utrecht (SRU) for recruitment of participants; Etiënne Blaas of the Department of Rheumatology and Clinical Immunology of the University Medical Center Utrecht (UMCU) for patient assessment; Kim Jacobs of the Laboratory of Rheumatology and Clinical Immunology UMCU for the processing of urine samples and cytokine analyses; Dr Inge Maitimu and colleagues of the Laboratory of Endocrinology UMCU for cortisol analyses; Dr Ron Voorbij and Arendje Peterusma of the Laboratory of Clinical Chemistry UMCU for noradrenaline analyses; and Hanna Zijlstra, Tanja van Meerveld, Nettie den Breejen, Femke Vergeer, Ariëtte van Wijngaarden, Jolien Spoelstra, Nicole Dreessen, Ruth Haakman and Annechien van den Bosch for their contribution to data collection.

The authors have declared no conflicts of interest.


    References
 Top
 Abstract
 Introduction
 Methods
 Results
 Discussion
 References
 

  1. Straub RH, Dhabhar FS, Bijlsma JW, Cutolo M. How psychological stress via hormones and nerve fibers may exacerbate rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 2005;52:16–26.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
  2. Baerwald CGO, Burmester GR, Krause A. Interactions of autonomic nervous, neuroendocrine, and immune systems in rheumatoid arthritis. Rheum Dis Clin North Am 2000;26:841–57.[ISI][Medline]
  3. Gross JJ. Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998;74:224–37.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
  4. Lepore SJ, Greenberg MA, Bruno M, Smyth JM. Expressive writing and health: self-regulation of emotion-related experience, physiology, and behavior. In: Lepore SJ, Smyth JM, eds. The writing cure: how expressive writing promotes health and emotional well-being. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 2002: 99–117.
  5. Lutgendorf SK, Ullrich P. Cognitive processing, disclosure, and health: psychological and physiological mechanisms. In: Lepore SJ, Smyth JM, eds. The writing cure: how expressive writing promotes health and emotional well-being. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 2002:177–96.
  6. Corcos M, Guilbaud O, Paterniti S et al. Correlation between serum levels of interleukin-4 and alexithymia scores in healthy female subjects: Preliminary findings. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004; 29:557–62.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
  7. Van Middendorp H, Geenen R, Sorbi MJ et al. Styles of emotion regulation and their associations with perceived health in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Ann Behav Med (in press).
  8. Bagby RM, Parker JD, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res 1994;38:23–32.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
  9. King LA, Emmons RA. Conflict over emotional expression: psychological and physical correlates. J Pers Soc Psychol 1990;58:864–77.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
  10. Bleiker EM, Van der Ploeg HM, Hendriks JH, Leer JW, Kleijn WC. Rationality, emotional expression and control: psychometric characteristics of a questionnaire for research in psycho-oncology. J Psychosom Res 1993;37:861–72.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
  11. Gross JJ, John OP. Mapping the domain of expressivity: multimethod evidence for a hierarchical model. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998;74:170–91.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
  12. Miki K, Sudo A. Effect of urine pH, storage time, and temperature on stability of catecholamines, cortisol, and creatinine. Clin Chem 1998; 44:1759–62.[Free Full Text]
  13. Thompson PW, Silman AJ, Kirwan JR, Currey HLF. Articular indices of joint inflammation in rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 1987;30:618–23.[ISI][Medline]
  14. Wald FD, Mellenbergh GJ. De verkorte versie van de Nederlandse vertaling van de Profile of Mood States (POMS) [The shortened version of the Dutch translation of the Profile of Mood States (POMS)]. Ned Tijdschr Psychol 1990;45:86–90.
  15. Huiskes CJEA, Kraaimaat FW, Bijlsma JWJ. Development of a self-report questionnaire to asses the impact of rheumatic diseases on health and lifestyle. J Rehabil Sci 1990;3:65–70.
  16. Bijlsma JWJ, OudeHeuvel CHB, Zaalberg A. Development and validation of the Dutch questionnaire capacities of daily life (VDF) for patients with rheumatoid arthritis. J Rehabil Sci 1990;3:71–4.
  17. Stucki G, Liang MH, Stucki S, Bruhlmann P, Michel BA. A self-administered rheumatoid arthritis disease activity index (RADAI) for epidemiologic research. Psychometric properties and correlation with parameters of disease activity. Arthritis Rheum 1995;38:795–8.[ISI][Medline]
  18. Salovey P, Stroud LR, Woolery A, Epel ES. Perceived emotional intelligence, stress reactivity, and symptom reports: further explorations using the trait meta-mood scale. Psychol Health 2002;17:611–27.[CrossRef][ISI]
  19. Jula A, Salminen JK, Saarijärvi S. Alexithymia. A facet of essential hypertension. Hypertension 1999;33:1057–61.[Abstract/Free Full Text]
  20. Tsigos C, Papanicolaou DA, Defensor R, Mitsiadis CS, Kyrou I, Chrousos GP. Dose effects of recombinant human interleukin-6 on pituitary hormone secretion and energy expenditure. Neuroendocrinology 1997;66:54–62.[ISI][Medline]
  21. Boss B, Neeck G. Correlation of IL-6 with the classical humoral disease activity parameters ESR and CRP and with serum cortisol, reflecting the activity of the HPA axis in active rheumatoid arthritis. Z Rheumatol 2000;59(Suppl. 2):II62–II64.[CrossRef]
  22. Crofford LJ, Kalogeras KT, Mastorakos G et al. Circadian relationships between interleukin (IL)-6 and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis hormones: failure of IL-6 to cause sustained hypercortisolism in patients with early untreated rheumatoid arthritis. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1997;82:1279–83.[Abstract/Free Full Text]
  23. Kanik KS, Chrousos GP, Schumacher HR, Crane ML, Yarboro CH, Wilder RL. Adrenocorticotropin, glucocorticoid and androgen secretion in patients with new onset synovitis/rheumatoid arthritis: relations with indices of inflammation. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2000;85:1461–6.[Abstract/Free Full Text]
  24. Straub RH, Paimela L, Peltomma R, Schölmerich J, Leirisalo-Repo M. Inadequately low serum levels of steroid hormones in relation to interleukin-6 and tumor necrosis factor in untreated patients with early rheumatoid arthritis and reactive arthritis. Arthritis Rheum 2002;46:654–62.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
  25. Maier SF, Watkins LR. Cytokines for psychologists: implications of bidirectional immune-to-brain communication for understanding behavior, mood, and cognition. Psychol Rev 1998;105: 83–107.[ISI][Medline]
Submitted 20 December 2004; revised version accepted 25 February 2005.



This Article
Abstract
Full Text (PDF)
All Versions of this Article:
44/7/907    most recent
keh626v1
Alert me when this article is cited
Alert me if a correction is posted
Services
Email this article to a friend
Similar articles in this journal
Similar articles in ISI Web of Science
Similar articles in PubMed
Alert me to new issues of the journal
Add to My Personal Archive
Download to citation manager
Disclaimer
Request Permissions
Google Scholar
Articles by van Middendorp, H.
Articles by Bijlsma, J. W. J.
PubMed
PubMed Citation
Articles by van Middendorp, H.
Articles by Bijlsma, J. W. J.
Related Collections
Other Rheumatology