1Dipartimento di Fisiologia umana e Farmacologia, Università di Roma `la Sapienza', 00185 Rome; 2Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico S. Lucia, 00179 Rome, Italy; and 3Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale U. 483, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, 75005 Paris, France
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Ferraina, Stefano, Alexandra Battaglia-Mayer, Aldo Genovesio, Barbara Marconi, Paolo Onorati, and Roberto Caminiti. Early Coding of Visuomanual Coordination During Reaching in Parietal Area PEc. J. Neurophysiol. 85: 462-467, 2001. The parietal mechanisms of eye-hand coordination during reaching were studied by recording neural activity in area PEc while monkeys performed different tasks, aimed at assessing the influence of retinal, hand-, and eye-related signals on neural activity. The tasks used consisted of 1) reaching to foveated and 2) to extra-foveal targets, with constant eye position; and 3) saccadic eye movement toward, and holding of eye position on peripheral targets, the same as those of the reaching tasks. In all tasks, hand and/or eye movements were made from a central position to eight peripheral targets. A conventional visual fixation paradigm was used as a control task, to assess location and extent of visual receptive field of neurons. A large proportion of cells in area PEc displayed significant relationships to hand movement direction and position. Many of them were also related to the eye's position. Relationships to saccadic eye movements were found for a smaller proportion of cells. Most neurons were tuned to different combination of hand- and eye-related signals; some of them were also influenced by visual information. This combination of signals can be an expression of the early stages of the composition of motor commands for different forms of visuomotor coordination that depend on the integration of hand- and eye-related information. These results assign to area PEc, classically considered as a somatosensory association cortex, a new visuomotor role.
![]() |
INTRODUCTION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Understanding the cerebral
cortical coding of reaching requires knowledge of the functional
properties of reach-related neurons within different areas. Recent
physiological studies have elucidated the potential role of different
regions of the superior parietal lobule (SPL) on visuomanual and
oculomotor behavior (Batista et al. 1999;
Battaglia-Mayer et al. 2000
; Ferraina et al.
1997
; Johnson et al. 1996
; Tanné et
al. 1995
).
Nothing is known on the physiological properties of neurons of parietal
area PEc. As part of area 5, PEc has traditionally been considered as a
somatosensory association cortex. However, PEc is linked by association
connections (Pandya and Seltzer 1982) with 7m, PEci, in
the cingulate sulcus, and with parietal areas PE, PEa, and PG (7a). In
addition, PEc is reciprocally connected with dorsal premotor cortex
(Johnson et al. 1996
; Matelli et al. 1998
), where cell activity is tuned to arm position and
movement direction (Caminiti et al. 1991
), and is also
influenced by eye position signals (Jouffrais and Boussaoud
1999
).
This pattern of association connections is consistent with a visuomotor, rather than a somatosensory, role of PEc. Therefore single-cell activity was recorded in PEc while monkeys were performing four behavioral tasks, aimed at assessing the influence of eye- and hand-related information on neural activity. These tasks consisted of reaching to foveated targets, reaching to extrafoveal targets with constant eye position, and saccadic eye movement. A visual fixation task was used as control.
![]() |
METHODS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Animals, apparatus, and tasks
Two rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta; body weights 3.7 and 3.5 kg) were used in this study. They sat in a primate chair, 17 cm in front of a touch-sensitive computer monitor used to display the tasks and control the animals' hand position. Monkeys performed four different tasks (Fig. 1).
|
REACHING TO FOVEATED TARGETS (REACH, R). A red center light was presented. The animals fixated and touched it with the hand for a variable control time (Fig. 1A; CT, 1-1.5 s). Then, one of the eight red targets was lit, in a randomized block design. The animals moved the eyes and the hand to it within a reaction-time task (RT, 0.5 s, upper limit; movement time MT, 1 s, upper limit; Fig. 1A, RMT = RT + MT) and keep them there for a variable target holding time (Fig. 1A; THT, 1-1.5 s), before receiving a liquid reward.
REACHING TO EXTRAFOVEAL TARGETS (REACH-FIXATION, RF). This task was similar to the previous one, but reaches were made with eye position kept constant on a central fixation point.
EYE MOVEMENT AND POSITION TASK (SACCADE, S). Monkeys made saccades of the same amplitude from a central origin toward the same targets used in the two previous tasks, within a RT paradigm. The position of the hand was controlled by using a telegraph key.
VISUAL FIXATION TASK.
Visual stimuli were moved along 16 radial directions of the visual
field, at 22.5° angular intervals. The orientation of the visual
stimulus was always perpendicular to the direction of motion. Location
and extent of the visual receptive fields were reconstructed as
described in Battaglia-Mayer et al. (2000).
![]() |
RESULTS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Sixty-six microelectrode penetrations were performed in PEc (Fig.
1C). Ninety-five cells were studied in the Reach
task, 91 in the Reach-Fixation task, 93 in the
Saccade task, and 78 in the Visual Fixation task.
In the flat exposed part of the superior parietal lobule, recording of
cell activity was confined to the first 2-3 mm of cortical tissue. The
assignment of cell to area PEc, rather than to the adjacent V6A or 7m,
was made not only on architectonic criteria (Pandya and Seltzer
1982), but also on the basis of the pattern of association
connection with frontal cortex. Injections of different fluorescent
tracers in dorso-rostral (F7) (Matelli et al. 1985
) and
dorso-caudal (F2) (Matelli et al. 1985
) premotor cortex
of one of the two experimental animals (Caminiti et al. 1999
)
showed that V6A and 7m are cortico-cortically connected with F7, while
PEc is mainly linked to F2 (see also Matelli et al.
1998
).
Figure 2A shows a typical parietal cell. Neural activity was directional during hand movement (RMT) and holding of combined eye-hand position on the target (THT), both in the Reach and Reach-Fixation tasks. In this last task, the hand moved toward and then remained motionless on the target, in absence of eye movement. No significant difference was observed in the activity of this cell across these two task conditions during RMT, suggesting that neural activity during this epoch carries information about hand movement, independently of eye-related signals. On the contrary, a significant difference was observed during THT, indicating that eye position signals influence hand position-related activity. The eye and hand position records obtained during collection of neural data from this cell (Fig. 1, D and E) indicate that this difference cannot be attributed to differences in hand position across tasks during static holding.
|
In the Saccade task, cell activity was rather low and directional during RMT and THT.
This cell had an extrafoveal visual receptive field (VRF, Fig. 2A) straddling the vertical meridian, and extending over parts of both the ipsi- and contralateral visual fields. The location and extent of the VRF indicate that cell activity, during hand movement and holding of static position on the targets, was not dependent on the cell's VRF.
A different type of processing is illustrated by the cell shown in Fig. 2B. Neuronal activity was modulated when the eyes and the hand moved to the target (Reach, RMT) and during hand reaches with the eyes fixating at the center of the workspace (Reach-Fixation, RMT). A significant difference of cell modulation was observed across these conditions during both RMT and THT, in spite of the absence of cell modulation in the Saccade task. Therefore an eye position signal influenced neural activity during all epochs of the reaching task. This cell had no visual properties.
At the population level (Fig. 3A), in the Reach task, significant modulation of activity, relative to the control time, was observed in 76/95 (80%) cells during RMT, and in 47/95 (50%) during THT. In the Reach-Fixation task, 67/91 (78%) cells were modulated during RMT, and 54/91 (57%) during THT. The cells modulated in both tasks were 56/84 (67%) during RMT and 33/84 (39%) during THT. Therefore the activity of these cells was influenced by both arm movement and position signals. Similar results were obtained when the analysis was performed on the directional properties of cell activity during different epochs (Fig. 3A).
|
The comparison of neural activity during the Reach and Reach-Fixation tasks, for those cells modulated in both tasks, showed no differences in 24/56 (43%) cells during RMT, and in 13/33 (39%) cells during THT (Fig. 3B). Therefore the activity of these cells was related to hand movement and position, respectively, and was not influenced by eye-related information. On the contrary, the differences observed for the remaining cells (32 during RMT, 20 during THT), in addition to eye-related signals, could be attributed to the influence of a variety of inputs, including visual ones. In fact, a VRF was found in 43% of the cells with significant Reach/Reach-Fixation differences during RMT, and in 33% of those with such differences during THT. In conclusion, it cannot be excluded that, for the cells with visual properties, the modulation of activity, during hand movement and holding of hand position on the target, could be attributed to stimulation of the VRF.
In the Reach-Fixation task, the arm moved from the fovea toward the periphery of the visual field. Therefore this task offered the opportunity to assess whether or not cell activity during arm movement and static holding was dependent on the stimulation of a potential visual receptive field. Figure 3C shows that only a minority of cells out of those modulated or directional in different epochs of the Reach-Fixation task had a VRF. Thus for the majority of cells, neural activity during reaches was arm-related.
It is worth stressing that when the data analysis concerning RMT was repeated for RT in isolation, the cells modulated were 67/95 (71%) in the Reach task, 64/91 (70%) in the Reach-Fixation task, and 49/84 (58%) in both tasks. When the activities of these last cells were compared, significant differences were found in 33% of the cells studied. Therefore these changes in activity across Reach and Reach-Fixation tasks cannot be attributed to potential difference in arm kinematics across task conditions, since they were observed before onset of arm movement.
In the Saccade task (Fig. 3A), only 16/93 (17%) cells were modulated during eye movement (RMT). A higher percentage of cells was influenced by eye-position signals, as shown by the directional activity during THT (38/93; 41%).
![]() |
DISCUSSION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Neural activity in area PEc relates to directional signals
concerning arm movement and position in space, and, to a lesser extent,
to information about eye position and movement direction. Many cells
are also sensitive to retinal inputs. These signals influence different
cells to different degrees. They are common to all the areas of the
distributed system underlying reaching (Battaglia-Mayer et al.
2000; Johnson et al. 1996
).
Our study suggests that area PEc is an early node of parietal system underlying eye-hand coordination during reaching. It assigns to PEc a visuomotor role, rather than the classical somatosensory one. This role in combined eye-hand actions was predicted by the pattern of association connections of PEc.
When studied only in one task, the activity of most cells appeared as related either to visuo-manual or oculomotor behavior. When studied under different task conditions, designed to dissociate the hand- and eye-related signals, the activity of most neurons was influenced by a combination of these signals. Although different response properties were observed, the relationships with arm movement and position were prevalent.
Reach-related activity was sometime influenced by the position of
the eye in the orbit. This was observed by comparing reaches with and
without previous eye movement. The eye influence was detected already
during the hand reaction time in the Reach-Fixation task, when the eye
was fixating at the center of the workspace. This suggests that the eye
signals are of central origin and that the change of activity under
these circumstances was independent of the kinematics of the arm, since
it occurred well before onset of arm movement. An influence of eye
position signals on reach-related activity in the superior parietal
lobule has recently been described also by Batista et al.
(1999). The activity of fewer cells was instead dominated by
eye position information alone.
This combinatorial mechanism that operates at a very early stage in the
information processing flow leading from vision to movement emerges as
a prominent functional feature of area PEc. A similar combination of
eye- and arm-related information has also been observed in areas 7m
(Ferraina et al. 1997) and V6A (Battaglia-Mayer
et al. 2000
), where, however, eye movement-related neurons are
more numerous than in PEc. In spite of this, the functional properties
of the two adjacent areas PEc and V6A are remarkably similar, at least
when analyzed under the experimental conditions of the tasks adopted in
this study. Thus different parietal areas share common functional
properties, although different signals are represented with different
strength. Similar results were obtained in a previous study
(Johnson et al. 1996
) of parietal areas 5 (PE) and MIP.
Recently, it has been reported (Jouffrais and Boussaoud
1999) that reach-related activity in the dorso-caudal premotor
(PMdc, F2) cortex is influenced by eye position signals, probably
carried through the reciprocal association connections linking PEc and premotor cortex (Johnson et al. 1996
; Matelli et
al. 1998
; Tanné et al. 1995
).
This similarity of dynamic properties of cells in parietal and premotor
cortex suggests that the coordinate transformation underlying hand
movement to spatial targets is based on a parallel and recursive
mechanism. Reentrant signaling (Edelman 1993) through reciprocal association connections can be a way to link the functional operations of those areas underlying the coordinated eye-hand action
necessary for different motor tasks.
![]() |
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS |
---|
This study was supported by funds from the Human Frontiers Science Program Organization and the Ministry of Scientific and Technological Research of Italy.
![]() |
FOOTNOTES |
---|
Address for reprint requests: R. Caminiti, Dipartimento di Fisiologia umana e Farmacologia, Università di Roma `La Sapienza', Piazzale Aldo Moro 5, 00185 Rome, Italy (E-mail: roberto.caminiti{at}uniroma1.it).
Received 20 July 2000; accepted in final form 11 September 2000.
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|