For individual data (see RESULTS and Fig. 3), activated clusters that did not reach statistical significance also were reported. These data were used to obtain additional information on the anatomic location of activation foci identified in the group analysis.

View larger version (82K):
[in this window]
[in a new window]
| FIG. 3.
Individual foci of activation. For each volunteer, foci are reported on normalized individual axial slices (right side of the brain is right side of image). The distance from the bicommissural plane (z coordinate) is indicated below each slice. Activations maps were obtained with thresholding at z > 2.58 (P < 0.005 uncorrected) and cluster size >5 voxels for volunteers V1, V2, V3, and V5. For volunteers V4 and V6, whose activations were less significant, we used a lower threshold: z > 1.64 (P < 0.05 uncorrected) and cluster size >5 voxels. Some of the depicted foci are not significant at P < 0.05 corrected: this is explained in METHODS, and significance is indicated in RESULTS. Each colored arrow indicates a specific focus of activation: blue, inferior frontal gyrus; yellow, temporo-parietal junction; pink, middle frontal gyrus or middle precentral gyrus; green, intraparietal sulcus. White arrows indicate the position of the central sulcus. vol., volunteer.
|
|
 |
RESULTS |
Preliminary experiments
Preliminary experiments first were performed on phantoms and then on a volunteer's leg. No loss of signal was observed in the vicinity of the electrodes when comparing images with or without current. No additional noise was measured in the images with the GVS installation turned on. No detectable current was induced by gradient commutation. LC filters reduced RF pickup from ~5 mA (leading to painful cutaneous stimulation) to <200 µA (undetected by the volunteer), and when tested on a volunteer's leg, no RF sensation was induced at increasing levels of RF power (
3 times the level used for imaging).
Consequently, all further experiments were performed safely, and no volunteer reported heating of the skin.
Psychophysical results
The vestibular sensations induced by GVS during the pretests were similar in all subjects. They consistently experienced small-amplitude (5-15°) oscillations of the entire body at stimulus frequency about an approximately midsagittal axis, which was perceived variably as passing through the body somewhere between the head and the upper abdomen. The orientation of this axis relative to the body did not change essentially when the body was brought from the seated to the supine position. These illusory vestibular phenomena are equivalent to those reported in previous studies making use of sinusoidal GVS (Grüsser and Kleine 1995
; Von Romberg et al. 1951
).
Inside the magnet during fMRI, all participants had qualitatively similar sensations as in the pretests. However, the perception of illusory body oscillation was generally less intense. The subjects spontaneously attributed this to the rigid head fixation and to the noise generated during image acquisition, which was still prominent despite our sound-reducing measures. Nevertheless, all subjects reported to have had distinct sensations of body movement during the periods of GVS and not during the resting periods. The build-up and the cessation of the vestibular sensation were perceived as quick and took <2 s from the onset and offset, respectively, of stimulation in all subjects. Somatosensory side effects at the electrode sites were present in all subjects and were described mostly as a moderate heating sensation. All subjects except one perceived a slight metallic taste during GVS. Nausea, as in the pretests, did not occur.
fMRI results
The comparison of the GVS condition with the control condition showed significant increase of MR signal (activation) during GVS in seven cortical areas, and significant decrease of MR signal (deactivation) during GVS in one area. Areas with significant signal change are shown in Figs. 1, 2, and 4, and presented in Table 1 with their location and their statistical significance.

View larger version (20K):
[in this window]
[in a new window]
| FIG. 2.
Axial slices showing significant activation foci (group analysis). Foci are reported on axial slices of a standardized brain (right side of the brain is right side of image). Distance from the bicommissural plane (z coordinate) is indicated below each slice.
|
|

View larger version (34K):
[in this window]
[in a new window]
| FIG. 4.
Area of significant deactivation (group analysis), reported on a 3-dimensional reconstruction of a standardized brain. From left to right: anterior view, paramedian sagittal view, inferior view. Letters correspond to codes in Table 1. Talairach coordinates are given in Table 1.
|
|
View this table:
[in this window]
[in a new window]
|
TABLE 1.
Areas of significant signal change in the comparison of the GVS condition with the control condition (group analysis)
|
|
TEMPORO-PARIETAL JUNCTION ACTIVATION.
Group analysis. Significant activation was found in the left temporo-parietal junction (TPJ; area F), but was not seen in the right hemisphere. This activation was located in the posterior part of the lateral sulcus, on its external banks, and overlaps both the posterior part of the superior temporal gyrus and the inferior part of the supramarginal gyrus in the parietal lobe.
Individual analysis. Individual analysis indicates that five volunteers (V1-V5) showed an increased signal in the TPJ region (see Fig. 3). This activation was located in the posterior part of the lateral sulcus, except in V3 where it appeared more posteriorly in the angular gyrus (significant at P < 0.05 corrected). The lateralization pattern appearing in the group analysis was not found at the individual level because four volunteers showed an increased signal in the right TPJ (significant at P < 0.05 corrected in V5).
CENTRAL SULCUS ACTIVATION.
Group analysis. Activation was found in the right central sulcus (area C). This activation is large (44 voxels) and highly significant (corrected P = 0.002). The most significantly activated voxel was located in the depth of the central sulcus (~2 cm inside the sulcus), at about the level of the middle frontal gyrus. The area of activation also extended into the adjacent postcentral and precentral gyri.
In the left hemisphere, area A shows a highly significant activation extremum in (
48,
12, 48), adjacent to the central sulcus, in the anterior part of the left postcentral gyrus.
Individual analysis. In three volunteers (V1-V3), individual analysis indicated a significant (P < 0.05 corrected) and bilateral activation in the depth or on the banks of the central sulcus, at the level of the middle frontal gyrus (see Fig. 3). V4 and V5 also showed increased signal in this area at z > 1.64 in the left and in the right hemisphere, respectively (not appearing in Fig. 3 for V5).
PARIETAL LOBE ACTIVATION.
Group analysis. Activation was found in only one area of the parietal lobe, unilaterally in the left hemisphere (area A). This area was very large (52 voxels), showing the highest level of significance of all areas in this study (corrected P = 0.001) and presented two significant extrema (see Table 1).
Area A was located mainly on the lateral surface of the left postcentral gyrus but also extended posteriorly, and the second extremum was found in the most anterior part of the intraparietal sulcus. As previously stated, area C in the right hemisphere extended into the postcentral gyrus, and thus we cannot exclude bilateral activation of the postcentral gyrus. However area A extended much more posteriorly, and the activation in the intraparietal sulcus was observed in the left hemisphere only.
Individual analysis. The lateralized pattern also was suggested by the individual analysis, which showed significant activation foci (P < 0.05 corrected) in the left parietal lobe only (volunteers V1-V3). In these three volunteers, the activation was located at the most anterior part of the left intraparietal sulcus, whereas in V6 increased signal (at z > 1.64) appeared more posteriorly in the left parietal lobe (see Fig. 3). At z > 1.64, two volunteers also showed increased signal in the right parietal lobe (V1 and V5, not appearing in Fig. 3).
FRONTAL LOBE: INFERIOR FRONTAL GYRUS.
Group analysis. Bilateral activation was found on the lateral surface of the frontal lobe (area B in the right hemisphere, area E in the left). These activations were highly significant with a corrected P value of 0.002 for B and of 0.02 for E.
In both hemispheres, the activation was located in the most inferior and posterior part of the inferior frontal gyrus, i.e., in the pars opercularis. In the right hemisphere, the activation was wider (24 voxels) and also appeared to extend into the frontal operculum as well as into the inferior part of the precentral gyrus. However, in both hemispheres, the most significantly activated voxels were located inside the inferior frontal gyrus (see coordinates in Table 1).
Individual analysis. Five volunteers show an increased signal in the posterior part of the right inferior frontal gyrus (significant at P < 0.05 corrected for V5, not appearing in Fig. 3 for V2). In the left hemisphere, significant activation was found only in V1.
FRONTAL LOBE: INTERSECTION OF PRECENTRAL SULCUS AND INFERIOR FRONTAL SULCUS.
Group analysis. In the right hemisphere, significant activation was also found dorsal to the previous area, in the vicinity of the intersection of the precentral sulcus and of the inferior frontal sulcus (area D, corrected P = 0.004). This large activation (30 voxels) was located ~1 cm inside the sulcus and overlapped the cortex of the precental gyrus and of the inferior and middle frontal gyri. In the left hemisphere, increased signal also appeared in the same region (at z > 3.09), but it did not reach statistical significance.
Individual analysis. In the right hemisphere, individual analysis showed in five volunteers a signal increase in this area (significant at P < 0.05 corrected for V1). In the left hemisphere, only V1 showed an increased signal (at z > 1.64) in the corresponding region.
OTHER ACTIVATIONS.
In the group analysis, the middle cingulate sulcus also showed an increased signal at the uncorrected threshold, but this increase did not reach statistical significance. The voxel with highest z value in this cluster was located in (0,0,48).
"DEACTIVATIONS": GROUP ANALYSIS.
In addition to these areas of activation, one region showed significant "deactivation," i.e., decrease of the MR signal during GVS. This deactivation (corrected P = 0.03) was found in the most anterior part of the frontal lobe (area G, see Fig. 4).
It was located around the interhemispheric fissure and was located in the middle and inferior parts of the frontopolar gyrus. It also extended into the medial wall of the frontal lobe. The group analysis showed a more intense deactivation in the left hemisphere, with 20 of 28 voxels located in the left hemisphere.
Other areas showing a signal decrease at uncorrected threshold were found (ventral medial frontal gyrus, posterior cingulum), but the signal changes did not reach statistical significance.
 |
DISCUSSION |
Variability of individual activations
The group results of this study indicates that in humans a large set of areas is activated by GVS, including in particular the TPJ, the intraparietal sulcus, and the premotor regions of the frontal lobe. At the individual level, there exists however more variability (see Fig. 3), and three factors at least may contribute to this interindividual variability.
First, image acquisition requires the head to be rigidly fixated. This necessarily will lead to sensory conflicts between proprioceptive information signaling stability of the head and GVS-elicited vestibular signals indicating head movement. According to animal experiments, convergence of polysensory signals related to self-motion or object motion appears to be a fundamental property of cortical vestibular areas. Interindividual differences in the weighing of conflicting sensory signals may not only cause hardly controllable differences in the intensity of movement sensation but also lead to variability in the measurable blood-flow changes. Second, unlike in the vestibular periphery, in which all neurons will be either activated or inhibited by GVS, a variable number of type I and type II neurons can be found in cortical vestibular areas, which will respond with an increase or decrease in discharge, respectively, to a given vestibular stimulus. For example, Buettner and Büttner (1978)
reported an almost equal number of type I and type II responses (26 vs. 25) to yaw rotation in vestibular neurons from area 2v of the rhesus monkey. The simultaneous occurrence of inhibitory and excitatory responses in a cortical vestibular region may end up in a relatively small net increase in neuronal activity and, therefore, blood flow. A third factor, specifically relevant for GVS, is related to the different galvanic sensitivities of regular and irregular vestibular afferents (Goldberg et al. 1984
). Irregular units are highly sensitive with respect to galvanic stimulation, whereas regular afferents are only slightly modulated even by high-amplitude stimuli. There are about three times more regular than irregular units in the mammalian vestibular nerve (Baird et al. 1988
; Goldberg et al. 1990
), and it has been shown that regular units contribute significantly to both vestibulo-ocular and vestibulo-spinal output pathways of secondary neurons in the vestibular nuclei (Highstein et al. 1987
). Therefore, it appears likely that regular afferents also participate in the mediation of vestibular information from the periphery to the cortex. The low galvanic sensitivity of regular afferents will be reflected in a low amplitude of the transmitted signals because only moderate stimulus amplitudes could be applied in our experiments.
Thus on one hand, the observed interindividual variability may result from specific properties of the stimulus and of the peripheral and cortical vestibular system. On the other hand, averaging across subjects will increase the predictably low contrast-to-noise ratio in the data and enable the reliable detection of activations. Accordingly, the following discussion will put the focus on group results.
TPJ activation
In the group analysis, activation appeared in the left TPJ (area F). The individual analysis allocates this focus to the posterior end of the lateral sulcus and also shows in four volunteers a signal increase in the right hemisphere.
This area corresponds closely to a polysensory vestibular field, which recently was discovered in several primate species and designated parieto-insular vestibular cortex (PIVC) by Grüsser and co-workers (Grüsser et al. 1983
, 1990
; for review, see Guldin and Grüsser 1998
). The PIVC is located deep in the lateral sulcus, posterior to the insular cortex. It is one of the primate areas in which neurons responding to vestibular stimulation have been found and appears to be a core area of the primate cortical vestibular system. Most vestibular neurons in the PIVC also receive an optokinetic and/or somatosensory input.
A positron emission tomography (PET) study in humans (Bottini et al. 1994
) showed two distinct areas in the TPJ region contralaterally activated by cold CVS: a posterior insula region and a more superficial posterior lateral sulcus activation. The authors proposed that the posterior insula region be the human's equivalent of the PIVC. To our knowledge, this was the first functional imaging study pointing to a vestibular function of the human's posterior insula. Posterior insula activation, however, had been found in other functional imaging experiments using nonvestibular stimuli such as painful hot stimulation of the arm (Casey et al. 1996
; Coghill et al. 1994
; Talbot et al. 1991
).
Until then, experiments concerning the human vestibular cortex had pointed to the involvement of the superficial part of the superior temporal or inferior parietal gyri. Penfield (1957)
and Smith (1960)
, using electrical microstimulation of the brain in epileptic patients during surgery, evoked vestibular sensations when the electrode was placed on the external banks of the lateral sulcus, at a depth-excluding insular stimulation. Friberg et al. (1985)
, using SPECT and warm CVS, showed contralateral activation also coming from the superficial part of the TPJ.
In our group and individual data, no significant insular activation was found. Area F is closer to the area found by Bottini et al. (1994)
in the lateral sulcus, although the location they report is 1.5 cm deeper in the sulcus. It cannot be confounded with the auditory cortex which is located almost 2 cm deeper in the lateral sulcus, in Heschl's gyrus according to several studies (Johnsrude et al. 1997
; Wise et al. 1991
; Zatorre et al. 1992
). Area F, in the posterior part of the lateral sulcus, therefore could be the human equivalent of the monkey's PIVC region.
Central sulcus activation
In the right hemisphere, the group analysis showed an activation centered in the depth of the central sulcus and extending into the adjacent postcentral and precentral gyri (area C). In the left hemisphere, activation also was found in the postcentral gyrus (anterior part of area A). This pattern was confirmed by the analysis of individual results, which showed significant bilateral activation of this region in three volunteers.
Such an activation could be expected. In several species including monkeys, there exists a bilateral vestibular projection to the central sulcus, inside area 3a (Ödkvist et al. 1973a
,b
, 1974
). This vestibular area is small and located within the arm field of the somatosensory cortex. A bilateral projection is in good agreement with our data because areas A and C are located in and around the central sulcus, where area 3a usually is located in humans according to architectonic data (K. Zilles, personal communication) and because areas A and C are at the level of the hand somatosensory cortex located between z = +40 mm and z = +57 mm in Talairach coordinates according to several studies (Coghill et al. 1994
; Fox et al. 1987
; Kawashima et al. 1994
; Stephan et al. 1995
; Walter et al. 1992
).
Activation in areas A and C cannot be due to the finger-tapping task (FT) in which subjects were involved because FT was performed continuously during the whole experiment and should be subtracted out by the comparison of the two conditions. Even if differences in rhythm or attention during FT could have led to differences in the two conditions, such FT-related activations would be located unilaterally in the left motor cortex because FT was performed with the right index finger only.
Areas A and C are distinct from the frontal eye fields (FEF), which are located 1.4 cm more anterior in the right hemisphere and 1.9 cm more anterior in the left (review in Paus 1996
). They are also distinct from the neck somatosensory area, which is situated above the hand area (Penfield and Rasmussen 1950
). It is therefore unlikely that the activation could be related to the somatosensory component of the stimulus.
It appears that the region we found can be the homologue in humans of the vestibular projection to area 3a described in animals. The specific role played by this area is not well understood, but its high degree of convergence of vestibular and proprioceptive input could make it an important relay to ensure correct performance of directional movements.
Parietal lobe activation
An important result of this study is the unilateral activation in the left posterior parietal lobe. Group analysis as well as individual analysis showed a significant activation of an area centered around the anterior part of the left intraparietal sulcus (posterior part of area A).
Activation in this area was expected. Foerster (1936)
reported that electrical stimulation of the anterior intraparietal sulcus in epileptic patients results in typical vestibular sensations of being rolled with apparent motion of the visual surroundings. In the cat, a short-latency vestibular projection to the parietal lobe was then demonstrated by Walzl and Mountcastle (1949)
. A homologous area also was identified in the monkey within the lower bank of the anterior tip of the intraparietal sulcus in a specific architectonic area called 2v, located posteriorly to SI neurons responding to hand or mouth stimulation (Fredrickson et al. 1966
). Many neurons in area 2v receive deep somatic afferents (Schwarz and Fredrickson 1971
) or respond to optokinetic stimulation (Buettner and Büttner 1978
). In cats and in monkeys, vestibular projections to the parietal lobe are mainly contralateral, although a weak ipsilateral projection also exists (Fredrickson et al. 1966
; Kornhuber and DaFonesca 1964
).
Although an activation in the region of the intraparietal sulcus could be predicted based on these previous studies, the asymmetry in the activation pattern is an unexpected but fairly robust result, which also is reflected largely by the individual data (except for V5 who shows signal increase in the right parietal lobe but not in the left). Given that the observed asymmetry does reflect an underlying functional lateralization, it could indicate a specialization of the human left parietal lobe in the processing of self-motion-related information. In line with this argument is a PET study that compared blood flow during coherent and incoherent wide-field visual motion, in which a similar asymmetry was observed (Cheng et al. 1995
). However, such an interpretation apparently contradicts a wealth of evidence pointing to a specialization of the right hemisphere in spatial information processing.
The region of the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) is known to be involved in coordinate transformations, from retino- to cranio- or spatiotopic coordinates and also from sensory to motor coordinates (reviewed in Andersen 1994
). Our results suggest that the most anterior part of the IPS is a specific region where vestibular information is processed and where it could be integrated with spatial information from other sensory modalities, particularly visual motion signals, which can be found in the IPS (Colby et al. 1993
; Dupont et al. 1994
).
Frontal activations
One of the most interesting results of this study is the bilateral activation in the frontal premotor regions during GVS (areas B, D, and E). No such activations have yet been reported in previous PET experiments on vestibular stimulation.
This activation is distinct from the FEF, which is localized >2.5 cm away, in the depth of the precentral sulcus at its intersection with the superior frontal sulcus (Lobel et al. 1996a
; Paus 1996
). In humans, Israël et al. (1995)
reported that lesions of the middle and inferior posterior frontal gyri, as well as lesions of the medial anterior frontal gyrus (SEF), result in specific impairments in a task of vestibular-guided memory saccades in the dark. Furthermore, in a recent study of evoked potentials induced by selective stimulation of the vestibular nerve in humans during surgery, it was shown that three dipoles could be modeled in the frontal lobe 10 ms after the stimulation (Baudonnière et al. 1996
).
Based on old (Brodmann 1909
) and recent (Zilles et al. 1995
) cytoarchitectonic studies, the activations we find correspond to Brodmann's area 44 (BA44) or to the most ventral part of BA6, with a possible anterior extension of area D onto the intersection with BA9/BA44. According to their location, these frontal activations appear to be homologous to the monkey vestibular region in the anterior ventral part of premotor area 6. This region sends direct corticofugal projections to the vestibular nuclei and also projects strongly to areas PIVC and 3aV (Akbarian et al. 1993
, 1994
), which together with area 2v form a key circuitry in the primate cortical vestibular system (Guldin et al. 1992
).
Up to now, functional imaging studies failed to demonstrate the existence of this vestibular area in humans. Because these studies used CVS, one may speculate that the frontal activations we find are related to the stronger modulation of otolith afferent activity brought about by GVS. In fact, the vestibular part of area 6 is certainly among the candidate regions for a representation of otolithic signals in the primate cortex (Akbarian et al. 1993
). However, our simple paradigm did not allow to separate the effects of GVS on otolith and semicircular canal afferents, and it can therefore not be decided, based on the present data, whether these frontal activations are due to an otolithic component of the galvanic stimulus or result from other methodological differences between our and the previous imaging studies. Nevertheless, the present finding of an activation focus in the region of area 6 is an important result, indicating that a vestibular region also exists in the human premotor cortex and increasing the evidence for a close correspondence between the cortical vestibular system in human and in nonhuman primates.
Finally, the asymmetrical pattern of activation our bilateral stimulation evoked in this area may be related to the general specialization of the right hemisphere in spatial behavior and can be compared with the right-sided hemispheric dominance of lesions provoking spatial hemineglect (reviewed in Bisiach and Vallar 1988
). In the right hemisphere, lesions to this particular frontal region can indeed result in neglect symptoms (Husain and Kennard 1996
). Furthermore recent fMRI experiments have shown that in healthy volunteers the computation of the subjective midsagittal plane, a basic egocentric spatial reference that can be strongly disturbed in spatial hemineglect, involves a bilateral activation of the same region of the lateral premotor frontal cortex with a much stronger activation in the right hemisphere (Galati et al. 1997).
Negative BOLD responses ("deactivations")
A focus of significantly negative BOLD response appeared in the transverse frontopolar gyrus (area G). According to the Talairach and Tournoux atlas (Talairach and Tournoux 1988
), this focus is located at the border between BA10 and BA11. Focal signal decreases during the "stimulation" relative to the "resting" condition frequently have been observed in functional imaging studies, but the interpretation of this phenomenon remains controversial (Le Bihan and Dohi 1995). However, recent findings suggest that it could correspond to a decrease in cerebral blood flow, i.e., represent a true "deactivation" (McKinstry et al. 1998
).
The present finding of a signal decrease in the prefrontal cortex is interesting because it may indicate the involvement of this region in the processing of vestibular information, as already suggested by a recent study of vestibularly evoked potentials in which input to this region was observed (Baudonnière et al. 1996
). However, there is no anatomic or single unit data supporting this hypothesis, and it may be worth studying this area in future vestibular animal experiments.
Conclusion
GVS led to activation in the region of the TPJ, the central sulcus, and the intraparietal sulcus, which may correspond to areas PIVC, 3aV, and 2v, respectively, in the monkey. Because of their dense reciprocal connections, these areas are thought to form a key vestibular circuitry in the primate cortex, which has been labeled as the "inner vestibular circle" (Guldin et al. 1992
).
In addition, activation was found in the frontal premotor region, presumably analogous to the anterior ventral part of area 6, which by anatomic investigations has been identified as a vestibular region in several primate species (review in Guldin and Grüsser 1998
). One may speculate, that the observed activation could be related to the modulation of otolith afferent activity elicited by GVS because previous studies using caloric stimulation failed to show an increase in activity in this area. Clearly, single-unit recordings from the various candidate areas, including areas 6, T3 and the cingulate cortex, must be performed to clarify the question of a cortical representation of the otolith organs in the primate brain. In any case, our study increases the evidence indicating a very similar organization of the cortical vestibular system in human and nonhuman primates.
Using a simple paradigm we showed that GVS can be implemented safely in the fMRI environment. Manipulating stimulus waveforms and thus the GVS-induced subjective vestibular sensations in future imaging studies may yield further insights into the cortical processing of vestibular signals.