Department of Physiology, Hokkaido University School of Medicine, Sapporo 060-8638, Japan; and Department of Physiology and Biophysics and Regional Primate Research Center, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington 98195
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Fukushima, Kikuro, Toshikazu Sato, Junko Fukushima, Yasuhiro Shinmei, and Chris R. S. Kaneko. Activity of Smooth Pursuit-Related Neurons in the Monkey Periarcuate Cortex During Pursuit and Passive Whole-Body Rotation. J. Neurophysiol. 83: 563-587, 2000. Smooth pursuit and vestibularly induced eye movements interact to maintain the accuracy of eye movements in space (i.e., gaze). To understand the role played by the frontal eye fields in pursuit-vestibular interactions, we examined activity of 110 neurons in the periarcuate areas of head-stabilized Japanese monkeys during pursuit eye movements and passive whole-body rotation. The majority (92%) responded with the peak of their modulation near peak stimulus velocity during suppression of the vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) when the monkeys tracked a target that moved with the same amplitude and phase and in the same plane as the chair. We classified pursuit-related neurons (n = 100) as gaze- velocity if their peak modulation occurred for eye (pursuit) and head (VOR suppression) movements in the same direction; the amplitude of modulation during one less than twice that of the other; and modulation was lower during target-stationary-in-space condition (VOR ×1) than during VOR suppression. In addition, we examined responses during VOR enhancement (×2) in which the target moved with equal amplitude as, but opposite direction to, the chair. Gaze-velocity neurons responded maximally for opposite directions during VOR ×2 and suppression. Based on these criteria, the majority of pursuit-related neurons (66%) were classified as gaze-velocity with preferred directions uniformly distributed. Because the majority of the remaining cells (32/34) also responded during VOR suppression, they were classified as eye/head-velocity neurons. Thirteen preferred pursuit and VOR suppression in the same direction; 13 in the opposite direction, and 6 showed biphasic modulation during VOR suppression. Eye- and gaze-velocity sensitivity of the two groups of cells were similar; mean (± SD) was 0.53 ± 0.30 and 0.50 ± 0.44 spikes/s per °/s, respectively. Gaze-velocity (but not eye/head-velocity) neurons showed significant correlation between eye- and gaze-velocity sensitivity, and both groups maintained their responses when the tracking target was extinguished briefly. The majority of pursuit-related neurons (28/43 = about 65%) responded to chair rotation in complete darkness. When the monkeys fixated a stationary target, more than half of cells tested (21/40) discharged in proportion to the velocity of retinal motion of a second laser spot (mean velocity sensitivity = 0.20 ± 0.16 spikes/s per °/s). Preferred directions of individual cells to the second spot were similar to those during pursuit. Visual responses to the second spot movement were maintained even when it was extinguished briefly. These results indicate that both retinal image- and gaze-velocity signals are carried by single periarcuate pursuit-related neurons, suggesting that these signals can provide target-velocity-in-space and gaze-velocity commands during pursuit-vestibular interactions.
![]() |
INTRODUCTION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Accurate visual information about a moving object can be obtained
using smooth-pursuit eye movements. During movement of the head and/or
whole body, the smooth-pursuit system does not work independently but
interacts with the vestibular system to maintain the precision of eye
movements in space (i.e., gaze) (Robinson 1981). This
interaction requires calculation of gaze to match the
eye-velocity-in-space to the actual target velocity.
Target-velocity-in-space can be computed by adding
retinal-image-velocity, head (i.e., vestibular) velocity, and eye
velocity. The latter two signals are used to calculate gaze-velocity.
To drive ocular motoneurons during smooth gaze movement, two stages of
further signal conversion are necessary: omnidirectional gaze-velocity
signals must be sorted into roughly horizontal and vertical components
and such gaze-velocity components must be converted into oculomotor signals.
The importance of gaze-velocity signals for generating smooth tracking
of a moving target has been suggested by studies of the cerebellar
floccular lobe. It contains many horizontal gaze-velocity Purkinje
cells (Lisberger and Fuchs 1978; Miles and Fuller
1975
; Miles et al. 1980
, Stone and
Lisberger 1990
), and lesions severely impair smooth pursuit
(Zee et al. 1981
). Smooth tracking of a moving object
during whole-body rotation also is impaired severely (Zee et al.
1981
). Because preferred directions of gaze-velocity signals in
the floccular lobe are either horizontal or vertical (Fukushima
et al. 1999a
; Krauzlis and Lisberger 1996
;
Miles et al. 1980
, Shidara and Kawano
1993
; Stone and Lisberger 1990
), the first
sorting largely already has taken place by the floccular lobe. This
suggests that the conversion of retinotopic (i.e., omnidirectional)
visual into gaze-velocity signals may be generated upstream to the
floccular lobe (Fukushima et al. 1999a
).
Retinal image- and gaze-velocity signals are found in the posterior
parietal cortex, particularly in the medial superior temporal (MST)
area (Kawano et al. 1984; Komatsu and Wurtz
1988
; Newsome et al. 1988
; Sakata et al.
1983
; Their and Erickson 1992
; see Anderson 1997
for review). Gaze movement might be driven
by the omnidirectional signals represented in the MST and the
dorsolateral pontine nucleus (e.g., Kawano et al. 1992
),
which receives direct projections from the MST and projects directly to
the floccular lobe (see Keller and Heinen 1991
for
review). However, the origin of eye-velocity signals in the MST is
still unknown (e.g., Their and Erickson 1992
).
The frontal eye fields (FEF) have long been known as structures that
control purposive saccades (see Goldberg and Segraves 1989 for review). Recent studies have shown that these
areas, particularly the fundus and posterior bank of the arcuate
sulcus, also are involved in smooth pursuit (Bruce et al.
1985
; Gottlieb et al. 1994
; Keating 1991
,
1993
; Lynch 1987
; MacAvoy et al.
1991
; Tanaka and Fukushima 1998
; Tian and
Lynch 1996a
). Despite these detailed studies, important
questions still remain. In particular, it is unknown whether pursuit
neurons in the periarcuate areas carry signals related to eye movement
in the orbit or eye movement in space. It is also unknown whether or
not they carry target velocity information as well. Answers to these
questions are essential to understand the role of the periarcuate areas
in pursuit-vestibular interactions and their relationship with the MST.
In this study, by assigning the monkeys the behavioral tasks that
dissociate eye movement in the orbit from eye movement in space, we
were able to show that pursuit-related neurons in the periarcuate areas
carry not only eye-velocity signals as previously reported
(Gottlieb et al. 1994; MacAvoy et al.
1991
; Tanaka and Fukushima 1998
), but also
vestibular signals and that the activity of the majority of them is
related to gaze-velocity. Moreover, when the monkeys fixated a
stationary target, half of them discharge in proportion to retinal
image-motion of the second, test target. These results suggest that
both retinal-image- and gaze-velocity signals carried by single
pursuit-related neurons in the periarcuate areas can provide
target-velocity-in-space during pursuit-vestibular interactions. Some
of these results have been presented in preliminary form (J. Fukushima et al. 1998
; K. Fukushima et al. 1997
,
1999b
).
![]() |
METHODS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
General procedures
Two Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata, 5.0 and 6.5 kg)
were used. All experiments were performed in strict compliance with the
Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (DHEW Publication NIH85-23 1985). Specific protocols were approved by the Animal Care and
Use Committee of Hokkaido University School of Medicine (9290). For
surgery, each monkey was sedated with ketamine hydrochloride (5 mg/kg
im), and then anesthetized with pentobarbital sodium (Nembutal, 25 mg/kg ip) under aseptic conditions. Atropine was administered
subcutaneously (0.1 mg/kg). Electrocardiogram and expired
CO2 were monitored continuously during the
surgery, and additional anesthesia (0.5-1.0% halothane mixed with
50% nitrous oxide and 50% oxygen) was administered as necessary.
Analgesics and antibiotics were administered postsurgically to reduce
pain and prevent infection. Two head-holders were installed over the skull, and a scleral search coil was implanted on the right eye to
record vertical and horizontal components of eye movement (Fuchs and Robinson 1966; Judge et al. 1980
). After a
week of recovery, we started training the animals. Body weight and
general health conditions were checked regularly.
Training procedures
Our training procedures were described elsewhere
(Fukushima et al. 1996). Monkeys' heads were restrained
firmly in the primate chair in the stereotaxic plane. The monkey chair
was fixed to the turntable that had two degrees of freedom of motion
(horizontal and vertical rotation) under computer control. The
interaural midpoint of the animals' head was brought close to the axis
of vertical and horizontal rotation. The chair was rotated sinusoidally in the vertical, horizontal, or oblique planes. Rotation in the oblique
planes was applied by combining vertical and horizontal rotations. A
tangent screen was positioned 75 cm in front of the animals' eyes and
subtended 60 by 80° of visual angle, and the monkeys were trained to
track in darkness a laser spot (0.2° diam) back-projected onto the
tangent screen for apple juice reward. The target moved either
sinusoidally or stepwise in vertical, horizontal, or two oblique
directions at 45 and 135° polar angle. Reward circuits compared
target position signals with the monkeys' gaze position signals. The
latter signals were calculated electronically as the sum of eye
position and chair position. If the monkeys' gaze was within the error
window of ±1° for 0.5-1 s, a drop of apple juice was delivered
automatically to the animal. Target position signals were calibrated
first before a recording session by placing the target at known
horizontal and vertical locations (0, ±10, ±20°). Eye position
signals then were calibrated to the target by requiring the animal to
fixate the stationary target or pursue a slowly moving one. After the
animals were trained, a recording chamber was installed over a hole cut
in the skull at Ant. 25 and Lat. 10-15 to allow single-cell recording
in the periarcuate cortical areas in both monkeys as previously
described (Tanaka and Fukushima 1998
).
Recording procedures and behavioral paradigms
All stimuli were applied sinusoidally. For our search stimulus, the target was moved obliquely (at 0.5 Hz, ±10°) in association with chair rotation at the same frequency either in the yaw or pitch plane. Once responsive single cells were encountered as judged visually and on the audio monitor (e.g., Fig. 1B), smooth-pursuit responses were tested in four planes (vertical, horizontal, and two oblique planes at 45° angles) at 0.5 Hz (±10°) to determine the preferred direction for pursuit activation without chair rotation (Fig. 1B1). To dissociate eye movement in the orbit from that in space (i.e., gaze), we employed three tracking conditions (Fig. 1A, 2-4). In the vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) suppression task (Fig. 1A2), the monkeys tracked a target that moved in space with the same amplitude, direction and phase as the chair. This condition required the monkeys to suppress the VOR so that the eyes remained virtually motionless in the orbit and gaze therefore moved with the chair (Fig. 1, A2 and B2). In the VOR enhancement (×2) task (Fig. 1A3), the target moved in space with the same amplitude as the chair but in antiphase. This condition required the monkeys to increase eye movement in the orbit to twice the chair movement but with the opposite direction and thereby caused a gaze movement equal to that produced during VOR suppression but in the direction opposite to the chair movement (Fig. 1, A3 and B3). In the third condition (Fig. 1A4, VOR ×1), the target stayed stationary in space during chair rotation, which required perfect VOR and no gaze movement (Fig. 1, A4 and B4). For many neurons, responses to a variety of frequencies were examined (0.1-1.0 Hz, at ±10°) for each of the tracking conditions (Fig. 1A, 1-4).
|
Chair rotation was applied either horizontally (yaw) or vertically (pitch) in the first monkey so that the three conditions with chair rotation (Fig. 1A, 2-4) were tested either vertically or horizontally, whichever was closer to the preferred activation directions of individual neurons. In the second monkey, chair rotation was applied in the oblique planes as well as horizontally and vertically so that the three tasks that included chair rotation (Fig. 1A, 2-4) also were tested in the oblique planes. To examine vestibular responses, chair rotation also was applied in complete darkness. The monkeys were not required to perform any particular task during this condition but were kept alert by occasional drops of apple juice.
To examine the importance of a visual target during gaze tracking, the
target was briefly extinguished ("blanking") during smooth pursuit
or VOR suppression (Fig. 1A, 1 and 2). We tested blanking effects using longer duration blanking periods (200 or 800 ms)
than our previous study (100-200 ms) (Tanaka and Fukushima 1998). In particular, to examine the voluntary nature of
initiating smooth gaze tracking without a target, we extinguished the
visual target shortly before it changed direction during sinusoidal
movement at 0.5 Hz (±10°). The monkeys were required to continue
smooth-gaze tracking by changing direction without the target.
To examine whether periarcuate pursuit-related neurons receive retinal image information about target movement (Fig. 1A5), the stationary monkeys were rewarded for fixating a stationary laser spot (1st target, 0.2° diam) while a second laser spot (0.6° diam) moved sinusoidally along one of the four directions. For some cells in this task (Fig. 1A5), the second laser spot moved with a variety of frequencies (0.1-3.0 Hz, at ±10°, typically 0.3-1.5 Hz) and amplitudes (±1.25 ~ ±30° at 1 or 2 Hz, typically ±5 ~ ±20°) while the monkeys fixated the first spot. For assessing the affects of amplitude, a higher stimulus frequency was used because it was easier for the monkeys to fixate the stationary target without being disturbed by the second laser spot. The first target occasionally was extinguished while the second laser spot was presented continuously. This procedure was used to reward the monkeys for pursuing the second laser spot so that the second laser spot would not become behaviorally meaningless. We also tested cell responses to the second laser spot by extinguishing it briefly (200 ms) while the monkeys fixated the first spot.
Responses to saccades also were examined. The monkeys first fixated a
stationary target at the center of the screen. After 1-2 s, the target
jumped to a new position 5 or 10° away from the center, and the
monkeys made a saccade to the visible target as described previously
(Tanaka and Fukushima 1998).
Data analysis
The data were analyzed off-line as previously described
(Fukushima et al. 1995, 1999a
). Cell discharge was
discriminated with a dual time-amplitude-window discriminator and
digitized together with eye-, chair-, and target-position signals at
500 Hz using a 16-bit A/D board. All position signals were
differentiated by software to obtain velocity. Gaze velocity was
calculated as the sum of eye velocity and chair velocity. In addition,
eye-position signals were differentiated by analogue circuits (DC-50
Hz,
12 dB/octave) to obtain eye velocity for the task conditions with "blanking" (e.g., Fig. 12). Saccades were marked with a cursor on
eye- and gaze-velocity traces and were removed using an interactive computer program (Fukushima et al. 1995
, 1999a
). Those
occasional bursts or pauses in cell discharge associated with saccades
were marked manually and removed from the analysis. Rasters and
histograms were constructed by averaging between 10 and 30 cycles. Each
cycle was divided into 64 equal bins together with averaged velocity. Marked bursts or pauses in discharge did not contribute to the histograms, although they are shown in the cycle rasters.
To quantify responses, a sine function was fit to the cycle histograms
of cell discharge, exclusive of the bins with zero spike rate, by means
of a least-squared error algorithm. Responses that had a harmonic
distortion (HD) of >50% or a signal-to-noise ratio (S/N) of <1.0
were discarded; S/N was defined as the ratio of the amplitude of the
fitted fundamental frequency to the root mean square amplitude of the
third through eighth harmonics and HD as the ratio of the amplitude of
the second harmonic to that of the fundamental. The phase shift of the
peak of the fitted function relative to upward or rightward stimulus
velocity was calculated as a difference in degrees. Gain was calculated
as the peak amplitude of the fundamental component fitted to the cycle
histogram divided by the peak amplitude of the fitted stimulus velocity
(i.e., target velocity for pursuit and chair velocity for other tasks
during chair rotation, Fig. 1A). Gain 0.10 spikes/s per
°/s was taken as significant modulation. The program also calculated
mean discharge rate of each cycle histogram. For responses with oblique
stimulus directions, radial stimulus velocity first was calculated as a
square root of the sum of the squares of the vertical and horizontal
components, and gain was calculated by dividing amplitude of modulation
of cell activity by radial stimulus velocity. The phase shift of cell
response with oblique preferred direction was calculated relative to
the rightward component of eye, gaze, or stimulus velocity. Eye- and
gaze-velocity responses were calculated similarly after deleting
saccades. Velocity sensitivity of a cell's response was the slope of
the linear regression fit to the amplitude of modulation versus eye- or
gaze-velocity.
Preferred direction of a cell's response was estimated by the method
of Krauzlis and Lisberger (1996) using a Gaussian
function. Discharge rate of each cell during straight-ahead gaze
immediately before each series began was used as the resting rate.
Responses to eight polar directions along the four stimulus planes were examined. Resting discharge rates were usually similar to mean discharge rates of the corresponding cycle histograms. However, for
some cells in which resting rates changed considerably during testing
the eight directions, we estimated the Gaussian fit by plotting gain
(re stimulus velocity). Gain values were plotted as positive for the
increasing discharge and as negative for the direction to which
discharge rate decreased.
To analyze retinal image-motion response (Fig. 1A5), all traces were aligned with the second stimulus cycles. Traces that contained saccades or slow eye movement were removed because they were indicative of the monkeys' failure to fixate the stationary target, and only those traces with eye position changes of <1° during each cycle were analyzed (see Fig. 14). Paired or unpaired Student's t-tests were used for statistical analysis.
Histological procedures
Near the conclusion of the recording period in each monkey, the sites of pursuit-related cell activity were marked by iron deposits produced by passing positive current (10-15 µA for 60-100 s; 800-1200 µ Coulombs) or by electrolytic lesions produced by passing negative current through the microelectrode (20 µA for 30 s). After recording was completed, each monkey was anesthetized deeply by Nembutal (50 mg/kg ip). After histological fixation, coronal sections were made at 100 µm thickness on a freezing microtome. The sections then were stained for cell bodies and fibers, and the locations of recording sites were verified microscopically.
![]() |
RESULTS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
We recorded a total of 110 neurons in the periarcuate areas of two monkeys that responded during ocular tracking of a sinusoidally moving target spot and whole-body rotation. Of these, 103 neurons responded to smooth pursuit (e.g., Fig. 1B1), and the remaining 7 did not. Two of the seven responded during VOR suppression but not during other conditions; responses of the other five during chair rotation (Fig. 1A, 2-4) were similar regardless of the behavioral conditions, suggesting primarily vestibular origin of their responses. Of 103 pursuit-responding neurons, 100 were recorded long enough to allow analysis. Preferred activation directions (see METHODS) were determined for 96, but were indeterminate in the remaining 4 neurons.
Of the 100 pursuit-related neurons, 92 responded during VOR suppression (e.g., Fig. 1, A2 and B2, see METHODS). For the remaining eight, the activity of six cells was modulated during VOR suppression but with HD > 1.0 (typically ~2), indicating biphasic responses. Only two of the eight showed no modulation in discharge rate during VOR suppression. Of the 92, preferred activation directions for 59 were determined from their response during VOR suppression in two (yaw, pitch) or four rotation planes (yaw and pitch plus 2 oblique directions at 45° angles, see METHODS). For the remaining neurons, their responses were recorded in the plane close to the preferred activation directions for smooth pursuit as judged visually and by the audio monitor.
Table 1 summarizes overall mean (±SD) eye gains during different task conditions (Fig. 1A, 1-4) and chair rotation in complete darkness.
|
Comparison of preferred activation directions of periarcuate pursuit-related neurons during smooth-pursuit and VOR suppression tasks
Gaze movement can be performed either by eye movement alone
without head movement (Fig. 1, A1 and B1) or
during whole- body movement without appreciable eye movement by
suppressing the VOR (Fig. 1, A2 and B2). To
examine how periarcuate pursuit-related neurons respond during gaze
movement, we first compared preferred activation directions of
individual cells during these two tasks. The majority of them showed
similar preferred directions during these tasks. A representative cell
is shown in Fig. 2 during eye or gaze
movement to different directions (Fig. 2, A, 1-4, and B, 1-4). Its preferred direction during smooth pursuit was
downward with the peak discharge near peak eye velocity (Fig.
2A1). The preferred direction during VOR suppression was
also downward with the peak discharge near peak gaze velocity (Fig.
2B1). Figure 3 shows examples
of directional tuning of three representative cells (A-C)
with leftward (Fig. 3, A, 1 vs. 2, and B,
1 vs. 2) and downward (Fig. 3C, 1 vs.
2) preferred directions (cf. Krauzlis and Lisberger
1996). For cells that changed resting rates during these tasks,
we plotted their responses as gains (re stimulus velocity) and
determined their preferred directions (Fig. 3C, 1, and
2, see METHODS).
|
|
Figure 4 summarizes preferred directions
of individual cells during smooth pursuit (Fig. 4A) and VOR
suppression (Fig. 4B) in a polar format with respect to the
recording side. Preferred directions for individual cells were
distributed evenly for all directions. Figure 4C compares
preferred directions for individual neurons. In 23 of the 59 neurons
(Fig. 4C, ), preferred directions during VOR suppression
were determined from their response in two (yaw, pitch) rotation
planes, thus from four directions. In the remaining 36 (Fig.
4C,
), preferred directions were determined from their
response in four rotation planes (8 directions; see METHODS). The points are distributed around the unity slope
line confirming that the preferred directions are similar in the two tasks. Only six neurons showed oppositely directed preferred directions (Fig. 4C, *).
|
Classification of periarcuate pursuit-related neurons during pursuit-vestibular interactions: gaze-velocity neurons and eye/head-velocity neurons
The similar preferred activation directions (Fig. 4C)
suggest that the activity of most periarcuate pursuit-related neurons may be related to gaze. To test this possibility, we examined responses
of each cell during the three tasks that dissociate eye movement in the
orbit from eye movement in space (Fig. 1A, 2-4). We
classified pursuit-related cells as "gaze-velocity" if they met the
following criteria that characterized the horizontal gaze-velocity
Purkinje cells of Lisberger and Fuchs (1978; also Fukushima et al. 1999a
): 1) modulation
occurred for movements of the eye (smooth pursuit) and the head (VOR
suppression) in the same direction, 2) modulation during one
of these two tasks was less than twice that during the other, and
3) modulation during the VOR ×1 was less than that during
VOR suppression. In addition, we examined responses during VOR
enhancement (×2) in which the target moved with equal amplitude as,
but in the opposite direction to, the chair. Gaze-velocity neurons
responded maximally for opposite directions (i.e., phase reversal)
during VOR ×2 and suppression (Fig. 1A, 2 vs.
3). Based on these criteria, 66 of the 100 (66%) were
classified as gaze-velocity neurons.
Figure 5A shows activity of a representative gaze-velocity neuron the preferred direction of which during smooth pursuit was upward and in phase with stimulus velocity (Fig. 5A1). During VOR suppression, it also responded well during upward pitch with phase shift of only 23° (re stimulus velocity) compared with its response during pursuit (Fig. 5A, 1 vs. 2). During VOR enhancement (×2, Fig. 5A3), the response reversed its phase compared with that during VOR suppression (Fig. 5A2). During the VOR ×1 (target stationary in space) condition, in which gaze was nearly stable (Fig. 5A4, gaze vel), the modulation was minimal. Thus the activity of this cell during these conditions defines an upward gaze-velocity neuron.
|
As described in the preceding text, the activity of all but 2 of the
remaining 34 cells was modulated during VOR suppression, although 6 of
the 34 showed biphasic modulation so that their responses were not
well-fit by a sinusoid. The response during VOR suppression suggests
that the great majority (32/34) of non-gaze-velocity neurons, like the
gaze-velocity neurons, have vestibular sensitivity but their magnitudes
and/or preferred directions are not consistent with the gaze-velocity
criteria, so we called these cells eye/head-velocity neurons (cf.
Scudder and Fuchs 1992) as in our previous study (Fukushima et al. 1999a
). Of the 26 cells that responded
during VOR suppression without biphasic modulation, 13 preferred
pursuit and VOR suppression in the same direction but did not satisfy all of the criteria for gaze-velocity neurons described in the preceding text. The remaining 13 had oppositely directed eye- and
head-movement sensitivity during pursuit and VOR suppression.
Figure 5B shows the activity of a representative eye/head-velocity neuron the preferred direction of which during pursuit was leftward (Fig. 5B1). Although it increased activity during leftward gaze movement during VOR suppression (Fig. 5B2), a phase reversal was not observed during VOR enhancement (×2, Fig. 5B3). Furthermore it responded clearly during the VOR ×1 condition when the chair moved toward the left (and the eyes moved toward the right) side (Fig. 5B4) with magnitude comparable to that during pursuit (Fig. 5B1). Thus responses of this cell are not related to gaze movement.
Responses of the two types of neurons during these four task conditions (Fig. 1A, 1-4) are summarized in polar plots in Fig. 6 where the radius is the gain and the angle is the phase shift (re stimulus velocity). Gaze-velocity neurons showed similar distributions during smooth pursuit and VOR suppression (Fig. 6A, 1 vs. 2). During VOR enhancement (×2), many gaze-velocity neurons showed responses in the quadrant opposite to that seen during VOR suppression (Fig. 6A, 3 vs. 2), whereas their responses were minimal during the VOR ×1 condition (Fig. 6A4). In contrast, eye/head-velocity neurons showed larger responses during the VOR ×1 condition than gaze-velocity neurons (Fig. 6, B4 vs. A4), and their responses were largest during VOR ×2 (Fig. 6B, 1-4). Table 1 summarizes mean (±SD) gains for the two types of cells at 0.5 Hz (±10°). The average gains for the first three conditions for gaze-velocity neurons were similar but there was no significant modulation for VOR (×1). Mean gains for eye/head-velocity neurons indicated significant differences from gaze-velocity cells during the VOR (×1) condition (P < 0.001).
|
Mean discharge rates (see METHODS) during these tasks were similar for the two types of neurons. Overall mean (±SD) discharge rates during smooth pursuit and VOR suppression were 18.5 ± 11.7 and 19.2 ± 12.7 spikes/s for gaze-velocity neurons and 17.6 ± 10.0 and 21.5 ± 10.5 spikes/s for eye/head-velocity neurons.
To examine more directly whether the activity of periarcuate
gaze-velocity neurons indeed code gaze-velocity during
pursuit-vestibular interactions, in Fig.
7 we plotted amplitude of modulation of 10 representative cells of this group against peak gaze-velocity obtained at different stimulus frequencies (Fig.
8) during our various tasks (Fig.
1A, 2-4). For some cells, we changed the amplitudes of
target movement to twice or half of the chair movement to provide a
wider range of gaze velocities. According to the convention described
by Lisberger and Fuchs (1978), responses were plotted as positive for phase shifts in the direction of those during VOR
suppression and as negative for those with opposite phase. Discharge
modulation was significantly linearly correlated with gaze-velocity;
correlation coefficients ranged from 0.85 to 0.99 (Fig. 7).
|
|
Frequency response
To study frequency response during the four tasks (Fig.
1A, 1-4), each cell was examined at
different stimulus frequencies at a constant amplitude. Figure 8
summarizes phase (top) and gain (middle)
and amplitude of modulation (bottom) against stimulus frequency for individual cells. Since discharge did not vary with preferred directions, their data were combined. Responses of
gaze-velocity () and eye/head-velocity neurons (
) were similar
except that the former tended to show greater modulation, thus higher
gains, at higher stimulus frequencies during pursuit and VOR
suppression (Fig. 8, A and B) and that
many of the latter responded during the VOR ×1 (Fig.
8D) as expected (see Fig. 6B4). Phases of
the two types of neurons were fairly constant over the wide frequency range, either near 0° or ±180°, suggesting that their responses correspond mostly to stimulus velocity (see following text). Gains of
the majority of neurons were constant except at the lowest frequency of
0.1 Hz, particularly during VOR suppression. This also is shown as an
increase in amplitude of modulation at higher frequencies, suggesting
that velocity sensitivity of the majority of cells is constant (Fig.
8).
Comparison of response properties of periarcuate gaze-velocity neurons and eye/head-velocity neurons during pursuit-vestibular interactions
PREFERRED ACTIVATION DIRECTIONS. Periarcuate gaze-velocity neurons and eye/head-velocity neurons showed many differences in their response properties. Figure 4 (D and E) compares preferred directions of smooth pursuit and VOR suppression for these two groups. For gaze-velocity neurons, the two directions were well correlated (Fig. 4D, r = 0.97) with the slope of the least-squares linear regression close to one (0.95). In contrast, six eye/head-velocity neurons had opposite preferred directions (Fig. 4E, *), and no significant correlation was observed between the two variables.
EYE MOVEMENT AND VESTIBULAR-RELATED ACTIVITY. Periarcuate gaze-velocity neurons tended to show more lead than eye/head-velocity neurons during smooth pursuit although phase shifts were distributed widely. We calculated the phase shifts of individual neurons relative to eye velocity and gaze velocity, respectively, in their preferred directions. Figure 9 (A, 1-2, and B, 1-2) summarizes their responses during pursuit (A1 and B1) and VOR suppression (A2 and B2) at 0.5 Hz. The mean phase shifts during pursuit (re eye-velocity) were 14 ± 42° (mean ± SD) lead for gaze-velocity neurons (Fig. 9A1) and 15 ±49° lag for eye/head-velocity neurons (Fig. 9B1). The difference between the two groups was significant (P < 0.05). During VOR suppression, both types of cells showed similar distribution with mean phase leads of 6 ± 43° and 17 ± 38° (re gaze-velocity), respectively (Fig. 9, A2 and B2, P > 0.05).
|
VELOCITY SENSITIVITY. We also examined whether gaze- and eye/head-velocity neurons could be distinguished on the basis of their velocity sensitivity (see METHODS). Comparing the modulation of each cell during smooth pursuit against peak eye velocity at different stimulus frequencies revealed 15/16 gaze-velocity neurons and 4/7 eye/head-velocity neurons (Fig. 10A, *) that had a linear relationship between amplitudes of modulation and peak eye velocity. Mean eye-velocity sensitivity for the two groups was not significantly different (P > 0.05); the overall mean was 0.53 (±0.30 SD) spikes/s per °/s. During VOR suppression, 14/14 gaze-velocity and 3/6 eye/head-velocity neurons showed linear relationships between amplitudes of modulation and peak gaze velocity (Fig. 10B). Mean gaze-velocity sensitivity of the two groups was not significantly different (P > 0.1); the overall mean was 0.50 ± 0.44 spikes/s per °/s.
|
VESTIBULAR RESPONSES IN COMPLETE DARKNESS. Gaze-velocity responses require vestibular input (Fig. 5, A2 and B2). To examine the nature of that input further, the animals were rotated without the target in complete darkness. A total of 43 cells (23 gaze-velocity, 20 eye/head-velocity) was examined by applying rotation in the plane close to their VOR suppression preferred direction (Fig. 4, D and E). Representative responses are shown in Fig. 5. These cells showed a range of responses from weakly but clearly (Fig. 5A5) to strongly modulated (Fig. 5B5). Figure 11 (A and B) summarizes gain and phase (re chair velocity) of the 43 cells. The majority (28/43 = ~65%) responded (Fig. 11, A and B, top), and the mean gain for the responsive cells was 0.30 ± 0.16 spikes/s per °/s (n = 28). Eye/head-velocity neurons (15/20 = 75%) tended to respond with higher gains (re chair velocity) than gaze-velocity neurons (13/23 = ~57%). The mean (±SD) gains (including 0 values) for the two groups were 0.27 ± 0.21 and 0.15 ± 0.15 spikes/s per °/s, respectively (Table 1) with the overall mean for all cells tested of 0.18 ± 0.18 spikes/s per °/s (n = 43). The difference in gain between the two groups is significant (P < 0.02). Phases were distributed widely and almost uniformly (Fig. 11, A and B, bottom) unlike their responses during VOR suppression (Fig. 8B, top), but similar to their responses during VOR ×1 (Fig. 8D, top).
|
Maintenance of gaze-tracking-related activity of periarcuate pursuit-related neurons
By briefly extinguishing the tracking target during step-ramp
pursuit tracking, we have shown recently that periarcuate
pursuit-related neurons maintain their discharge without a tracking
target (Tanaka and Fukushima 1998). In this study, we
used the same technique to examine the discharge of a total of 20 (15 gaze-velocity and 5 eye/head-velocity) neurons during smooth tracking
of a visual target with or without chair rotation (see
METHODS). The majority (13/20 cells during smooth pursuit,
7/9 cells during VOR suppression) continued to discharge during the
200-ms blanking period without consistent change in their activity
(e.g., Fig. 12B1). In the
remaining seven cells, their activity decreased after 100-150 ms after
target offset (e.g., Fig. 12A, 1 vs. 2). To
quantify the blanking effects for the seven cells, we compared mean
discharge rates during the last 100 ms of the 200-ms blanking period
with mean rates for an equivalent portion of the tracking cycle without
blanking. During pursuit, discharge rates decreased on the average by
48% (range 21-73%), whereas during VOR suppression discharge rates decreased by 35%. Thus change in discharge rate during blanking tended
to be less during VOR suppression than during pursuit not only in its
occurrence (2/9 vs. 7/20 cells) but also in its magnitude (35 vs.
48%).
|
We also used an 800-ms blanking period that was applied shortly before
the target changed direction and required the monkeys to continue
tracking (see METHODS). In eight (5 gaze-velocity, 3 eye/head-velocity) neurons, blanking was timed so it occurred before
they normally increased their activity (e.g., Fig. 12, A3 and B2). Gaze-velocity neurons increased activity during
smooth pursuit (Fig. 12A1) and VOR suppression (Fig.
12B1) and also discharged during blanking (Fig. 12,
A3 and B2). Their activity without a visible
target during VOR suppression (Fig. 12B2) cannot be due to
vestibular input alone since chair rotation in complete darkness induced only a minimum response (Fig. 12B3, ). Figure
13 compares responses of the same cell
during pursuit and VOR suppression with and without blanking together
with the corresponding eye (A) and gaze velocity
(B). Cell activity during blanking (thick lines) was
associated with slower eye (A, thick lines) and gaze velocity (B, thick lines) compared with its activity without
blanking (thin lines, open arrows).
|
To quantify the blanking effects, we compared mean discharge rates during the last 200 ms of the 800-ms blanking period with mean rates for an equivalent portion of the tracking cycle without blanking for the eight cells. During smooth pursuit, the overall mean discharge rate decreased by 30% (range 0-52%) and the mean eye velocity decreased by 45% (range 13-72%). During VOR suppression, the overall mean discharge rate decreased by 9% (range: 21% increase to 36% decrease) and the mean gaze-velocity decreased by 14% (range 8-48%). Thus the decrease of discharge rate during blanking was associated with reduced eye- or gaze-velocity, but the reduction was much larger during smooth pursuit than VOR suppression. Phase shifts of each cell's response relative to eye or gaze-velocity remained virtually unchanged (range 17° lead to 14° lag) compared with the control values. Because eye-velocity sensitivity and gaze-velocity sensitivity of these neurons were similar (see preceding text), the difference in their behavior during blanking during pursuit and VOR suppression cannot be explained by their discharge sensitivity to eye velocity and gaze velocity alone. Thus these results suggest that periarcuate pursuit-related neurons initiate discharge associated with voluntary smooth gaze tracking even without a visible target (see DISCUSSION).
Retinal image-velocity responses of periarcuate pursuit-related neurons
For accurate gaze tracking of a moving target during whole-body
rotation, target-velocity-in-space information is necessary and it can
be computed from gaze velocity and retinal image velocity of the target
(Robinson 1981). To examine whether periarcuate pursuit-related neurons respond to retinal image velocity, we tested
the responses of 40 (30 gaze-velocity, 10 eye/head-velocity) cells to a
second laser spot moving sinusoidally while the monkeys fixated the
stationary target (Fig. 1A5). Of the 40, more than half
(21/40 = ~53%, 16/30 gaze-velocity, 5/10 eye/head-velocity neurons)
showed a clear modulation in their discharge with the movement of the
second spot and the remaining neurons (n = 19) showed
no consistent response. The mean gain (re velocity of the 2nd spot at
0.5-0.7 Hz) for the 21 cells was 0.26 ± 0.19 spikes/s per °/s
with the mean phase of 10 ± 89° lead. Gaze-velocity neurons and
eye/head-velocity neurons showed similar phase and gain distributions. Representative activity is shown in Fig.
14A for a gaze-velocity neuron that increased activity during leftward pursuit (Fig.
14A1) and VOR suppression (not shown). During testing with
the second spot, the same cell increased activity when it moved toward
the left (Fig. 14A2), and its response magnitude increased
with significant phase lag when the frequency of sinusoidal movement of
the test spot was increased (Fig. 14A, 3 and 4).
The monkey fixated the stationary target well in all these conditions
(Fig. 14A, 3 and 4). Thus the modulation of cell
activity during this task cannot reflect eye velocity. Rather, it codes
retinal image-motion of the second laser spot.
|
As illustrated in Fig.
15A, preferred activation
directions for retinal image-motion of the second spot (at 0.5-0.7 Hz)
were similar to preferred directions of individual cells during smooth pursuit and VOR suppression (Fig. 15A, 1 vs. 2 and 3). For the population of tested neurons, Fig. 15
(B and C) shows that the preferred directions are
similar (at 0.5-0.7 Hz) particularly for gaze-velocity neurons (;
r = 0.95, 0.94; slope close to 1).
|
Figure 16 summarizes the frequency
response for retinal image-motion-related discharge of 11 cells; 9 were
gaze-velocity neurons (, Fig. 16, A-C). Phases were
mostly constant <1 Hz, but at frequencies >1 Hz, some showed
considerable phase lag (Figs. 14A, 3 and 4, and
16A). Gains of the majority of cells were constant over the frequency range tested (Fig. 16B), which is consistent with
an increase in amplitude of modulation of many neurons at higher stimulus frequencies (Fig. 16C). We calculated velocity
sensitivity to the second spot for the 11 neurons from the slopes of
the amplitude of modulation versus retinal image-velocity plots. The
majority (8/11, all gaze-velocity neurons) showed a linear relationship between amplitudes of modulation and peak retinal image velocity (Fig.
16D). Discharge modulation of many cells increased with
retinal image velocity linearly beyond 100°/s. Correlation
coefficients for the linear regressions for these neurons ranged from
0.59 to 0.98 with the mean of 0.86. Velocity sensitivity ranged from 0.06 to 0.58 with the mean of 0.20 ± 0.16 spikes/s per °/s
(n = 8).
|
Retinal image-motion-related activity (Fig. 14) does not require actual retinal image-motion. In 7 of the 21 cells that responded to retinal image-motion (5 gaze velocity and 2 eye/head velocity), we briefly (200 ms) extinguished the second spot to examine how their activity was affected by the disappearance of this spot. About half of them (3/7) decreased activity during blanking as illustrated in our representative cell (Fig. 14B) then showed a visual response to reappearance of the second spot. We compared mean activity during the last 100 ms of the blanking period with the mean rates for an equivalent portion of the cycle without blanking. The mean activity decreased by 42% (range 33-58%). However, the remaining cells (4/7) maintained their discharge without showing any consistent change in their activity during the blanking period, indicating that periarcuate pursuit-related neurons are capable of maintaining the retinal image-motion response. In 9 of the 21 responding neurons (8 gaze-velocity and 1 eye/head-velocity), we also examined the effects of target blanking (for 200 ms) during smooth pursuit (see preceding text); 6 showed no change in their activity during blanking, whereas 3 decreased activity during blanking.
Activity during saccades
Previous studies showed that periarcuate pursuit-related
neurons do not respond during saccades (Gottlieb et al.
1993; MacAvoy et al. 1991
; Tanaka and
Fukushima 1998
). We examined the response of 56 cells during
visually guided saccades (35 gaze-velocity, 21 eye/head-velocity).
Nearly half (n = 26, 46%) showed some changes in their
activity associated with saccades (14/35 gaze-velocity, 12/21
eye/head-velocity neurons). Gaze- and eye/head-velocity neurons showed
similar responses, the most common of which (n = 16)
was a weak burst with peak activity occurring at the onset of or during
the saccade that sometimes was accompanied by a preceding gradual
increase of discharge rate. Representative responses are shown in Fig.
17 (A and B).
Many had preferred directions. For example, the cell shown in Fig.
17A responded to leftward VOR suppression (Fig.
17A) and leftward pursuit (not shown) and also had a weak burst that preceded saccades toward rightward or upward directions (Fig. 17A2) but not toward leftward or downward (Fig.
17A3). In contrast, the cell shown in Fig. 17B
increased activity during VOR suppression left and up (Fig. 17B1,
inset). This cell discharged a burst of activity irrespective of
saccade directions (Fig. 17B,2 and 3). Such weak
bursts occasionally were followed or preceded by suppression of
activity (n = 3). Pauses without bursts also were
observed (n = 2).
|
Figure 17C is an example whose response might have
been induced by the visual input of presenting the target. This cell
responded best to down and right smooth pursuit. During visually
induced saccades, it gradually increased activity before saccades as is evident when cell activity is aligned with saccade onset (Fig. 17C2). When aligned with target onset (Fig.
17C3), the first activity showed a clear burst with a
latency of ~100 ms, suggesting a visual response to the target onset.
Similar visual responses were observed in a total of five cells during
the visually induced saccade task (latency 100-110 ms).
Eye-position-related activity following saccades also was observed in
some cells (n = 6) as previously reported
(Tanaka and Fukushima 1998). Although many
pursuit-related neurons showed various degrees of burst activity during
visually induced saccades, virtually none of them showed burst activity associated with corrective saccades during VOR suppression or smooth
pursuit (e.g., Fig. 17, A1, B1, and C1, also Fig.
12) when the target was continuously presented. Of eight
pursuit-related neurons that showed burst activity associated with
visually induced saccades, only one also showed bursts associated with
quick phases of vestibular nystagmus in complete darkness.
Recording location
Figure 18
summarizes reconstructed recording locations for
the two monkeys. Recordings in one monkey (U) were performed
mostly in the fundus and posterior bank of the right arcuate sulcus
(Fig. 18, C and D) as in previous studies
(Gottlieb et al. 1993; MacAvoy et al.
1991
; Tanaka and Fukushima 1998
). Recordings in
the other monkey (N) were performed slightly more rostrally
and medially in the superior ramus of the left arcuate sulcus (Fig. 18,
A and B). Thirty of 66 gaze-velocity neurons and
19 of 34 eye/head-velocity neurons were recorded in the monkey
U (Fig. 18, C and D). The remaining 36 gaze-velocity and 15 eye/head-velocity neurons were recorded in the
monkey N (Fig. 18, A and B). We often
observed saccade-related bursts of activity in the same recording
tracks, particularly dorsally in the monkey N (Fig. 18,
A and B). There was no difference in the response
properties of neurons recorded in the two monkeys.
|
![]() |
DISCUSSION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
The main findings from this study are the following. 1) The great majority of pursuit-related neurons in and near the FEF (>92%, n = 100) responded during VOR suppression (Figs. 5A, 6B, and 10B), and the activity of the majority (66/100) was related to gaze velocity (Figs. 6A and 7) with preferred directions of individual cells distributed almost equally for all directions (Fig. 4, A-D). 2) The majority (28/43 = ~65%) responded to chair rotation in complete darkness, suggesting vestibular input (Fig. 11). 3) During smooth gaze tracking regardless of chair rotation, these neurons maintained their discharge even when the tracking target was briefly extinguished (Fig. 12). And 4) more than half of them tested (21/40 = ~53%) responded to retinal image velocity of the second laser spot (Fig. 16D) with preferred directions of individual cells similar to those during smooth pursuit and VOR suppression (Fig. 15). Moreover, these responsive cells maintained retinal image-motion response even when the second target was briefly extinguished (Fig. 14B).
From the mean eye-velocity, head-velocity and retinal
image-motion-velocity sensitivity, we can estimate their discharge
(R) during slow gaze tracking
as
![]() |
Smooth-gaze-tracking areas in and near the FEF
Smooth-pursuit-related areas near the FEF have been identified in
macaques as the posterior bank and fundus of the arcuate sulcus
(Gottlieb et al. 1994; MacAvoy et al.
1991
; Tanaka and Fukushima 1998
; cf. Tian
and Lynch 1996a
,b
). In Cebus monkeys, smooth-pursuit-related
areas also were found in the superior arcuate sulcus near its medial
tip (Fig. 5 of Tian and Lynch 1996a
). Gaze-velocity neurons were found in both of the periarcuate areas (Fig. 18), suggesting that pursuit-related areas may be more widespread in and
near the FEF including the surrounding area 6 (Tian and Lynch 1996a
,b
).
Two types of periarcuate pursuit-related neurons and vestibular input
The present results indicate that periarcuate pursuit-related neurons can be separated into two types on the basis of their response properties during pursuit-vestibular interaction conditions (Fig. 1A, 2-4): gaze-velocity and eye/head-velocity neurons (Figs. 5 and 6). These two types of neurons showed similar discharge characteristics during smooth pursuit and VOR suppression (Figs. 8-10) and retinal image motion of the second target (Figs. 15 and 16), although the number of eye/head-velocity neurons examined was small in the latter two conditions. In contrast to gaze-velocity neurons that had similar preferred directions for eye and vestibular sensitivity (Fig. 4D), eye/head-velocity neurons include cells that showed oppositely directed eye and vestibular sensitivity (Fig. 4E). Moreover, vestibular responses of these two types of neurons were different during chair rotation in complete darkness (Fig. 11, A and B, Table 1). These results indicate that the main difference in their activity during pursuit-vestibular interactions (Fig. 1A, 2-4) can be attributed to their vestibular input.
Previous studies reported vestibular responses in the motor cortex
(areas 4 and 6) but not in the FEF in anesthetized cats (Boisacq-Schepens and Hanus 1972; Frederickson et
al. 1974
; Mergner 1979
) or alert monkeys
(White and Brinkman 1988
) after electrical stimulation
of the vestibular nerve (see Fukushima 1997
for review). The present results indicate that vestibular related signals are widely
distributed in the FEF and nearby area 6 not only for cells related to
smooth pursuit but also for cells that are unrelated to eye movement
(Figs. 11, A and B, and 18).
Discharge characteristics of periarcuate pursuit-related neurons seem
similar to those found in the posterior parietal cortex, particularly
the MST (Komatsu and Wurtz 1988; Newsome et al.
1988
), as well as in the cerebellar vermis
(Büttner et al. 1991
; Kase et al.
1979
; Sato and Noda 1992
; Suzuki and
Keller 1988
). Both areas contain cells carrying gaze-velocity
and retinal image-velocity signals. For example, visual tracking
neurons in the MST (Kawano et al. 1984
, 1994
;
Sakata et al. 1983
; Thier and Erickson
1992
) carry gaze-velocity related signals (Thier and
Erickson 1992
) although others do not (e.g., type C neurons of
Kawano et al. 1984
). The periarcuate eye/head-velocity
neurons that have oppositely directed eye and vestibular sensitivity
resemble type C neurons in the MST (Kawano et al. 1984
).
Reciprocal connections between the periarcuate pursuit-related areas
and MST (Stanton et al. 1993
, 1995
; Tian and
Lynch 1996a
,b
; Tusa and Ungerleider 1988
) may
contribute to the similarity in the discharge characteristics of these
two areas.
Maintenance of gaze velocity-related discharge and retinal image-velocity response of periarcuate pursuit-related neurons
This study shows that activity of periarcuate pursuit-related
neurons is maintained during a brief period without a tracking target
not only during pursuit as previously reported (Tanaka and
Fukushima 1998) but also during VOR suppression (Fig.
12B1). Decreased discharge during blanking of the target
tended to be much less during VOR suppression than during pursuit,
possibly reflecting the vestibular input in part during VOR
suppression. Periarcuate neurons also responded during smooth
gaze-tracking direction-changes even without the tracking target (Fig.
12, A3 and B2). This task condition (Fig. 12,
A3 and B2) requires processing of the preceding
retinal image-velocity-information to estimate the appropriate
smooth-gaze-tracking response at the time an invisible target would
have changed directions. Appropriate discharge of our cells during such
direction changes even without the tracking target suggests an
involvement of these cells in initiation of such eye/gaze movement.
This study further shows that the retinal image-velocity response of some of our cells to the second target spot was maintained even when it was invisible (Fig. 14), suggesting that their activity is not a simple reflection of retinal image-motion response. It is possible that it may partly reflect reconstructed components of the second target motion in our task condition.
Possible role of the periarcuate cortical areas for visually guided behavior
To track a moving object accurately during whole-body rotation,
gaze-velocity signals must be calculated to match the velocity of the
eyes in space to target velocity (Robinson 1981).
Target-velocity-in-space can be calculated by retinal image velocity of
the target and gaze velocity. As this study shows, single
pursuit-related neurons in the periarcuate areas carry both signals.
FEF lesions or chemical inactivation of these areas severely impairs
smooth pursuit (Keating 1991
, 1993
; Shi et al.
1998
). After muscimol infusion into the periarcuate
pursuit-related areas our monkeys were unable, in many trials, to
generate smooth gaze movement when the tracking target was extinguished
before changing direction in an identical tracking condition to that in
Fig. 12A3 (Fukushima et al. 1999b
). These
results together suggest that the periarcuate cortical areas are
necessary for calculation of target velocity in space and/or gaze-velocity command signals, although how this is accomplished is
still unknown.
Gaze-movement- and/or retinal image-velocity-related signals are found
in many areas in the brain (i.e., MST, pontine nuclei, the cerebellar
vermis and the central thalamus) (e.g., Schlag and Schlag-Rey
1986; see Keller and Heinen 1991
for review) in addition to the periarcuate cortical areas. It has been suggested that
MST forms an internal positive feedback circuit in the pursuit system
that provides signals for the maintenance of pursuit (Newsome et
al. 1988
). It is possible that similar positive feedback
circuits include periarcuate pursuit-related neurons for calculation of target-velocity-in-space and/or gaze-velocity command signals (Fukushima 1997
; Tanaka and Fukushima
1998
), thus enabling those neurons to maintain
discharge during the blanking in response to the second target movement
(Fig. 14) or during maintenance of gaze tracking (Fig. 12).
Although periarcuate pursuit-related neurons seem to be positioned to
issue gaze-velocity commands, they remain much more central than the
cerebellar floccular lobe (Fukushima et al. 1999a; Krauzlis and Lisberger 1996
; Lisberger and Fuchs
1978
; Miles and Fuller 1975
; Miles et al.
1980
, Shidara and Kawano 1993
; Stone and
Lisberger 1990
). Preferred directions of gaze-velocity signals for the periarcuate neurons are uniformly distributed, whereas those
for floccular gaze-velocity Purkinje cells are sorted into roughly
horizontal and vertical components (Fukushima et al.
1999a
; Krauzlis and Lisberger 1996
; Miles
et al. 1980
, Shidara and Kawano 1993
;
Stone and Lisberger 1990
). The majority of periarcuate
pursuit-related neurons discharge before the initiation of pursuit
using a step-ramp pursuit task with the median latency of
19 to
12
ms (Gottlieb et al. 1994
; Tanaka and Fukushima
1998
), whereas many of floccular gaze-velocity Purkinje cells
are reported to discharge at or after the initiation of pursuit
(Stone and Lisberger 1990
; cf. Shidara and Kawano
1993
).
Gaze-velocity responses of our periarcuate gaze-velocity neurons seem
specific to the task conditions because most lose their gaze-velocity-related response during chair rotation in complete darkness in which the animals were not required to perform any task.
This difference between periarcuate neurons and floccular or brain stem
neurons (e.g., Lisberger and Fuchs 1978; Scudder and Fuchs 1992
) may be due to the more direct connection of the latter to their afferents.
Nearly half of our pursuit-related neurons (46%), that include
gaze-velocity and eye/head-velocity neurons, showed activity changes
associated with visually induced saccades. In particular, their various
degrees of burst activity (Fig. 17, A and
B) resemble the well-known activity of saccade-related
FEF neurons (Bruce and Goldberg 1985). Although we did
not examine further the nature of saccade-related activity of our
neurons, our results suggest the possibility that some pursuit-related
neurons in the periarcuate areas carry saccade-related signals as well.
Neurons that respond both to saccades and pursuit have been reported in
the cerebellar vermis (e.g., Suzuki and Keller 1988
) and
fastigial nucleus (e.g., Fuchs et al. 1993
).
Gaze-related activity is also evident in neurons related to
arm-reaching tasks in the premotor cortex near the posterior arcuate
area (Mushiake et al. 1997
; cf. Boussaoud et al.
1993
). These results substantiate the importance of
gaze-related activity in the periarcuate areas for visually guided
behavior that involves not only the eyes and but also the extremities.
![]() |
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS |
---|
We thank Dr. Yasuo Suzuki for surgical procedures, Dr. Sergei Kurkin for computer programs, and T. Yasuda for technical assistance.
This research was supported by grants from Core Research for Evolutional Science and Technology of the Japan Science and Technology Corporation, Japanese Ministry, Science, Culture and Sports (09268201, 09680806, 09670971), and Marna Cosmetics and National Institutes of Health Grants EY-06558 and RR-00166.
Present addresses: J. Fukushima, College of Medical Technology, Hokkaido University, Sapporo 060-0812; Y. Shinmei, Dept. of Ophthalmology, Hokkaido University School of Medicine, Sapporo 060-8638, Japan.
![]() |
FOOTNOTES |
---|
Address for reprint requests: K. Fukushima, Dept. of Physiology, Hokkaido University School of Medicine, West 7, North 15, Sapporo 060-8638, Japan.
The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. The article must therefore be hereby marked "advertisement" in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
Received 24 May 1999; accepted in final form 9 August 1999.
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|