Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, Champaign, Illinois 61820
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Lee, Daeyeol and Joseph G. Malpeli. Effects of saccades on the activity of neurons in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 79: 922-936, 1998. Effects of saccades on individual neurons in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) were examined under two conditions: during spontaneous saccades in the dark and during stimulation by large, uniform flashes delivered at various times during and after rewarded saccades made to small visual targets. In the dark condition, a suppression of activity began 200-300 ms before saccade start, peaked ~100 ms before saccade start, and smoothly reversed to a facilitation of activity by saccade end. The facilitation peaked 70-130 ms after saccade end and decayed during the next several hundred milliseconds. The latency of the facilitation was related inversely to saccade velocity, reaching a minimum for saccades with peak velocity >70-80°/s. Effects of saccades on visually evoked activity were remarkably similar: a facilitation began at saccade end and peaked 50-100 ms later. When matched for saccade velocity, the time courses and magnitudes of postsaccadic facilitation for activity in the dark and during visual stimulation were identical. The presaccadic suppression observed in the dark condition was similar for X and Y cells, whereas the postsaccadic facilitation was substantially stronger for X cells, both in the dark and for visually evoked responses. This saccade-related regulation of geniculate transmission appears to be independent of the conditions under which the saccade is evoked or the state of retinal input to the LGN. The change in activity from presaccadic suppression to postsaccadic facilitation amounted to an increase in gain of geniculate transmission of ~30%. This may promote rapid central registration of visual inputs by increasing the temporal contrast between activity evoked by an image near the end of a fixation and that evoked by the image immediately after a saccade.
The lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) receives a multiplicity of nonretinal inputs (for a recent review, see Sherman and Guillery 1996 Animal preparation
Data were collected from seven LGN in four cats. All surgeries were done aseptically under barbiturate anesthesia (sodium thiopental, intravenous, supplemented by buprenorphine, 0.0075 mg/kg im). Two or three doses of buprenorphine were given every 12 h after surgery for postoperative analgesia. Care and use of animals were in accordance with the guidelines of the American Physiological Society, the Society for Neuroscience, and the University of Illinois Laboratory Animal Care Advisory Committee.
Visual stimuli and data collection
All aspects of stimulus delivery and data collection (including behavioral tasks used in the visual-stimulation condition described in following text) were under computer control. Images were presented on a 58 × 58 cm rear-project screen located 47 cm from the eyes (subtending 63 × 63°). The fixation target was a spot produced by a laser beam the location of which was controlled with mirror galvanometers. Its intensity was adjusted with neutral density filters so that it was brighter and much more salient than other images. Sinusoidal gratings (used only for cell classification) or rectangles of uniform brightness (used to test effects of saccades on visually evoked responses) were produced by an image generator (Picasso CRT Image Synthesizer, Innisfree), displayed on a Tektronix 608 monitor, and projected onto the screen by a lens. The general background level of the screen was 1.7 × 10
Experimental paradigms
To assess effects of oculomotor behavior on LGN activity in the absence of visual stimuli, data were collected for 10-15 min in the dark for each cell. The animals had no task to perform while in the dark, and during this time free rewards occasionally were given. They usually looked about spontaneously, but when their eyes were stationary for more than a few seconds, we tapped on the walls of the experimental chamber to encourage saccades. Cats have the reputation of being difficult to keep awake, but we did not experience this problem with animals trained to work for food even during the periods in the dark.
Data analysis
For both dark and visual-stimulation conditions, saccade start was taken as the point when eye velocity exceeded 25°/s, saccade end as the point where velocity dropped below 10°/s, and saccade duration as the interval between these two points. Usually, an eye movement was considered a saccade only if its velocity exceeded 40°/s. However, for examining the relationship between saccade amplitude and peak velocity, any eye movement with velocity exceeding 25°/s was counted as a saccade. Trials were discarded if the velocity of secondary eye movements exceeded 25°/s within 1 s of saccade end.
Comparison of saccade metrics in the dark and in the light
As will be pointed out later, the latency of saccade-related facilitation of LGN activity varied with saccade velocity. There was a fairly linear relationship between peak velocity and saccade amplitude, similar to that reported by Evinger and Fuchs (1978) Activity during saccadic eye movements in the dark
Effects of saccades on "spontaneous" activity in the dark were examined for 71 cells (an analysis by cell type and LGN layer is provided later
Interactions of saccadic eye movements with visually evoked responses
Sixty-nine cells were examined in the visual-stimulation condition for left and right saccade directions, and 36 of these also were examined for up and down directions. Twenty-eight cells were tested in the centrifugal saccade paradigm and 41 in the centripetal saccade paradigm. The mean receptive-field eccentricity was 8.0 ± 2.73° (range 3.2-15.1°). The relationships between saccade and flash timing in different conditions (SODV = 0, SODP = 0, 50, and 100 ms) are shown in Fig. 7. The velocity-triggered condition essentially probed the beginning phase of saccades, and longer SODPs probed periods of stable eye position immediately after saccades. The eye was generally still moving during the 0-ms SODP condition so that condition probed the late phase of saccades.
Comparisons across cell categories
The distribution of our sample with regard to cell type (X or Y), LGN layer, and polarity of receptive-field center is given in Table 1. Saccade-related, fractional changes in activity for both the dark and visual-stimulation conditions are compared across different categories of cells in Fig. 12. The main difference is that the postsaccadic facilitation was substantially larger (by roughly a factor of 2) for X cells relative to Y cells both in the dark and during visual activation. The data revealed no significant differences in effects of saccades for the other comparisons. Not shown in Fig. 12 are similar comparisons for centripetal versus centrifugal saccade paradigms and for ipsiversive versus contraversive saccades, neither of which yielded significant differences.
Burst responses
For saccades in the dark, the overwhelming number of low-threshold Ca2+ bursts consisted of only two spikes (on average, 2.1 spikes/burst). Mean burst frequency was 0.094/s, which is identical to the rate reported by Guido and Weyand (1995) Presaccadic suppression
The data for saccades in the dark revealed a modest suppression of activity that began 200-300 ms before saccade start, extended through most of the saccade, and terminated at saccade end (Fig. 6). By the nature of our paradigms, we could not assess changes in LGN activity preceding the saccade for the visual-stimulation condition, but there is no reason to believe that the underlying effect is absent in the light. Indeed, a hint of a suppression for the earliest flash intervals can be seen in Fig. 10, bottom.
Postsaccadic facilitation
The largest effect of saccades on the activity of LGN cells was a facilitation that began at saccade end and peaked 70-130 ms later. The facilitation was independent of the motivation for the saccade in that it was similar for spontaneous saccades in the dark and for rewarded saccades made to discrete visual targets (Fig. 10). It was also independent of the location from which the saccade was launched and the predictability of the saccade-target location because it did not differ for centrifugal and centripetal saccade paradigms. Finally, it was largely independent of the state of the retina: the time course and magnitude of the postsaccadic facilitation were quite similar in the dark and visual-stimulation conditions, even though the firing rate of the LGN cells (and presumably the retinal ganglion cells driving them) differed radically. The only factor definitively influencing the facilitation was saccade velocity with higher velocities producing a larger facilitation that peaked more rapidly up to a critical peak velocity of 70-80°/s (Fig. 11). We conclude that this facilitation is a generalized, saccade-related increase in the gain of LGN transmission, resulting from the interaction of oculomotor and/or proprioceptive signals with retinal signals in a simple multiplicative fashion. Although we cannot exclude the possibility that patterned stimuli might have resulted in a different degree of facilitation, we think it unlikely because of the apparent automatic nature of this process.
Possible sources of oculomotor signals
Presently, none of the many potential sources of oculomotor or proprioceptive signals to the LGN can be eliminated as contributors to gain changes associated with saccades. This is so even for structures for which such signals have not been previously observed: effects of saccades in the LGN were rarely obvious for single cells, and signal averaging across cells comparable with that of the current study has seldom been employed for other structures. Nevertheless, there are some obvious candidates. The striate cortex appears to receive proprioceptive signals (Ashton et al. 1984 Function of saccade-related gain changes
The effect of saccades on the LGN can be summarized as a biphasic change in the gain of LGN transmission: a presaccadic decrease in gain that smoothly changes to a postsaccadic increase at saccade end. One could interpret these two phases as partial solutions to distinctly different problems: the suppression raising perceptual thresholds to avoid confusion resulting from rapid image motion and the facilitation aiding analysis of new images after each saccade. However, although there is both perceptual and physiological evidence for centrally originating saccadic suppression, there is at present no need to assume that the function of this suppression is to avoid perceptual confusion. As pointed out by Judge et al. (1980b)
INTRODUCTION
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
). For this reason and because of its early position in the visual pathway, the LGN has long been considered a site where visual- and saccade-related signals might converge. Indeed, there is considerable evidence that saccades modulate LGN transmission of visually evoked activity in both the cat (Fischer et al. 1996
; see also Noda 1975
) and monkey (Bartlett et al. 1976
; Büttner and Fuchs 1973
; Chukoskie et al. 1995
).
). Perceptual saccadic suppression appears to begin before saccade start, peak during the saccade, and dissipate by saccade end (Latour 1962
; Volkmann et al. 1968
; Zuber and Stark 1966
). Such suppression, however, need not depend entirely on oculomotor signals. Three categories of mechanisms have been proposed to account for it: centrally originating suppression of visual transmission (Holt 1903
), visual masking (Dodge 1905
; MacKay 1970
), and mechanical shearing forces in the retina (Richards 1969
). In this context, centrally mediated suppression usually is assumed to involve oculomotor signals (either corollary discharge or proprioceptive feedback), and visual masking refers to any destructive interference with perception resulting from the spatiotemporal pattern of the image (whether or not retinal image motion is caused by an eye movement). There is psychophysical evidence for both masking (Campbell and Wurtz 1978
; MacKay 1970
; Matin et al. 1972
; for a review, see Volkmann 1986
) and central suppression (Burr et al. 1994
; Latour 1962
; Riggs and Manning 1982
; Volkmann 1962
; Zuber and Stark 1966
).
, proposed that "cancellation" of previous images promotes "undisturbed transfer of the following retinal image," and centrally mediated suppression might underlie such a resetting mechanism. However, postsaccadic facilitation has been invoked more commonly as an aid in registration of new scenes (Jüttner and Röhler 1993
; Lal and Friedlander 1990
; Wolf et al. 1978
, 1980
). It also has been suggested that postsaccadic facilitation plays a role in evaluating saccade accuracy and programming corrective saccades (Becker 1976
; Deubel et al. 1982
).
; Noda 1975
) and for the monkey, present in <5% of cells (Büttner and Fuchs 1973
) or of much smaller magnitude than in the light (Bartlett et al. 1976
). This is odd, given that proprioceptive signals from the extraocular muscles modulate LGN activity in anesthetized cats (Donaldson and Dixon 1980
; Lal and Friedlander 1990
; see also Molotchnikoff and Casanova 1985
for similar results in the rabbit) and that cells in the pretectum projecting to the LGN carry strong saccade-related signals in the dark (Schmidt 1996
). Furthermore, cat LGN activity is modulated during the saccade-like rapid eye movements of sleep (Bizzi 1966
) and during vestibular nystagmus in encéphale isolé preparations (Jeannerod and Putkonen 1971
). If oculomotor inputs to the LGN are actively gated, the circumstances under which saccadic modulation of geniculate transmission is present or blocked might provide important clues as to the role of such modulation in perception. On the other hand, it might be that these inputs are undiminished in the dark but that their effects on LGN gain are obscured by low, variable rates of activity.
; Fischer et al. 1996
; Noda 1975
). Examining saccades in the dark potentially sidesteps these problems by eliminating any contribution of masking and allowing oculomotor influences to be directly detected. However, as pointed out above, relatively low rates of activity in the dark may obscure changes in excitability. Experiments with anesthetized animals (Donaldson and Dixon 1980
; Lal and Friedlander 1990
; Molotchnikoff and Casanova 1985
) allow precise control of stimulus parameters but at the expense of dealing with an abnormal brain and limiting eye-movement signals to proprioceptive input. In the present study, we decided to reexamine the effects of saccades in the dark on LGN activity with extensive averaging of neural responses to reveal saccade-related signals. For saccades in the light, we gave up the possibility of using optimal, patterned stimuli in return for the ability to decouple the timing of visual stimuli from saccade dynamics. Head-fixed cats made rewarded saccades to small targets, and at various times during and after saccades, visual stimuli were flashed onto the receptive fields of LGN cells. These stimuli were very large, uniform flashes that always extended well beyond the borders of the receptive fields of recorded LGN cells. We assumed that responses evoked several hundred milliseconds after saccade end could be taken as baseline and took differences between these and responses evoked during and immediately after saccades as measures of saccade-related changes. Our results suggest that oculomotor influences on LGN activity are substantial and have the same time course and are of similar magnitude, whether in the dark or at the peak of visually evoked activity. Activity undergoes a modest drop before and during saccades, followed by a larger postsaccadic facilitation beginning at saccade end. These changes appear to be related only to saccade dynamics and are independent of the timing or even the presence of visual stimuli.
METHODS
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
. An aluminum "crown" was bonded with antibiotic-doped bone-cement (Palacos R. Smith and Nephew Richards) to stainless-steel studs tapped into the skull. Scleral search coils (Robinson 1963
) were implanted on both eyes (Judge et al. 1980a
). During all training and experimental sessions, the head was immobilized by a fixture attached to the crown. A fiber-glass cap mounted on the crown protected skull implants between experimental sessions.
). During recording sessions, the electrode was advanced by a stepping motor via a telescoping shaft connected to the microelectrode drive by universal joints.
4 cd/m2, which, for fully dilated pupils, is ~3 log units below the mesopic range (Lee et al. 1992
). The projection screen was the only source of light in the recording chamber. To dark-adapted human observers, the screen was clearly visible at background level but it appeared uniform and featureless.
were sampled at 250 Hz by 12-bit A/D converters. Motorized flaps controlled the exposure of the eyes independently. Once a cell was isolated, small rectangles, either brighter or darker than the background, were used to locate its receptive field and determine center polarity (ON or OFF).
min)/(max + min)] were used to classify X and Y cells (Enroth-Cugell and Robson 1966
). After the cat acquired the fixation target, a large (
20 × 20°), fixed-position, contrast-reversing (2 Hz), sinusoidal grating was projected over the receptive field; elsewhere, the screen was illuminated at the general background level of 1.7 × 10
4 cd/m2. Temporally, the contrast reversals were abrupt not sinusoidal. A cell was classified X if excited by only one phase of contrast reversal and if no responses were obtained for a particular position of the eye on the grating (nulling of the response; Fig. 1, top). It was classified Y if excited by contrast reversals of both phases for all eye positions (frequency doubling) (Hochstein and Shapley 1976
) and not nulled for any position of the eye relative to the grating (Fig. 1, bottom). With paralyzed, anesthetized animals, one normally accomplishes this by shifting the zero crossings of contrast-reversing gratings relative to the stationary eye. However, in the present study, the counterphased grating was stationary on the screen, and its position on the retina was varied by the cat's natural fixation behavior. Small eye movements typically broke up a trial into several stable fixation periods, each of which was sufficiently long to evaluate the effects of several cycles of contrast reversal. Multiple trials provided the opportunity to assess many combinations of grating and eye position. Small variations in fixation position were further encouraged by pseudorandomly drawing from several (3-5) fixation targets, horizontally separated by 1-2°. Failure to null the response was associated always with obvious frequency doubling, whereas null positions (of the eye relative to the grating) were observed readily in those cells that responded only to one phase of contrast reversal. We previously have used systematically shifted, contrast-reversing gratings to classify LGN cells in paralyzed, anesthetized cats (Lee et al. 1992
). In comparison, relying on the cat's natural eye movement behavior to shift the grating is faster, and the classifications are at least as clear-cut. We have no reason to believe that these classifications differed from those obtained in anesthetized animals, including those previously reported by this laboratory (Lee et al. 1992
). As will be documented in RESULTS, effects of eye movements on LGN activity differed substantially for X and Y cells, an outcome that we take as evidence for the adequacy of our classification method.
View larger version (28K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 1.
Method of categorizing X and Y cells. Response histograms ( ) show the inverse of the interspike interval during presentation of a counterphased (2 Hz), sinusoidal grating during a single behavioral trial. Horizontal (H) and vertical (V) eye position are given as separate traces. Horizontal bars (bottom) indicate (by
and
) the points in time when the grating was counterphased abruptly. Tick marks on the abscissa indicate 100 ms. Top: example of a X cell recorded in layer A1. At the beginning of the trial, it responded to only 1 phase of reversal (1st 3 filled arrows). After a small spontaneous shift of gaze midway through the trial, there were no clear responses to either phase (nulling of the response; open arrows). Near the end of the trial, another change in eye position resulted in a response to the opposite phase of the grating (last filled arrow). Spatial frequency of the grating was 0.54 cycles/deg. Bottom: example of a Y cell recorded in layer A. This cell responded to both phases of reversal (filled arrows), and no nulling of the response was observed for this or several other trials. Response associated with the 10th arrow was smeared out because the eye was in motion during the phase reversal of the grating (spatial frequency = 1.0 cycle/deg).
3 cd/m2, against a background of 1.7 × 10
4 cd/m2. For OFF-center cells, the luminance of the stimulus and background were reversed. To keep flashes adequately centered on the receptive field, eye-position signals were used to update flash location every 4 ms. The monitor producing the flashes was refreshed at 200 Hz, but because of interactions with the computer sampling cycle, updating the display could take as long as 10 ms. The peak velocity of saccades under these conditions rarely exceeded 300°/s (see Fig. 3), so the center of the receptive field was always within ~3° of the center of the 20 × 20° stimulus. However, stabilization was usually much better than this because most flashes were delivered not at peak saccade velocity but at late phases of saccades or after the eye was stationary.
View larger version (23K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 3.
Saccade amplitude vs. peak saccade velocity for saccades in the dark (dark: n = 6,605, slope = 9.29°/s per deg, intercept = 12.1°/s) and saccades to visual targets (light: n = 3,494, slope = 19.1°/s per deg, intercept = 23.2°/s). These relationships are distorted somewhat by the exclusion of saccades with peak velocities <25°/s from the analysis: it is likely that the true intercepts are closer to 0°/s.
View larger version (15K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 2.
Spatial and temporal arrangement of visual stimuli used to study the effects of saccades in centrifugal and centripetal saccade paradigms. , saccades, which were directed either from the central fixation target to 1 of 4 peripheral targets (centrifugal saccade paradigm) or from peripheral targets to the central target (centripetal saccade paradigm). Squares (not to scale) represent large (~20 × 20°), uniformly bright, 500-ms flashes that were initiated at various phases of the saccade. Eye-position signals were used to keep the flashes roughly centered on the receptive field during saccades. Traces labeled Target 1 and Target 2 give the temporal relationship between the initial fixation target and the target to which the saccade was directed. In the position-triggered condition, the flash was triggered after a variable delay (position-triggered stimulus onset delay, SODP) from when the eye entered a 6 × 6° window (dashed line) centered on the saccade target. In the velocity-triggered condition, the flash was triggered after a variable delay (velocity-triggered stimulus onset delay, SODV) from the point when eye velocity exceeded 25°/s (dashed line).
NPOST/
NPRE)
1, taken as the "dark postsaccadic facilitation index." This index is positive for greater activity after saccade end and negative for lesser activity. To examine effects of saccade direction, the Michelson contrast, (NPOST
NPRE)/(NPOST + NPRE), was calculated for each saccade, and a directional tuning curve computed with a binwidth of 45°, smoothed with 90° wide moving boxcar. The difference between the largest and smallest values of this curve was used as a measure of the influence of saccadic direction on postsaccadic facilitation.
20 cells each contributed five or more saccades to that bin. When the sample was subdivided for comparisons of different subgroups of cells (Fig. 12), these criteria were relaxed to five or more cells each contributing five or more saccades.
View larger version (31K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 12.
Time course of fractional changes in activity aligned on saccade end, comparing different categories of cells for saccades in the dark and during visual stimulation: X and Y cells (top left), ON- and OFF-center cells (top right), different LGN layers (bottom left), and cells with receptive fields central to and peripheral to the median receptive field eccentricity of 8.0° (bottom right). Conventions are the same as in Fig. 11. Only significant difference for saccades in the dark is in the postsaccadic facilitations of X and Y cells(P = 0.03 for the interval from 0 to 250 ms after saccade end). For the 4th and 5th intervals during visually evoked activity (the peak of the facilitation), there was a significant difference between groups only for the X-Y comparison (P = 0.03).
; Silverman 1986
). This optimizes temporal resolution by continually adjusting the width of the smoothing window to keep it inversely proportional to the square root of spike frequency (
p = 10 ms was used as the initial estimate of the spike density function) (see Richmond et al. 1990
). Latencies were adjusted for delays related to the refresh of the display. There was an uncertainty of ±2.5 ms in this adjustment, but errors were distributed randomly and did not vary systematically across conditions. Because the data were averaged over many trials, any residual errors would not have significantly affected computations of mean effects of saccades on response latency.
, Lu et al. 1992
; Steriade and Llinás 1988
). To see if such bursts contribute significantly to saccadic modulation of LGN activity, we identified them, using the criteria employed by Lu et al. (1992)
: two or more spikes with interspike intervals
4 ms preceded by a silent period
100 ms. For the dark condition, bursts were counted in intervals that averaged 1,964 ms, consisting of a 800 ms-period before saccade start, the saccade itself and a 1,000-ms after saccade end. Because of the required 100-ms silent period, any burst occurring within the first 100 ms of a counting interval could not be identified as such, so 100 ms was subtracted from each counting interval for calculating burst frequency. For the visual-stimulation condition, each flash was divided into five 100-ms intervals, and burst were counted separately for each interval.
RESULTS
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
, but the slope of the function for saccades in the dark was about half that for saccades to visual targets: 9.29 versus 19.1°/s per deg (Fig. 3). Mean saccade amplitude was similar in both cases (8.0° in the dark, 7.5° to visual targets), whereas mean peak velocity was much higher for saccades to the laser target (165.7 vs. 86.1°/s). Saccade duration averaged 164.7 ± 69.2 (SD) ms in the dark and 89.9 ± 26.6 ms for saccades to visual targets. Although duration was quite variable in the light, it did not vary significantly with amplitude (the slope of the duration vs. amplitude function was 0.49 ms/deg; r = 0.038). Because the slope of the velocity versus amplitude function was lower for saccades in the dark than for saccades to visual targets, one would expect duration to increase with amplitude in the dark, and this was the case (the slope of the duration versus amplitude function was 11.0 ms/deg, r = 0.559).
see Table 1). The mean number of saccades analyzed for each cell was 55.8 ± 29.9. The mean rates of activity in the 500-ms periods before and after saccade end were 29.8 and 33.0 spikes/s, respectively. These rates differed significantly for 47 cells: 46 (65%) had relatively higher activity in the postsaccadic interval, and one cell had lower activity in this interval (Fig. 4, right). Figure 4, left, shows the pre- and postsaccadic activity for each cell, plotted on a log-log scale. The absolute level of activity varied among cells by a factor of 6. The data fall on a line parallel to the main diagonal, which, on a log-log plot, indicates a constant fractional increase in activity after the saccade. This suggests that over a wide range of firing rates the effect of saccades is multiplicative, rather than additive, i.e., a change in gain. Saccade direction was not a significant factor in the postsaccadic facilitation: the magnitude of the facilitation varied with direction for only five cells (7%).
View this table:
TABLE 1.
Distribution of sample with regard to experimental condition, LGN layer, cell type, and receptive-field center polarity
View larger version (17K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 4.
Effects of saccades in the dark on the activity of 71 LGN cells. Filled symbols or bars indicate statistical significance (P < 0.05). Left: comparison of activity in 500-ms periods before and after saccade end, plotted on a log-log scale. For the majority of cells, activity increased after saccade end. Right: distribution of dark postsaccadic facilitation index (see METHODS). Mean value of this index was 0.13 for all cells and 0.18 for those cells showing a statistically significant facilitation.
View larger version (54K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 5.
Effects of spontaneous saccades in the dark on the activity of an ON-center X cell located in layer A, aligned on saccade end. Top: horizontal (H) and vertical (V) eye movement traces. Middle: raster showing each action potential for all trials meeting the criteria for analysis. Bottom: spike density function computed by convolving the spike train with a Gaussian filter with a standard deviation of 10 ms. Postsaccadic facilitation index for this cell was 0.28 (P < 0.001), which is among the highest observed.
View larger version (27K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 6.
Activity during saccades in the dark, averaged for all 71 cells, aligned on saccade start (top) and end (bottom). Bold curves are for all saccades (median peak velocity = 68.1°/s); thin curves are for saccades with peak velocities above the median (mean = 104.5°/s); dashed curves, saccades with peak velocities below the median (mean = 52.8°/s).
View larger version (23K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 7.
Timing of flash onset with respect to saccade start (left) and saccade end (right) for several stimulus conditions. Individual panels show data for a velocity-triggered stimulus (SODV = 0 ms) and 3 position-triggered stimulus onset delays (SODP = 0, 50, and 100 ms). There is no panel for the 0-ms SODV condition for saccade start because this point in time defined saccade start. Data for the 50-ms SODV condition are not shown because this condition was used for only a few cells. Data for the 500-ms SODP condition are not shown because doing so would require a much compressed scale and for such long delays, small variations in timing are inconsequential.
View larger version (61K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 8.
Typical response of a lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) neuron (an ON-center Y cell in layer A1) averaged across all flash SODs employed. Flash timing is indicated ( ). There is a large primary response with a secondary peak on its falling phase, followed by a suppression and a rebound. Response is smoothed using adaptive kernel estimation, as described in METHODS. Dashed line illustrates how response latency was determined: time from flash onset for the primary response to reach half-maximum amplitude, relative to the preflash level of activity.
View larger version (13K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 9.
Effects of saccades on latency of visually evoked response for 68 cells (1 cell was not included because the onset of its response was too sluggish and irregular for a meaningful response latency to be assigned). Left: latencies for stimuli delivered during saccades; right: latencies for stimuli delivered after saccade end. These are the saccades contributing data to the left and right of the vertical dashed line in the right panel of Fig. 7, respectively. Abscissa gives the baseline latency for stationary eye position (SODP = 500 ms). , X cells;
, Y cells. Mean number of trials contributing to baseline latencies, latencies during saccades, and latencies immediately after saccades were 35.8 ± 17.7, 58.0 ± 17.9, and 60.0 ± 34.8 (SD) ms, respectively.
View larger version (26K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 10.
Time course of fractional changes in activity, aligned on saccade start (top) and end (bottom), relative to postsaccadic baseline activity. Curves are for activity during saccades in the dark: continuous (broken) curves are for saccades with peak velocity above (below) the median. These are similar, but not identical to curves in Fig. 6; those curves show averaged absolute activity. For the visual-stimulation condition, symbols indicate fractional changes in activity during five consecutive 100-ms intervals starting from flash onset, averaged across all 69 cells, and grouped into 50-ms bins relative to saccade start or end. Fractional changes associated with flashes of different SOD are not explicitly distinguished. Criteria for minimum data in a particular 50-ms bin (see METHODS) resulted in 20 bins (top) and 25 (bottom), each based on 963-2,456 saccades. Error brackets indicate standard error of the mean. None of the fractional changes for any 100-ms flash interval (individual symbols) differed significantly from the mean fractional change for all 100-ms flash intervals falling in the same 50-ms bin.
View larger version (47K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 11.
Time courses of fractional changes in activity, aligned on saccade start (top) and end (bottom) for each of 3 ranges of saccade amplitude (left), peak velocity (middle), and duration (right). Curves are for activity during saccades in the dark; symbols are for activity during saccades to visual targets. For each panel, saccades are equally apportioned into 3 groups: the thirds with the lowest, middle, and highest amplitudes, peak velocities, and durations. Keys between top and bottom panels give the mean amplitude, peak velocity, and duration of each group (numbers in parentheses are standard deviations). Conventions are the same as in Fig. 10, except that the number of symbols used is reduced to a minimum by no longer distinguishing among the 5 100-ms intervals into which each flash is subdivided.
. On average, a burst occurred only once every 5.7 saccades (including the entire perisaccade epoch). There were no statistically significant variations in burst rate with respect to saccade timing. During flashes delivered in the visual-stimulation condition, the mean number of spikes per burst was higher than in the dark, averaging 3.3 spikes, but bursts were even rarer, occurring exclusively during the first 100 ms after flash onset. There were 0.021 bursts/flash for the baseline flash (SODP = 500 ms), 0.031 bursts/flash for flashes delivered during saccades, and 0.021 bursts/flash for flashes presented within 250 ms after saccade end. The increase during saccades was statistically significant, but even for these flashes there was only a single burst for every 32.3 trials. There were no significant differences between X and Y cells for any of the analyses of burst frequency in either the dark or visual-stimulation conditions.
DISCUSSION
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
; Noda 1975
). The relatively weak influences of saccades in the dark on the monkey LGN (Bartlett et al. 1976
; Büttner and Fuchs 1973
) may reflect true species differences, but it is also possible that extensive averaging would reveal saccade-related modulations of activity similar to those in the cat.
for rapid, passive rotations of the eye in anesthetized cats. Their facilitation was abolished by retrobulbar blocks, implying a proprioceptive source. For the range of visual-field eccentricities corresponding to our sample, it peaked 70-170 ms after the onset of ramp-like, 15° movements lasting 75 ms (they reported suppressive effects only for receptive fields with eccentricity >15°). This is somewhat earlier than the peak we observed, but this is to be expected because they employed a 200°/s velocity ramp. With a ramp, high velocity is reached immediately, whereas with natural eye movements, peak velocity is not reached until well into a saccade.
; Steriade and Llinás 1988
) were too infrequent to account for a significant portion of the postsaccadic facilitation. In the dark, they contributed to <1% of the spikes of LGN cells. In the visual-stimulation condition, LGN cells fired, on average, 10.2 spikes in the first 100 ms of visual stimulation, and bursts contributed only ~0.1 spike to this activity. The conditions required for such bursts are the arrival of a depolarizing input after a relatively long period of hyperpolarization (Guido et al. 1992
; Steriade and Llinás 1988
), and we have no reason to believe that the conditions of the current experiment were conducive to the burst mode.
reported that the excitability of cat LGN cells was depressed after saccades. However, his results and ours are in no way contradictory: the two experiments address different phenomena, and by virtue of the design of each, the phenomenon revealed by one is largely factored out in the other. Noda (1975)
found that when cat LGN cells were driven to high rates of firing by saccades made across patterned stimuli, there was a substantial reduction in the frequency of postsynaptic responses evoked by single electrical shocks of the optic tract. Because moving the pattern at saccade velocities with the eye stationary produced comparable effects, he concluded that most of the reduction was not due to saccades per se but to high rates of firing induced by rapid stimulus motion on the retina however achieved. In the current experiment, the retinal input to the LGN was decoupled from the metrics of the saccade by comparing the postsynaptic responses of LGN cells receiving (presumably) identical retinal input at different times relative to saccade start or end. Noda's phenomenon, which is essentially a gain compression at high firing rates (which he interpreted in the context of visual masking), should have reduced the postsaccadic facilitation to the extent that firing rates were high enough to lead to significant gain compression. This may have been a factor at the highest firing rates evoked in the visual-stimulation condition. Although the difference is not statistically significant, the fractional change in activity at the peak of the facilitation was smaller for the first 100-ms epoch of the flash, which spanned the peak of the visually evoked response, than for subsequent epochs, for which firing rates were substantially less (Fig. 10). Obviously, in the extreme case where LGN cells were driven to the point of response saturation, the postsaccadic facilitation would disappear entirely.
) so the activation of facilitative signals to the LGN might lead to a slight shortening of response latency through temporal summation, and this was what we found. The shortening was more detectable for X cells than for Y cells (Fig. 9); this is consistent with the greater postsaccadic facilitation observed for X cells (Fig. 12). Fischer et al. (1996)
examined effects of saccades on response latency by comparing latencies during natural saccades across large, patterned, visual stimuli with those obtained during movement of the same stimuli at saccade velocity while the eye was stationary (they did not quantitatively examine effects of saccades on response magnitude). They also found that saccades shortened latency, but in other ways, their results differed substantially from ours: the latency shifts were several times larger (on the order of 10 ms and >30 ms for some individual cells), they were larger for Y cells than for X cells (on average, ~7 and 15 ms for X and Y cells, respectively), and they occurred during the saccade (where we observed response suppression and no change in response latency). However, the paradigms and method of analysis differ so greatly that these results cannot be directly compared. For example, Fischer et al. (1996)
used the first burst of activity to measure latency, and there is no particular reason why this should be related directly to the time for a visually evoked response to reach half-maximum amplitude.
; Buisseret and Maffei 1977
; Enomoto et al. 1983
) and a corollary discharge linked to saccades (Toyama et al. 1984
), either or both of which could provide the oculomotor component of an integrated response. To the extent that these signals play a role in the phenomena of the current study, it presumably would be in the postsaccadic facilitation of LGN activity: proprioceptive signals are available only after the movement begins, and the corollary discharge observed by Toyama et al. (1984)
peaked 10-70 ms after saccade end (their Figs. 5B, 9A, and 10A). The superior colliculus contains cells the activity of which is strongly time-locked to saccades (Peck et al. 1980
). Their discharges (depending on cell category) span the period from 200 ms before to 300 ms after saccade onset (Peck et al. 1980
), so collicular activity might play a role in either presaccadic or postsaccadic effects on LGN activity (see Lo 1988
; Lo and Xie 1987
for a possible role of the rabbit superior colliculus in saccadic suppression). Other potential sources of oculomotor inputs include the midbrain reticular formation (Singer and Bedworth 1974
), the frontal eye fields (Tsumoto and Suzuki 1976
), and the pretecto-geniculate pathway (Schmidt and Hoffmann 1992
).
reported that cat pretectal cells projecting to the LGN are excited during saccades, both for saccades made across visual patterns and for saccades in the dark. He proposed that this projection facilitates LGN responses through inhibition of inhibitory interneurons via an intrageniculate circuit identified anatomically by Cucchiaro et al. (1993)
. If pretectal activity is the source of LGN postsaccadic facilitation, the two should have similar time courses, with the latter lagging the former by an interval equal to the conduction time from the pretectum, plus a period sufficient for two synaptic delays (a couple of milliseconds) (see Fig. 2C in Schmidt 1996
) and associated integration times of unknown duration. Data on individual pretectal cells seem reasonably consistent with a pretectal source for the LGN postsaccadic facilitation. For example, in Fig. 5 of Schmidt (1996)
, the peak response of a pretectal cell occurred ~50 ms after saccade end for saccades in the dark. One impediment to considering the pretectum a likely candidate for the postsaccadic facilitation is the report by Funke and Eysel (1995)
in which they concluded that pretectal activity suppresses the excitability of LGN relay cells and therefore might contribute to saccadic suppression. However, they examined the effects of visually evoked pretectal signals on LGN activity in anesthetized animals, and it is not clear how this relates to saccade-induced pretectal activity.
, the threshold elevation associated with saccadic suppression is insufficient to prevent perception of much of the image for natural scenes. To illustrate this point, consider the demonstration of centrally mediated saccadic suppression of Burr et al. (1994)
. They had subjects make saccades parallel to sinusoidal gratings, a simple and elegant way of eliminating the confounding of image motion across the retina during saccades. They found an increase in threshold for the perception of gratings that was specific to isoluminant gratings of low spatial frequency. This increase, which they attributed to magnocellular LGN, raised the contrast threshold from ~1 to 10% for the maximally affected spatial frequency. A threshold elevation of a factor of 10 is certainly a large effect, but nevertheless, the contrast of natural images is commonly well above 10%. The fact is that LGN cells respond vigorously as images are swept acrossthe retina during natural saccades (Bartlett et al. 1996; Büttner and Fuchs 1973
; Noda 1975
). Were suppression at the LGN level required to prevent perceptual confusion during saccades, we continually would experience such confusion, but because we do not, other functions for centrally mediated saccadic suppression should be considered. We remind the reader that our study was designed to isolate oculomotor signals to the LGN, and as such, it purposely excluded visual masking effects that might more effectively suppress perception during saccades. Thus we do not argue against the importance of perceptual saccadic suppression. However, we see no reason to invoke it as an explanation for the saccade-related gain changes observed in the current experiment.
; Singer 1977
). Perhaps one should consider the presaccadic suppression and the postsaccadic facilitation to be a coordinated change in gain directed toward a single goal: maximizing temporal contrast between the pattern of activity near the end of one fixation and that which will need to be evaluated when the eye settles on the next location. The underlying assumption is that the impact on perception of a new pattern of cortical activation will be determined as much by the change and/or rate of change of activity as by the absolute level of activity. Looked at in this way, the overall gain change is substantial: an increase from the presaccadic to the postsaccadic period of ~30%. Larger facilitations might result in too many cells being driven into saturation. This proposal is consistent with the time course of LGN gain changes. If the primary function of the suppression is to prevent perceptual confusion, one would expect it to be in full force through most of the saccade. However, it begins well before the saccade, is maximum near the beginning of the saccade, begins to dissipate at saccade start, and is gone by saccade end (Fig. 10).
![]() |
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS |
---|
We are grateful to W. Busen, D. Cooper, J. Geistlinger, E. Kang, and V. Pavlovic for help in acquiring and analyzing these data and to W. Busen and P. Dassonville for valuable comments on the manuscript. We also thank M. Friedlander for suggesting that we analyze the relationship between saccade velocity and the time course of the postsaccadic facilitation.
This work was supported by National Eye Institute Grant R01 EY-02695.
![]() |
FOOTNOTES |
---|
Present address of D. Lee: Dept. of Neurobiology and Anatomy, Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, NC 27157.
Address for reprint requests: J. G. Malpeli, Dept. of Psychology, University of Illinois, 603 E. Daniel St., Champaign, IL 61820.
Received 4 March 1997; accepted in final form 8 October 1997.
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|