1Department of Neurobiology and Physiology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois 60208; 2Howard Hughes Medical Institute and Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York 10003; and 3Institute of Neuroinformatics, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology and University of Zurich, CH-8057 Zurich, Switzerland
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Anderson, Jeffrey S., Matteo Carandini, and David Ferster. Orientation Tuning of Input Conductance, Excitation, and Inhibition in Cat Primary Visual Cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 84: 909-926, 2000. The input conductance of cells in the cat primary visual cortex (V1) has been shown recently to grow substantially during visual stimulation. Because increasing conductance can have a divisive effect on the synaptic input, theoretical proposals have ascribed to it specific functions. According to the veto model, conductance increases would serve to sharpen orientation tuning by increasing most at off-optimal orientations. According to the normalization model, conductance increases would control the cell's gain, by being independent of stimulus orientation and by growing with stimulus contrast. We set out to test these proposals and to determine the visual properties and possible synaptic origin of the conductance increases. We recorded the membrane potential of cat V1 cells while injecting steady currents and presenting drifting grating patterns of varying contrast and orientation. Input conductance grew with stimulus contrast by 20-300%, generally more in simple cells (40-300%) than in complex cells (20-120%), and in simple cells was strongly modulated in time. Conductance was invariably maximal for stimuli of the preferred orientation. Thus conductance changes contribute to a gain control mechanism, but the strength of this gain control does not depend uniquely on contrast. By assuming that the conductance changes are entirely synaptic, we further derived the excitatory and inhibitory synaptic conductances underlying the visual responses. In simple cells, these conductances were often arranged in push-pull: excitation increased when inhibition decreased and vice versa. Excitation and inhibition had similar preferred orientations and did not appear to differ in tuning width, suggesting that the intracortical synaptic inputs to simple cells of cat V1 originate from cells with similar orientation tuning. This finding is at odds with models where orientation tuning in simple cells is achieved by inhibition at off-optimal orientations or sharpened by inhibition that is more broadly tuned than excitation.
![]() |
INTRODUCTION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
One of the properties that most profoundly affects the computation performed by a neuron is its input conductance. If the input conductance remains substantially constant, the neuron integrates its synaptic inputs linearly with excitation providing an additive term and inhibition providing a subtractive term. By contrast, increases in input conductance have a divisive effect on synaptic inputs and thus introduce a nonlinearity into the neuron's computation.
In the primary visual cortex (V1), the input conductance has been
invoked explicitly by two different models of cortical function. In
their veto model, Koch and Poggio (1985) proposed that a
cell's conductance should be highest for the orientations that are
most different from the cell's preferred orientation (Fig.
1A). Input conductance would
then play an important role in sharpening the orientation tuning of V1
cells. The contrast normalization model (Carandini et al. 1997
,
1999
; Heeger 1992
) posits that conductance should also grow
with visual stimulation, but in a manner that is independent of
stimulus orientation (Fig. 1B). In this view, the input
conductance would provide a gain-control mechanism that decreases the
responsivity of the cells at high stimulus contrast.
|
Large increases in input conductance (up to 300%) were demonstrated in
cortical neurons as long as 30 years ago (Dreifuss et al.
1969), both after electrical stimulation of the cortical surface and after iontophoretic application of GABA. Early measurements of input conductance during optimal visual stimulation, even with stimuli of optimal orientation, resulted in substantially lower estimates (Berman et al. 1991
; Carandini and
Ferster 1997
; Douglas et al. 1988
;
Ferster and Jagadeesh 1992
; Pei et al.
1991
). More recently, however, large visually evoked
conductance increases have been reported (Borg-Graham et al.
1998
; Hirsch et al. 1998
). We have therefore
recorded intracellularly from simple and complex cells in cat V1 while
stimulating with drifting gratings and made precise measurements of the
orientation tuning and contrast response of visually evoked changes in conductance.
We found, as did Borg-Graham et al. (1998) and
Hirsch et al. (1998)
, that input conductance grows by
20-300% with stimulus contrast and can show substantial temporal
modulation during the responses to optimal drifting gratings. Moreover
we found the input conductance of all cells to vary with the
orientation of the drifting gratings, with the maximal conductance
occurring in response to stimuli of the preferred orientation (Fig.
1C). Our results therefore run opposite to what would be
predicted by the veto theory. Our results are also at odds with a key
aspect of the contrast normalization model: while conductance increases could provide the cells with a divisive gain control mechanism, this
mechanism would be orientation selective and thus not depend purely on
visual contrast. Our results agree with conductance changes predicted
by a feedforward model of cat simple cell function that includes a
spatial push-pull arrangement of on and off excitation and inhibition
(Troyer et al. 1998
).
In addition to measuring the total input conductance, we have also
attempted to distinguish the components of the conductance changes
arising from excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs. This
distinction is based on the assumption that the changes in input
conductance are entirely synaptic. Using this approach, we studied the
orientation tuning of synaptic excitation and inhibition and found that
they were similar. This similarity would be predicted if intracortical
excitation and inhibition originated from cells with similar
orientation tuning and would explain previous reports that orientation
tuning is not sharpened by intracortical inhibition (Chung and
Ferster 1998; Ferster 1986
; Ferster et
al. 1996
; Nelson et al. 1994
).
Some of these results have been presented elsewhere in abstract form
(Carandini et al. 1998a-c
).
![]() |
METHODS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Experimental preparation
Details of the experimental preparation have been described
previously (Ferster and Jagadeesh 1992; Jagadeesh
et al. 1997
). Young adult cats were anesthetized with
intravenous thiopental sodium and placed in a stereotaxic headholder.
Either gallamine or pancuronium was given to minimize motion of the
eyes, and the animals were artificially respired. Phenylephrine
hydrochloride and atropine sulfate were applied to the eyes to retract
the nictitating membranes, dilate the pupils, and paralyze
accommodation. Contact lenses with artificial pupils (4 mm diam) were inserted.
Visual stimulation
Visual stimuli consisted of drifting sine-wave gratings displayed on a Tektronix 608 oscilloscope screen using a Picasso stimulus generator (Innisfree, Cambridge, MA). The peak contrast used was 64% and the mean luminance was 20 cd/m2. Stimuli lasted 4 s. The spatial frequency, drift rate, and spatial position of the gratings were optimized under computer control. An orientation of 0° corresponds to a vertical grating moving to the right; an orientation of 90° corresponds to a horizontal grating moving downward.
Input conductance was measured by recording the membrane potential
responses to visual stimuli while injecting, in turn, 3-11 different
steady currents, Iinj. Stimuli were
grouped in blocks of 12 different orientations at 64% contrast (or 7 different contrasts at preferred orientation for experiments measuring
contrast responses) together with one blank stimulus, all presented in
random order. Blocks were repeated with different random orders and
with different injected currents, which were held constant for an
entire stimulus block. The currents, ranging from 350 to 200 pA, were
most often negative, to minimize spiking and other effects of
voltage-dependent membrane nonlinearities.
Intracellular recording
Whole cell patch recordings were obtained from neurons of area
17 of the visual cortex using the technique developed for brain slices
by Blanton et al. (1989). Electrodes were filled with a K+-gluconate solution including
Ca2+ buffers, pH buffers, and cyclic nucleotides
as described by Spruston et al. (1995)
. Membrane
potentials, recorded with an Axoclamp amplifier (Axon Instruments,
Burlingame, CA) in current-clamp (bridge) mode, were low-pass filtered
and digitized at 4 kHz (visually evoked records) or 15 kHz
(electrically evoked records). To compensate for junction potentials
(Neher 1992
), an electrode was placed in a bath of
artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF), which was gradually replaced
with a solution that more resembled intracellular ionic concentrations.
The resulting 10-mV change in measured potential was subtracted from
all recordings. Electrical stimuli to the lateral geniculate nucleus
(LGN) consisted of 1.0-mA pulses, 200 ms in duration, electrode negative.
Cells were classified as simple or complex on the basis of the presence
or absence of ON and OFF subregions in the receptive field and by the
ratio of mean to modulation of the membrane potential response to
drifting gratings optimized for orientation, spatial frequency, and
drift frequency (Carandini and Ferster 2000;
Skottun et al. 1991
). Modulation is computed as twice
the amplitude of the response component at the frequency of the
drifting grating (1, 2, or 4 Hz).
Compensation for electrode resistance
The capacitance and resistance (4-15 M) of the electrodes
were easily neutralized before patching a cell. When a patch was obtained, however, the electrode resistance
Re increased dramatically. The
resistance of the electrode after the patch is formed is also known as
the access or series resistance, and its value must be properly
neutralized to measure input conductance accurately. To this end, we
employed a series of three methods, one qualitative, performed during
the recording, and the other two quantitative, performed off-line.
To measure electrode resistance by eye, 100-pA current pulses were
injected into the cell. In the standard method, the faster decay
component in the response, reflecting the electrode's time constant,
is then distinguished from the slower component, reflecting the cell's
time constant. The amplifier bridge is then balanced to neutralize the
electrode component for the purposes of recording. This method gives
only an approximate value for series resistance, however, given the
similarity of the time constants of the two components of in vivo patch recordings.
The first quantitative method for estimating the electrode resistance
was based on off-line fitting of the responses to current pulses of
several different amplitudes. We injected a series of current steps
periodically throughout the course of a recording, and we fitted them
with the predictions of a simple model. In the model, we considered the
patched electrode and the cell membrane to be simple resistor-capacitor
circuits arranged in series. The response V(t) of
such a model to a step of injected current
Iinj is given by a double exponential
![]() |
(1) |
This estimation is illustrated in Fig. 2,
A-D. The responses to each
current pulse (A) were divided by the injected current, and
the resulting responses (B) were fitted with Eq. 1. In every case a clear minimum in the error function was found.
An example is illustrated in C, which shows the error
corresponding to each pair of values
Re and
e, and to the corresponding best
estimate of
m. The estimated
contribution Iinj
Re of the electrode to the recorded potential was
then subtracted from the traces to yield the estimated true membrane
potential response (D). Because the main measurements in
this study were obtained with steady current injections, we did not
need to further compensate for the electrode time constant.
|
The reliability of these estimates was assessed in two ways. First, we
applied the fitting procedure to simulated data obtained from Eq. 1 with added noise. We found the estimated parameters to be
accurate to within 0.5% of the correct values. Second, we performed
each fitting procedure on 500 randomly selected subsets of the current
pulses from a single complete set (Efron and Tibshirani 1993). The resulting 95% confidence intervals for the
estimated parameters were between 2 and 10% for resistance values and
less than 1% for time constants.
The second quantitative method for estimating the electrode
resistance was based on the assumption that the threshold for action
potential generation Vthr should be
largely independent of the injected current
Iinj. This assumption is justified
because our injected current was constant (Koch 1999).
Any effect of injected current on the measured threshold should then be
ascribed to the electrode resistance.
The application of this technique is illustrated in Fig. 2, E-H. After Re had been neutralized according to the double exponential fitting technique, we compared spike thresholds, taken to be the minimum potential at which a spike was generated, at different Iinj (E-G). On the assumption that the actual Vthr was unchanged by current injection, any apparent change in Vthr would represent a resistive component in the patch electrode that had not been properly neutralized. Even though these potentials had been corrected for Re as calculated from the double exponential fitting procedure, the measured threshold varied with the injected current. A linear fit to the relationship between Vthr and Iinj yields Ra, the difference between the values of Re obtained from the exponential fitting and threshold estimation techniques (H).
The two independent methods used to estimate Re were in close agreement (within 15%) for all cells, close to the level of precision estimated for the two techniques. The values for the electrode and membrane resistance Re and Rm estimated from the exponential fitting technique, as well as the residual electrode resistance Ra obtained from the threshold estimation technique are reported in Table 1 for all cells tested with visual stimuli. Final values of Re were taken to be the mean of the values obtained from the two techniques. Changes in Re were tracked over the course of a recording. Values were linearly interpolated over time between measurements and applied to individual records of visually evoked responses. Drifts in Re were always less than 10% in amplitude.
|
Tuning curves
On various occasions we consider the orientation tuning curves
of various measures of cell responses, such as membrane potential or
input conductance. The amplitudes of these responses as a function of
orientation were fit with a simple function consisting of a sum of two
Gaussians. The two Gaussians are forced to peak 180° apart, and to
have the same width :
![]() |
(2) |
Bootstrap estimates for conductance
To assess the reliability of conductance measurements, we used
the bootstrap method (Efron and Tibshirani 1993). We
randomly selected 1,000 subsets (with repetition allowed) of the traces used to calculate input conductance. Each subset contained the same
number of elements as the total number of traces used to calculate
input conductance. Subsets were required to contain traces representing
at least three different levels of injected current. For each subset,
the data at each point in time were fit to a line (Eq. 3) to
obtain values of g(t). To construct 95% confidence intervals, the 2.5 and 97.5 percentiles were selected from
the 1,000 values for g(t) at each point in time.
![]() |
RESULTS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
We report here on data from 24 cells selected from a total of 49 that were recorded with multiple injected currents. These cells met or
exceeded the following minimal requirements: that their visual response
properties were well characterized (orientation, spatial frequency, and
temporal frequency preferences); that they had action potentials
greater than 10 mV in amplitude; (spikes are often of smaller amplitude
for in vivo whole cell recordings than in sharp microelectrode
recordings due to filtering by higher electrode resistances); that
their access resistance was less than 200 M; and that we were able
to record the responses to a complete set of visual or electrical
stimuli for at least three different injected currents (including the
0-current condition). Fourteen of the 23 cells (9 simple, 5 complex)
were tested with current injection paired with visual stimulation. A
summary of the properties of these cells is given in Table 1. Eleven of the 24 cells (3 simple, 8 complex) were tested with current injection paired with electrical stimulation in the LGN. One cell was
tested with both visual and electrical stimulation.
Visually evoked responses of one simple cell are illustrated in Fig.
3. The visual stimulus was a drifting
sinusoidal grating optimized for orientation, spatial frequency and
drift rate. These responses are in line with previous observations
(Jagadeesh et al. 1993): the 1% contrast grating
(top) did not evoke any noticeable response, whereas the
64% contrast grating (bottom) evoked approximately sinusoidal membrane potential modulations, with action potentials rising from the peaks. These modulations had the same frequency as the
drift rate of the stimulus, and resulted in a spike train that was
strongly modulated at the same frequency. Injecting negative (
300 pA,
left) or positive (140 pA, right) steady currents
during the stimulus presentation had the predictable effect of
hyperpolarizing or depolarizing the membrane. The current-induced
changes in membrane potential were greater during the low-contrast
stimulus (top) than during the high-contrast stimulus
(bottom). This difference indicates a decrease in
conductance at high contrasts. Differences like these form the basis
for quantitative measurements of visually evoked conductance changes.
|
Measurement of input conductance
The effect of current injection on the membrane potential is more
evident in Fig. 4B, where
responses were averaged cycle by cycle and low-pass filtered. The thick
traces show the membrane potential responses in the absence of current
injection and the thin traces above and below them indicate the
responses in the presence of positive and negative injected current. As
in the individual traces, the vertical distance between the averaged traces is larger for the low-contrast stimulus than it is for the
high-contrast stimulus. To quantify this effect, at each time in the
cycle the relation between the injected current and the membrane
potential was fitted with a line
![]() |
(3) |
|
To assess the time course of changes in input conductance, we calculated a fit to Eq. 3 at each time, t, in the stimulus cycle. An estimate of the quality of these fits can be obtained by constructing a series of linear predictions for the potential in the presence of injected currents. That is, for each injected current and at each moment in time, we predicted the membrane potential from the corresponding fit to Eq. 3. The quality of these predictions can be judged in Fig. 4B by comparing the dotted curves with the continuous curves. The fits exhibit some systematic errors but are in general satisfactory, indicating that in this range of potentials and signal frequencies the membrane is acting approximately linearly.
It thus becomes possible to model the behavior of the cell with a single value of conductance at each point in time, as illustrated in Fig. 4, C and D. Using this method, we see that for the simple cell in Fig. 4, the input conductance rose from around 20 nS for very low contrast stimuli (1%) to more than 80 nS for high contrast stimuli (64%). This corresponds to a 330% conductance increase. For some stimulus contrasts, moreover, the increase in conductance could be strongly modulated at the drift frequency of the stimulus. For example, the conductance increases elicited by the 64% contrast grating were largely confined to an interval immediately following the peak of the membrane potential responses. The 17% contrast grating, instead, elicited increases in conductance that were smaller but largely constant over the stimulus presentation time. At no time and for no stimulus contrast was the conductance ever smaller than at the resting condition.
We will see that all the other simple cells in our sample displayed similarly large, modulated conductance increases, in the range of 40-300%, whereas complex cells showed smaller (20-100%) conductance increases that were generally constant in time.
Reliability of the measurements
The value of these observations on the input conductance depends, of course, on the reliability of our measurements. To measure this reliability we computed 95% confidence intervals for the conductance, obtained from bootstrap estimates of the slope of the lines relating Iinj and V, such as those in D. These confidence intervals are indicated by the gray areas around the conductance traces in C and around the fitted lines in D. The gray areas are quite small even in the face of substantial spread in the membrane potential distributions, and indicate that we can trust our estimation of the input conductance.
Another important issue is whether the linear model described in
Eq. 3 provided satisfactory fits in the tested range of
membrane potentials. As described in the preceding text, the quality of the fits can be observed directly in Fig. 4B, where the
membrane potentials predicted by the model are compared with the actual recorded membrane potentials. It can also be estimated statistically by
comparing the actual membrane potential to the predicted membrane potential for the entire data set obtained from this cell. The density
plot in Fig. 5 summarizes 9,408 data
points, as in this data set there were 12 samples during each cycle
(after low-pass filtering), 16 cycles per stimulus, seven stimulus
contrasts, and seven presentations of the stimulus set (3 with
Iinj = 300 pA, 2 with
Iinj = 0 pA and 2 with
Iinj = 140 pA). The horizontal streaks result from the trial-to-trial variability in the actual responses for a given stimulus condition. Overall, the linear model
(Eq. 3) accounted for 85% of the variance in this data set. A comparable degree of accuracy was common in our sample (77 ± 44% of the variance; mean ± SE).
|
An accurate prediction of membrane potential responses by Eq. 3 requires that the omitted capacitative term, C · dV/dt, be small. This term is required for a mathematically exact description of the changes in membrane potential. In the case of visually evoked responses, however, dV/dt is negligible (1,000 times smaller than it is during action potentials, for example). We found that including the C · dV/dt term changed the predicted membrane potentials by less than 2%.
Considering the nonlinearities that exist in cortical cells, such as
voltage-dependent channels (Koch 1999), this performance may be considered surprising. Part of the explanation may lie in our
having chosen mostly small, negative injected currents, a choice that
was made expressly to minimize the activation of voltage dependent
conductances at depolarized potentials. Indeed, while Fig. 5 shows that
the model did not perform worse at high values of the membrane
potential than at low values, it may be that forcing the potential much
above threshold by using larger positive injected currents would have
revealed substantial nonlinearities.
Contrast dependence of conductance changes
Having established a method for estimating the conductance at each
point in time during a stimulus, we can now measure the dependence of
visually evoked changes in conductance on stimulus parameters such as
orientation and contrast. The dependence of input conductance on
contrast for the cell in Fig. 4, as well as for four other simple
cells, is illustrated in Fig. 6. To
quantify the changes in input conductance observed during visual
stimulation, we have focused on two measures. The first is the
mean conductance, and the second is the
modulation of the conductance, which is taken to be twice
the amplitude of the Fourier component of the response at the frequency
of the drifting grating. Thus for a sinusoidal response, modulation
represents the peak-to-peak amplitude of the response. These two
measures are commonly used to characterize the firing rate responses
(e.g., Movshon et al. 1978) and the membrane potential
responses (Carandini and Ferster 1997
, 2000
) of simple
cells to drifting gratings. Both mean and modulation of the input
conductance are shown in Fig. 6C, and both of them increase
in all cells as a function of stimulus contrast.
|
These increases in mean and modulation of the input conductance are similar but not identical to concurrent increases in the firing rate and membrane potential responses (Fig. 6, A and B). For example, in the cell from Fig. 4 (Fig. 6, 4th column), contrasts as high as 17% elicited substantial firing rate and membrane potential responses, but did not elicit a significant conductance increase. The two highest contrasts, 34 and 64%, elicited very similar membrane potential (and firing rate) responses but different conductance increases.
Complex cells also showed increases in conductance with visual
stimulation; these increases, however, were a third to a half the size
of conductance increases seen in simple cells (See Table 1 for
comparison of maximal conductance increases). The increase in
conductance for a complex cell is illustrated for different stimulus
contrasts in Fig. 7. As with the simple
cell in Fig. 4, conductance grew with visual stimulation, particularly
once the contrast reached 17%. Like the membrane potential
(Carandini and Ferster 2000), the average input
conductance of the complex cell was essentially constant over the
stimulus cycle. The maximal conductance increase was 64% from a
baseline of 12.1 nS to a peak of 19.8 nS at 64% contrast.
|
Orientation tuning of the conductance changes
Up to now we have considered only the conductance changes evoked by gratings of the preferred orientation and direction of motion. We have seen that input conductance grows with stimulus contrast, but we have not determined whether it depends on other stimulus attributes. Now we report on our main result, that conductance changes are tuned for stimulus orientation: stimuli having a cell's preferred orientation, as defined by changes in membrane potential, elicit maximal conductance increases.
Examples of our results with stimuli of different orientations are illustrated for a simple cell in Fig. 8. The firing rate responses (A) and membrane potential responses (B) of the cell were tuned for orientations of approximately 120 and 300°, which correspond to identical orientations but opposite directions of motion. The firing rate responses of the cell exhibited clear direction selectivity, being much stronger for stimuli drifting at 120° than for stimuli drifting at 300°. A glance at the cycle averages of the input conductance for each orientation (C) reveals the following two observations. First, at all orientations the conductance was higher than at rest (far left and · · · in all panels). Second, the conductance increases were tuned for orientation and were strongest at the same orientations at which the firing rate and membrane potential responses were maximal.
|
Results from another simple cell are illustrated in Fig. 9. This cell was not particularly selective for stimulus direction and was tuned for orientations around 75°. The input conductance was similarly tuned and was substantially modulated over time. The timing of this modulation in input conductance was opposite to that of the firing rate and membrane potential responses: The maximal conductance consistently occurred during the trough of these responses. Other simple cells in our population showed similar behavior in that the maximum conductance was not simultaneous with the peaks in potential or in firing rate. Thus the conductance increases that we observe are unlikely to be related to the cell's approaching or reaching the threshold for action potential generation.
|
More generally, there was no simple relation between the input conductance of a simple cell and its membrane potential. For the cells in Figs. 4 and 9, the conductance consistently peaked at very different times than the membrane potential response. For the cell in Fig. 8, responses at preferred and nonpreferred orientations had similar mean values for membrane potential but nonoverlapping ranges for input conductance. In some simple cells, the membrane potential obtained with blank stimuli and with gratings drifting at nonpreferred orientations were quite similar, whereas conductance was significantly lower for recordings during blank stimuli. These considerations suggest that the observed changes in input conductance were not simply caused by intrinsic voltage-dependent channels in the membrane.
The orientation tuning of input conductance in simple cells was similar
to that of the membrane potential. This similarity can be observed for
seven simple cells in Fig. 10, where
the tuning curves for conductance (C and F) are
shown together with those for the firing rate (A and
D) and membrane potential (B and E). Two components of the conductance changes are shown, the mean value
(C and F: ) and the size of the modulation in
time (C and F:
).
|
The tuning curves for the cell in Fig. 8 (cell 4) are among
those illustrated in Fig. 10. In this cell the presence of a drifting grating elevated the mean input conductance () from a resting value
of 10 nS to average values of about 16 nS for nonpreferred orientations
and 29 nS for preferred orientations. Input conductance modulation
(
) varied more than 10-fold with orientation, from around 0.4 to 10 nS peak to peak. The orientation tuning of the input conductance was
very similar to that of the membrane potential. In contrast, the
orientation tuning of the spike rate was significantly narrower
(Carandini and Ferster 2000
).
Analogous observations can be made for the cell in Fig. 9, whose orientation tuning is also illustrated in Fig. 10 (cell 6): in this cell the stimulus-driven increase in mean input conductance was not tuned, but the modulation in input conductance was clearly tuned. As with the previous cell, the modulation of input conductance peaked at the cell's preferred orientation.
Similar results apply to all remaining cells in Fig. 10: one or both
components of the input conductancemean and modulation
were well
tuned for orientation. In cells 1 and 6, the mean
component was not tuned for stimulus orientation, but the modulation
component was strong and well tuned. In cells 2 and
7, the situation was reversed, where the modulation
component was almost negligible, but the mean component was strong and
well tuned. In the remaining cells, both the mean and the modulation of
the input conductance were well tuned for orientation.
The input conductance increases were also tuned for orientation in complex cells. As with simple cells, complex cells exhibited maximal conductance increases at the preferred orientation. In the cell in Fig. 11, for example, conductance increases of up to 60% were observed around the preferred orientation, which was between 270 and 300°. For the remaining orientations, conductance values were generally at or around baseline. Exceptions were common, however, such as occurs for this cell at 30°, which is roughly orthogonal to the preferred orientation. Whether or not these isolated increases in conductance at nonpreferred orientations are significant is somewhat difficult to determine because in complex cells the membrane potential traces and the associated conductance values were much more variable than in simple cells. The confidence limits in Fig. 11 are correspondingly larger in absolute terms than they are in Figs. 8 and 9, and the confidence limits for the trace at 30° nearly overlap with those at adjacent orientations. Figure 12 shows tuning curves of conductance, membrane potential and spike rate for this cell (2nd column) and for three additional complex cells.
|
|
To compare the orientation tuning of membrane potential and input
conductance, we fitted the tuning curves in Fig. 10 with the
descriptive function of Eq. 2 (METHODS). This
function is the sum of two Gaussians that can have different heights
but are constrained to peak 180° apart and to have identical widths.
To minimize the number of tuning parameters, some constraints were imposed on the fits. First, the mean and modulation of the input conductance were fitted together, forcing them to peak at the same
orientations and have the same tuning width. Second, the mean and
modulation of the membrane potential were similarly fitted together.
Third, the fits of the firing rate responses were obtained by requiring
that they peak at the same orientation as the membrane potential
responses. These requirements simplify the following analysis without
significantly reducing the quality of the fits. Inspection of Fig.
10 reveals that the fits () closely approximate the data for simple
cells. By contrast, the data from complex cells (not shown) were more
noisy, and the fits were less satisfactory.
Preferred orientation and tuning widths derived from the tuning curves
of Fig. 10 are compared in Fig. 13. The
preferred orientation for the input conductance (A) was
always close to the preferred orientation for the potential: the two
differed in 7 of the 11 cells by less than 10°, and in all cells by
less than 30°. Similarly, the tuning widths of the conductance
increases were not significantly different from those of the membrane
potential responses (B). For simple cells, the half-width at
half-height of the functions fitted to the firing rate, membrane
potential, and input conductance were 16.9 ± 2.6°, 28.8 ± 4.2°, and 25° ± 2.2° (n = 7). For complex cells,
tuning width for firing rate, mean potential, and mean input
conductance were 14.8° ± 4.1°, 18.6 ± 3.8°, 17.8 ± 4.4° (n = 4). The narrower tuning width for firing
rate can be easily explained by the presence of the spike threshold
(Carandini and Ferster 2000; Chung and Ferster
1998
).
|
Distinguishing excitation and inhibition
We have seen that visual stimulation can substantially increase the conductance of cortical neurons and that these conductance increases are not explained by membrane nonlinearities such as those involved in the generation of action potentials. What is the source of these conductance increases? The simplest explanation is that they are largely a consequence of increased synaptic activity. If this explanation were correct, it would be useful to determine how much of this synaptic conductance was excitatory and how much inhibitory.
On this assumption, we have attempted to derive from our data the time
course and tuning of the excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs. We
consider each neuron to have three conductances: an excitatory synaptic
conductance ge(t), an inhibitory
synaptic conductance gi(t), and a
constant resting conductance grest.
The sum of the three conductances is the total conductance
![]() |
(4) |
Given Eq. 4, the visually driven membrane potential depends
on the conductances as follows
![]() |
(5) |
We find the excitatory and inhibitory synaptic conductances,
ge(t) and
gi(t), by solving Eqs. 4 and 5. We set Ve = 0 mV and Vi = 85 mV. The latter is
intermediate between the equilibrium potentials of
GABAA and GABAB inhibitory
channels. The remaining parameters in Eqs. 4 and 5 are known from the analysis described in the first part of
the paper: Vrest is obtained from the
membrane potential recordings without injected current, and
grest, g(t), and
Vvisual(t) are obtained by fitting
the plot of membrane potential versus injected current with a line as
described in Eq. 3. Equations 4 and 5 are thus a
system of two equations in two unknowns,
ge(t) and
gi(t), which can be solved at each
point in time with simple linear methods.
As a validation of our methods, we first attempted to distinguish excitatory and inhibitory conductance changes evoked not by visual stimuli, but by electrical stimulation of the LGN because the conductance changes evoked by electrical stimuli are relatively well understood. An example of our results, obtained from a complex cell, is illustrated in Fig. 14. For this analysis, 10-100 responses to LGN shocks were averaged for each of 21 different injected currents. Figure 14, left, shows baseline recordings (without stimulation) and derived conductances. Figure 14, middle and right, shows the responses at two different time scales.
|
The excitatory and inhibitory conductance changes shown in C
and D conform well to what one would expect from the
response to electrical stimulation (e.g., Douglas et al.
1991a; Dreifuss et al. 1969
; Pei et al.
1991
). The excitatory conductance began rising 3-4 ms after
the shock, reached its peak by 11 ms, decayed to its baseline value by
25 ms, and dropped below the baseline value for about 300 ms.
Inhibitory conductance began to rise about 2-5 ms following the
initial increase of the excitatory conductance, peaked at about 18-20
ms, and showed a very slow withdrawal, lasting about 300 ms, before
returning to baseline values. The decay of the inhibitory conductance
appeared to show two components, a fast component (left arrow) and a
slow component (right arrow), which may represent
GABAA and GABAB components
of the inhibitory response (Douglas et al. 1991a
). Note
that the long-lasting drop below baseline of the excitatory conductance
occurs in parallel with the rise in inhibition. We attribute this to
the concomitant decrease in activity in neighboring cortical cells,
many of which presumably excite the recorded cell.
In the cell of Fig. 14, the total conductance rose by about 100% of its resting value, a change that is on the moderately high end of the spectrum of conductance increases we observed in responses to both visual and electrical stimulation in complex cells. The magnitude of conductance changes observed in response to electrical stimulation and the timing of their excitatory and inhibitory components are summarized in Table 2 for the eight complex cells and three simple cells studied.
|
Push-pull arrangement of excitation and inhibition
Having tested its validity with electrical stimuli, we then
applied the decomposition method to the visually evoked excitatory and
inhibitory conductances of a simple cell (Fig.
15). Figure 15, left, shows
responses to blank stimuli, right shows responses to a
drifting grating. This cell exhibits a property common to all the
simple cells we analyzed: that is, excitatory and inhibitory conductances are modulated in counterphase, such that whenever one
increases, the other decreases, and vice versa (Douglas et al.
1991b; Ferster 1988
; Ferster and
Jagadeesh 1992
; Heggelund 1981
; Palmer
and Davis 1981
). In D and E, the
excitatory and inhibitory conductances induced by the stimulus show an
approximately 180° difference in temporal phase. This difference has
the effect of producing a very small modulation in total conductance
(C) compared with the large modulation in membrane potential
(B).
|
Examples of this push-pull arrangement of excitation and inhibition can be observed in the responses of two additional simple cells, displayed in Figs. 16 and 17. Similar to the cell in Fig. 15, these cells showed a push-pull organization in excitation and inhibition for orientations near the preferred. The excitatory and inhibitory conductances, however, were not always in exact counterphase. In the cell of Fig. 16, for example, excitatory and inhibitory components of conductance were in counterphase at an orientation of 270°, but at the preferred orientation (90°) they were only 60° out of phase. To compare the difference in phase between excitatory and inhibitory components of conductance, we measured this difference at each orientation for which modulation over time was significant. By averaging these measurements from all of our simple cells, we found the excitatory and inhibitory components were out of phase by an average of 153 ± 14° (n = 7).
|
|
It should be noted that the excitatory components of conductance do not
descend below baseline levels, which supports the view that
intracortical inhibition is the dominant or perhaps sole mechanism for
generating troughs in membrane potential fluctuations, while withdrawal
of baseline thalamic drive plays little or no role (Hirsch et
al. 1998).
Orientation tuning of excitation and inhibition
Using measurements like those shown in Fig. 16, D and E, one can study the orientation tuning of synaptic excitation and inhibition. In the cell in that figure, both excitatory and inhibitory conductances increased at or near the preferred orientation and were modulated over time as was total conductance. The magnitude of the increase in inhibitory conductance, however, was greater than for excitatory conductance. Therefore the modulation in total conductance was most similar to the modulation in inhibitory conductance, with the peak conductance coinciding with the trough in membrane potential.
Similar measurements for the seven simple cells in our sample are shown
in Fig. 18, which shows tuning curves
for synaptic excitation and inhibition. The cell in Fig. 16 is
illustrated in the bottom right column. Excitatory conductances
(A and C) were generally lower in magnitude than
inhibitory conductances (B and D). With the
exception of cell 2, where the inhibition was mostly tonic,
both excitation and inhibition tended to be modulated in time. In the
majority of cases, visual stimulation increased the synaptic
conductances above baseline. Modulations in synaptic conductances thus
were accompanied by increases in mean conductance. As a result, with
the exception of cell 6, the mean component for both
excitation and inhibition () was tuned just like the modulation
component (
). Excitation and inhibition were both tuned for
orientation but could in some cells be affected by stimuli of all
orientations. Only in cells 2 and 5 did stimuli
orthogonal to the preferred elicit no increases in excitatory or
inhibitory conductance. In the other cells, the tuning curves for the
conductances were generally elevated with respect to the values at
rest.
|
The orientation tuning of the excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs appears to be very similar. In turn, this orientation tuning is rather similar to that of the membrane potential and of the total conductance (Fig. 10). To compare the preferred orientation and tuning width of excitation and inhibition, we fitted the descriptive tuning curve (Eq. 2) to the mean and modulation of the excitatory and inhibitory conductances. We then compared the parameters of the function with those obtained from fitting the firing rate, the membrane potential, and the input conductance. Comparisons between tuning widths for excitatory conductance, inhibitory conductance, and membrane potential are shown in Fig. 19 for the seven simple cells studied. The tuning widths of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic conductances were not systematically different (A), with a mean difference of only 0.5 ± 1.8° (n = 7). In addition, the tuning widths of the synaptic conductances were in the same range as those of the membrane potential (B): the tuning width of the excitatory synaptic conductance differed from that of the membrane potential by only 3.3 ± 6.4° (n = 7) in simple cells. The tuning width of the inhibitory synaptic conductance differed from that of membrane potential (C) by only 3.2 ± 3.5°.
|
The close agreement in tuning widths of excitatory and inhibitory
conductances that we have measured in Fig. 19 extends previous observations that excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs and IPSPs) have similar orientation preference (Ferster 1986). Although the spacing between orientations in our stimuli was rather coarse (30° for 5 cells, 15° for 1 cell, 10° for 1 cell), tuning widths for all relevant parameters were sufficiently large (i.e., half-width greater than 17°) to adequately constrain a
Gaussian fit at our sampling resolution. Our measurements therefore should have been able to detect a systematic difference in tuning width
or preferred orientation between excitation and inhibition. The close
agreement between excitation and inhibition suggests that intracortical
excitation and inhibition originate from cells that are similarly tuned
and could explain previous results (Chung and Ferster
1998
; Ferster et al. 1996
; Nelson et al.
1994
) that the orientation tuning of cat V1 simple cells is not
significantly sharpened by synaptic inhibition.
![]() |
DISCUSSION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
In agreement with Borg-Graham et at. (1998) and
Hirsch et al. (1998)
, we have found that the input
conductance of cells in primary visual cortex increases significantly
(20-300%) during the presentation of drifting gratings at the
preferred orientation. In addition, we have found that these increases
in input conductance are well tuned for stimulus orientation and that
their preferred orientation and tuning widths are nearly identical to
those of the membrane potential responses.
These results argue against both of the models depicted in Fig. 1. The
results argue against the veto model (A) because it predicts
that a cell's conductance should be highest for the orientations that
are most different from the preferred orientation (Koch and Poggio 1985). The results argue against the normalization model (B) because it predicts that conductance should be
independent of stimulus orientation (Carandini and Heeger
1994
; Carandini et al. 1997
, 1999
). The
increases in input conductance that we measured do constitute a
gain-control mechanism that decreases the responsivity of the cells at
high stimulus contrast, but this mechanism is orientation selective and
thus does not depend purely on visual contrast.
Measuring input conductance in vivo
While our data agree with recent measurements of visually evoked
changes in conductance (Borg-Graham et al. 1998;
Hirsch et al. 1998
), earlier measurements of input
conductance during visual stimulation, even with stimuli of optimal
orientation, resulted in lower values. Guided by the large conductance
changes observed by Borg-Graham et al. (1998)
and
Hirsch et al. (1998)
, we have examined the factors that
may have resulted in lower estimates in previous studies. These
estimates ranged from a lack of measurable increase (Carandini
and Ferster 1997
; Douglas et al. 1988
;
Pei et al. 1991
), to 16% (Berman et al.
1991
) or 25% (Ferster and Jagadeesh 1992
). Most
likely, the reasons for the divergence in estimates lie in the
differences in methods for visual stimulation and more importantly in
differences in estimating the series resistance of the electrodes.
Indeed, the most critical steps in measuring the input resistance from single-electrode, whole cell recordings in vivo is the correct estimation of the electrode's access, or series, resistance. The change in potential evoked by an injected current pulse, which is used to measure input resistance, is equal not to the injected current multiplied by the cell's input resistance but to the current times the sum of the input and electrode resistances. With in vitro recordings, the components of the response to a current pulse belonging to the electrode and to the cell are easy to distinguish. The electrode and cell time constants are so different that the electrode capacitance has finished charging by the time the membrane capacitance has just begun to do so. It is therefore a simple matter to "balance the bridge" by eye, subtracting out the electrode component and thereby reaching an accurate estimate of the cell's input resistance.
The situation in vivo is different. The time constant of the electrode
is larger in vivo than in vitro. First, the capacitance of the
electrode is higher because its shank is embedded in several millimeters of tissue and agar. Second, the resistance of the electrode
is increased as its tip passes through many hundreds of micrometers of
tissue. The time constant of the electrode therefore approaches that of
the cell, making it difficult to distinguish the electrode and cell
components of the response to a current pulse, thereby requiring more
quantitative methods for "balancing the bridge." This requirement
is most likely the source of the error in our own previous study of
conductance (Carandini and Ferster 1997) where we used
only the on-line bridge-balance technique for compensating for
electrode series resistance and arrived at the conclusion that visual
stimulation has little effect on conductance. Without using the more
quantitative methods used here, we likely did not properly correct for
the electrode resistance.
In this paper, we have adopted two independent methods that together allow precise measurements of the electrode resistance. The methods also provide correction for drifts in electrode resistance over time. They achieve a precision of better than 10%, over which range none of the conclusions of the study are significantly affected. This uncertainty in electrode resistance, and the corresponding uncertainty in membrane resistance, may yield an additional increase in the size of confidence intervals for measurements of input conductance.
One assumption that enters into our measurement of conductance is that
the current-voltage relationship in the recorded neurons is linear. The
I-V curves that we measured tend to bear this assumption out, which is somewhat surprising given the large number of
voltage-sensitive conductances present in neurons. We have used
hyperpolarizing currents for most of our tests to minimize the
involvement of active currents. Nevertheless even when the cells were
spiking, the I-V relationship was approximately linear,
suggesting that the active currents underlying the spike are
concentrated in the initial segment or even the first node of Ranvier
(Colbert and Johnston 1996), away from the soma. That
simple cells, at least, behave relatively linearly is borne out by
their responses to visual stimulation, which are in some aspects quite
linear (Carandini and Ferster 2000
; Jagadeesh et
al. 1993
, 1997
).
Distinguishing excitation and inhibition
In addition to measuring total input conductance, we have employed
a simple method for deriving the excitatory and inhibitory components
of conductance changes underlying visual responses based on the
assumption that such changes are synaptic in origin. In this respect,
one simplification in our model is our use of a single type of synaptic
inhibition. Cortical cells have both GABAA and
GABAB receptors, which are permeable to
K+ and Cl ions. Normally
these channels would have equilibrium potentials near
65 and
100
mV. But the solution in our electrodes, which contains little chloride,
would hyperpolarize the GABAA equilibrium potential. We have therefore chosen the equilibrium potential for our
inhibitory conductance (Vi) to be
85
mV, somewhere between GABAA and
GABAB. The equilibrium potential for the
excitatory conductance (Ve) was set to
0 mV, close to the equilibrium potential of glutamatergic synapses. We
have repeated all of the analyses of excitatory and inhibitory
components of conductance, varying the values of
Ve and
Vi by 10 mV in either direction. These
changes did not significantly affect any of the basic conclusions of
the study, either for electrically or visually evoked responses, and yielded distributions of excitation and inhibition that were within 5%
of each other.
One potential limitation of our technique for deriving excitatory and
inhibitory conductances arises from the possibility that some synaptic
inputs are located in electrotonically distant portions of the
dendrites. In that case, our current-clamp measurements would suffer
the same problem that voltage-clamp recordings do when the space clamp
is inadequate: the current-induced changes in potential would be
different in different parts of the dendritic tree and soma. These
differences would in turn mean that the excitatory synaptic currents in
the dendrites would reverse only when the potential in the soma was
higher than 0 mV. Similarly, inhibitory synaptic currents in the
dendrites would reverse only when the potential in the soma was lower
than 65 or
100 mV. We would then mistake a portion of the
excitatory conductance for a negative inhibitory conductance, and vice
versa. There are hints of this type of error in the responses to
electrical stimulation (Fig. 14). But the time course and relative
magnitude of the calculated conductances in this case are similar to
what would be expected. Furthermore this type of error could not
generate negative inhibitory conductances that are larger in amplitude
than the excitatory conductances. Since the derived inhibitory
conductances are two to three times larger than the excitatory
conductances, we conclude that errors arising from inadequate space
clamp are not large enough to affect our results significantly.
The technique used here to derive excitatory and inhibitory
conductances offers significant advantages over other methods. For
example, in the study of Pei et al. (1994), tuning
curves for excitation and inhibition were inferred from the
hyperpolarization and depolarization amplitudes. Such techniques, which
rely solely on membrane potential traces, would miss concurrent
excitation and inhibition. This would lead to a significant
underestimation of inhibitory responses at orientations where
excitation is strongest. Similar considerations apply to the work of
Volgushev et al. (1992
, 1993
), who analyzed membrane
potential responses to visual stimuli, in one case during injection of
current. Finally, there may have been flaws in the approach of
Ferster and Jagadeesh (1992)
, who attempted to measure
conductance changes by observing changes in the amplitude of a test
EPSP evoked by electrical stimulation of the LGN. Because the test
EPSPs were so brief, the measurements were likely dominated by membrane
capacitance rather than resistance.
Push-pull arrangement of excitation and inhibition
While both excitation and inhibition are evoked by the preferred
stimulus, in the subfields of simple cells they are spatially segregated. As Hubel and Wiesel (1962) suggested, on
regions receive off inhibition, and off regions receive on inhibition
in a push-pull arrangement. The first description of push-pull
organization in intracellular recordings focused on hyperpolarizing
IPSPs evoked by flashing bars (Ferster 1988
). More
recently, large changes in conductance associated with these IPSPs have
been described by Borg-Graham et al. (1998)
and by
Hirsch et al. (1998)
. In agreement with Hirsch, we find
that the hyperpolarizing off responses in the on region (and
conversely, on responses in the off region) are dominated by the
increase in inhibitory inputs with little contribution from the
withdrawal of excitation. The use of sinusoidal gratings has allowed us
to make careful measurements of the relative placement of the
excitatory and inhibitory inputs within the subfields, where we find a
surprisingly accurate push-pull arrangement. The 153° average
displacement in the spatial phases of the inhibition and excitation
represents less than 1/6 of a stimulus cycle difference from a perfect
counterphase arrangement. Given that the stimulus is at the optimal
spatial frequency, such that the spacing of the stimulus bars is
comparable to the spacing of the subfields, this 153° offset means
that the on inhibitory inputs are superimposed with OFF excitatory
inputs (and vice versa) with an error of less than 20% of the
receptive field width. This 20% displacement, given the relatively
small sample size, cannot be reliably distinguished from perfect
counterphase. This small difference might also be accounted for by a
difference in temporal phase of excitatory and inhibitory inputs.
Our results agree closely with a model of cat simple cell function in
which a spatial push-pull arrangement of on and off excitation and
inhibition is a prominent feature. Troyer et al. (1998)
have applied push-pull inhibition to the feedforward models of
orientation selectivity (Hubel and Wiesel 1962
) to
correct the feedforward model's failure to predict the contrast
invariance of orientation selectivity. Their model makes a number of
predictions that are borne out by our results. First, it predicts an
identical orientation tuning of excitatory and inhibitory conductance
(e.g., Troyer 1998
, Fig. 10). Second, it predicts an
identical orientation tuning of the modulation of membrane potential
and modulation of conductance. Third, it predicts conductance changes
at the preferred orientation (about 100%) similar to those that we report.
Tuning of inhibition
We found that excitation and inhibition have similar tuning for
orientation, so that inhibition is unlikely to sharpen the tuning
conveyed by the excitatory inputs. These results are at odds with a
number of studies suggesting that intracortical inhibition is more
broadly tuned than excitation and thus helps in establishing the
orientation tuning of the cells (Ben-Yishai et al. 1995;
Blakemore and Tobin 1972
; Creutzfeldt et al.
1974
; Hata et al. 1988
; Sillito 1979
; Somers et al. 1995
; Sompolinsky and
Shapley 1997
).
More recently, Ringach et al. (1998) have suggested that
inhibition would be most effective in sharpening orientation tuning when its tuning width is only slightly greater than that of excitation. Inspection of Fig. 19 does not support such an arrangement. There does
not appear to be any systematic difference between the tuning width of
excitation and inhibition. On the other hand, this figure plots the
tuning widths of combined fits to mean and modulation. It might be that
individual tuning curves for the mean or modulation components of
excitation might be wider than their counterparts for excitation.
Figure 18, however, again shows no obvious systematic indication of
such an effect.
In most cells we did observe increases in membrane conductance for
stimuli of all orientations. These increases in conductance may go some
way toward explaining the suppression effects measured with plaids
obtained with two gratings differing in orientation (Bonds
1989; Carandini et al. 1999
; Gizzi et al.
1990
; Morrone et al. 1982
). In addition, our
data cannot rule out the possibility that broadly tuned inhibition may
be operative in cat complex cells or in cells of other species such as
the primate (Ringach et al. 1997
).
While our results are not consistent with a broadly tuned inhibition,
they do suggest an important role for intracortical excitation in
establishing orientation selectivity, as proposed by a number of
models, both recurrent (Ben-Yishai et al. 1995; Chance et al. 1999
; Douglas et al. 1991a
,
1995
; Martin 1988
; Ringach et al.
1997
; Somers et al. 1995
) and feedforward
(Troyer et al. 1998
). According to these models,
cortical cells receive substantial excitatory input from other cortical
cells that are similarly tuned. Our result that the mean input
conductance of simple cells is increased more by stimuli of the optimal
orientation than by stimuli of other orientations is consistent with
this view. To see why this is the case, it may help to consider the
predictions of a basic linear feedforward model of orientation tuning
in simple cells (Ferster 1987
; Hubel and Wiesel
1962
). Imagine that changes in input conductance of a cell
resulted from a weighted sum of the responses of properly aligned
unoriented cells in the LGN. In principle, the weights could be
positive and negative, reflecting both synaptic excitation and
inhibition, possibly through interneurons. Because the LGN cells are
not oriented, the orientation of the stimulus affects the relative
timing
but not the size
of their responses. When the stimulus has the
preferred orientation, these responses would be synchronized so that
their weighted sum is strongly modulated. But the mean input
conductance generated by the geniculate input would not be tuned for
stimulus orientation because the mean response of the individual cells
would be the same for all orientations.
If we are correct in our assumption that the changes in input
conductance are entirely synaptic, then our finding that the mean
conductance of simple cells is often tuned for stimulus orientation indicates that either simple cells integrate their inputs nonlinearly or they receive input from cells that have similar orientation tuning.
These, of course, would have to be other cortical cells. The first
explanation is supported by mounting evidence for nonlinearities operating at the synaptic (e.g., Abbott et al. 1997;
Markram and Tsodyks 1996
) or dendritic level (Mel
et al. 1998
). The second explanation is supported by evidence
that the cortical circuitry contributes a two- to threefold
amplification of the responses of simple cells (Chung and
Ferster 1998
; Ferster et al. 1996
; Martin
1988
). These explanations are not mutually exclusive, and further experiments will be needed to determine their validity.
![]() |
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS |
---|
We thank L. Borg-Graham and J. Hirsch for helpful suggestions on bridge-balancing techniques and K. Martin and T. Troyer for comments on the manuscript.
This work was supported by National Eye Institute Grant EY-04726 to D. Ferster. J. S. Anderson was supported by NEI Training Grant EY-07128. M. Carandini was partly supported by a Howard Hughes Medical Institute investigatorship to J. A. Movshon.
![]() |
FOOTNOTES |
---|
Address for reprint requests: D. Ferster, Dept. of Neurobiology and Physiology, Northwestern University, 2153 N Campus Dr., Evanston, IL 60208 (E-mail: ferster{at}northwestern.edu).
The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. The article must therefore be hereby marked "advertisement" in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
Received 10 January 2000; accepted in final form 5 May 2000.
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|