Department of Biology and the Program in Movement Science, Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri 63130
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Field, Edelle C. and Paul S. G. Stein. Spinal cord coordination of hindlimb movements in the turtle: intralimb temporal relationships during scratching and swimming. J. Neurophysiol. 78: 1394-1403, 1997. Spinal cord neuronal circuits generate motor neuron activity patterns responsible for rhythmic hindlimb behaviors such as scratching and swimming. Kinematic analyses of limb movements generated by this motor neuron output reveal important characteristics of these behaviors. Intralimb kinematics of the turtle hindlimb were characterized during five distinct rhythmic forms of behavior: three forms of scratching and two forms of swimming. In each movement cycle for each form, the angles of the hip and knee joints were measured as well as the timing of a behavioral event, e.g., rub onset in scratching or powerstroke onset in swimming. There were distinct differences between the kinematics of different forms of the same behavior, e.g., rostral scratch versus pocket scratch. In contrast, there were striking similarities between forms of different behaviors, e.g., rostral scratch versus forward swimming. For each form of behavior there was a characteristic angular position of the hip at the onset of each behavioral event (rub or powerstroke). The phase of the onset of knee extension within the hip position cycle occurred while the hip was flexing in the rostral scratch and forward swim and while the hip was extending in the pocket scratch, caudal scratch, and back-paddling form of swimming. The phase of the onset of the behavioral event was not statistically different between rostral scratch and forward swim; nor was it different between pocket scratch and caudal scratch. These observations of similarities at the movement level support the suggestion that further similarities, such as shared spinal circuitry, may be present at the neural circuitry level as well.
Motor tasks are classified according to their goal. The goal of scratching is to use a limb to generate force against a site on the body surface that has received a stimulus; the goal of locomotion is to move an organism's center-of-mass from one place to another. Each task may have a number of variations, e.g., forward walking or backward walking (cat: Buford and Smith 1990a Red-eared turtles (n = 48), Trachemys scripta elegans (formerly Pseudemys scripta elegans; 285-730 gm) were studied. All procedures followed National Institutes of Health guidelines and were approved by the Washington University Animal Care and Use Committee. Detailed analyses of limb movement were performed on a subset of turtles (n = 20). Spinalized turtles (n = 15) were used to study scratching; intact turtles (n = 5) were used to study swimming. In all turtles, bony landmarks of the hip, the knee, and the third toe of both hindlimbs were marked with reflective paint.
Activation of scratching and swimming
During experiments that examined scratching behaviors in spinal turtles, each turtle was held in a band clamp that encircled the middle of its body. Scratching was evoked by stimulation in the approximate center of each scratch form's receptive field. The sites used (Fig. 1) were the rostral scratch receptive field site SP2 (stimulus position 2), pocket scratch receptive field site Femoral 5, and caudal scratch receptive field site Anal 5; for additional descriptions of these sites see Mortin et al. (1985)
Data collection
For scratch trials, the spinal turtle rested on a panel of nonreflective glass. The turtle was viewed from below via a mirror placed below the glass panel at an angle of 45°. When performed on a flat substrate, hindlimb scratching movements were relatively planar; a single camera was therefore adequate for measurements of joint angles. For swim trials, the turtle was also viewed from below via a camera positioned beneath the aquarium. Hindlimb movements were videotaped at 60 Hz with a shutter speed of 1/250 s. Data from trials in which the limb movement was not in a plane orthogonal to the optical axis of the camera were not analyzed. This was verified by calculating ratios with the use of measurements obtained from video data: the ratio of the length of the thigh segment to the length of a fixed segment on the shell and the ratio of the length of the shank segment to the length of a fixed segment on the shell. This was compared with the corresponding ratios measured directly on the animal. Trials in which the corresponding measures were within 90% of each other were analyzed.
Quantitative analyses
We selected episodes of scratching for analysis that were suitable both for this paper reporting intralimb kinematics and for our companion paper (Field and Stein 1997 ONSET OF THE BEHAVIORAL EVENT.
The timing of the onset of the behavioral event (rub for scratch, powerstroke for swim) was determined from the video recording. For scratch behaviors, the onset of the rub was defined as the time at which the limb made contact with the stimulated site. For swim behaviors, the onset of the powerstroke was defined as follows: for forward swimming, the time at which the foot began to move in a backward direction; for back-paddling, the time at which the foot began to move in a forward direction. At the onset of powerstroke, the foot was usually aligned in a vertical position. In addition, at this time, the distance between the toes was increasing and the webbing between the toes was spreading out.
PHASE ANALYSIS.
Phase analyses were performed with the use of custom software. Cycle period was defined as the time from the onset of a hip flexion to the onset of the subsequent hip flexion. Previous investigations have demonstrated that the timing of bursts in hip flexor motor neurons innervating the puboischiofemoralis internus, pars anteroventralis (VP-HP) muscle (Robertson et al. 1985 STATISTICAL METHODS.
Nonparametric statistical methods, both linear and circular, were used to compare movement parameters within behaviors and between behaviors; these statistics were chosen because the variance of the measures of interest differed widely between behaviors and because some parameters were not normally distributed. The Mann-Whitney U test (Hayes 1988 Scratch data from 15 turtles were analyzed. This included rostral scratch data from eight turtles, pocket scratch data from eight turtles, and caudal scratch data from four turtles. In three turtles, both rostral and pocket scratch data were collected; in one turtle, rostral, pocket, and caudal scratch data were collected. A total of 899 scratch cycles comprising 130 episodes (53 rostral, 52 pocket, and 25 caudal episodes) were analyzed. The number of cycles per episode ranged from 5 to 32. Swim data from five turtles were analyzed. A total of 67 swim cycles in eight episodes (3 forward swimming, 5 turning swimming) were analyzed.
General characteristics of scratch behavior
The three forms of scratch could be distinguished by the trajectory of the third toe of the hindlimb (Fig. 2) (Mortin et al. 1985
General characteristics of swim behavior
Spontaneous swimming was examined in intact turtles. Two common swim behaviors were studied: same-form forward swimming and mixed-form turning swimming. Both swim behaviors used all four limbs; in this study, only the hindlimb relationships were analyzed. In same-form forward swimming, each hindlimb displayed the forward swim form (Fig. 5A) and exerted force against the water during hip extension. When forward swimming movements (Lennard and Stein 1977
Quantitative comparisons between behaviors
We compared three movement variables across all behaviors: 1) the angular position of the hip at the onset of the behavioral event (rub for scratch, powerstroke for swim), 2) the phase of onset of knee extension within the hip cycle, and 3) the phase of onset of the behavioral event within the hip cycle. For each of the three measures, there were no significant differences between different turtles performing the same behavior; the Kruskal-Wallis test was used for the hip angle data and the Watson U2 test for the phase data. For each measure, we therefore pooled the data from all turtles performing the same behavior; these data are presented in Table 1.
Intralimb performance with bilateral symmetrical stimulation compared with unilateral stimulation
Intralimb characteristics during bilaterally evokedscratching in response to symmetrical sites were qualitatively similar to unilaterally evoked scratching. For each form of scratching, the trajectory of the third toe did not differ between unilaterally evoked and bilaterally evoked scratching. Interlimb phase measurements during bilateral stimulation are presented in the companion article (Field and Stein 1997
Intralimb performance with bilateral asymmetric stimulation compared with unilateral stimulation
We used the same quantitative measures to assess whether the intralimb kinematics of one hindlimb were altered when the contralateral hindlimb was engaged in a different form of movement. In the case of scratching, by simultaneously stimulating asymmetric stimulus sites on the two sides of the body, e.g., left rostral scratch receptive field and right pocket scratch receptive field, the animal was induced to perform two different forms of scratching concurrently. In the case of forward swim, we compared the kinematics of the hindlimb exhibiting the forward swimming form during mixed-form turning swimming with similar measures during same-form forward swimming. In each turtle, the ipsilateral limb kinematics were not significantly different during bilaterally asymmetric scratch or swim, as compared with unilateral scratch or bilaterally symmetrical swim, respectively, for the following measures: the angle of the hip at the onset of the behavioral event [Mann-Whitney U test, not significant; data marked with Contralateral limb activity with unilateral stimulation
In scratch behavior, there was often movement of the limb contralateral to the side of stimulation. In the rostral scratch, the contralateral movement had a limited range of motion and usually involved only hip movement. The one exception to this was a single episode in one turtle in which bilateral rostral scratching was produced in response to a right unilateral stimulation (Fig. 7A). This animal produced seven cycles of rhythmic behavior in the contralateral limb. Analysis of this episode demonstrated that the left limb was directed toward the left rostral scratch receptive field but the left toe did not come in contact with the shell bridge to perform a rub. In this contralateral limb, the phase of the onset of knee extension was slightly later than that observed in the same limb when it responded to stimulation in the ipsilateral rostral scratch receptive field; this difference was statistically significant (Watson U2 test, P < 0.01).
The major results of this paper are the demonstrations of 1) kinematic similarities between specific forms of one task and specific forms of another task and 2) kinematic differences between one form of a task and another form of the same task. Forward swimming kinematics were similar to rostral scratching kinematics; back-paddling kinematics were similar to both pocket scratching and caudal scratching kinematics. Forward swimming kinematics were different from back-paddling kinematics; rostral scratching kinematics were different from both pocket scratching and caudal scratching kinematics. Not all forms of the same task show major kinematic differences, however; there were similarities in the kinematics of pocket scratching and caudal scratching. The demonstration of kinematic similarities for different behaviors supports the concept that some of these similarities may be the result of shared neuronal circuits for these different behaviors.
Intralimb kinematics
The angular position of the hip at onset of the behavioral event (the rub in scratching, the powerstroke in swimming) differed for each form of scratch and for both forms of swim. The mean value of hip angle at powerstroke onset for back-paddle behavior was between the mean values of hip angle at rub onset for pocket scratch and for caudal scratch. The amount of hip extension varied considerably during back-paddle. In cycles with large hip excursions, the angle of the hip at event onset in back-paddle resembled that of the caudal scratch. In less robust responses, the hip excursion was limited and the angle of the hip at event onset resembled that of pocket scratch.
Intralimb phase during bilaterally evoked scratching
For the most part, the scratching performance of an individual limb was not altered when the contralateral limb was also induced to scratch at the same time. In particular, the hip angle at the onset of the rub was similar for 17 of 19 comparisons (Fig. 6). The few instances in which there were differences between the phase of the onset of the rub under the unilateral and bilateral stimulus are most likely due to shortening of the hip excursion that occurred in four of the bilateral trials. It is possible that excitation from contralateral elements resulted in relatively early onset of hip flexion in these episodes. This shortening of the excursion caused the rub to occur relatively later in the cycle period.
Rhythmic behavior in the contralateral limb
In the performance of scratch behavior, there may be tonic muscle activity present in a contralateral limb not engaged in the rub event (Sherrington 1910 Relationships of kinematics to motor patterns
Previous studies of electromyographic (EMG) recordings during the three forms of scratching in spinal turtles have demonstrated rhythmic alternation between hip flexor and hip extensor EMG activities in each movement cycle (Robertson et al. 1985
INTRODUCTION
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
,b
; Stein and Smith 1997
; humans: Thorstensson 1986
; Winter et al. 1989
). Each variation of a task is a specific movement strategy, a "form" of that task (Stein and Smith 1997
; Stein et al. 1986
). The turtle hindlimb displays three different forms of scratching (rostral, pocket, or caudal) (Mortin et al. 1985
) and two different forms of swimming (forward swimming or back-paddling) (Lennard and Stein 1977
; Stein 1978
). Previous kinematic analyses of scratching (Mortin et al. 1985
; Robertson et al. 1985
) and swimming (Davenport et al. 1984
; Lennard and Stein 1977
; Zug 1971
) focused on individual examples of a few cycles of movement. In the present paper we expand these earlier observations and provide quantitative characterization of the intralimb kinematics of the turtle hip and knee joints during each form of scratching and swimming. The companion paper (Field and Stein 1997
) provides a characterization of interlimb kinematics during each form.
). The rub is accomplished by the dorsum of the foot in rostral scratching, by the side of the thigh in pocket scratching, and by the heel of the foot in caudal scratching. For all three forms of turtle hindlimb scratching, the rub occurs during knee extension. In swimming, the powerstroke is the behavioral event during which propulsive force is exerted against the water. During the powerstroke, the knee is extended and the foot is held in a vertical position with toes spread apart; this maximizes the surface area of the webbing between adjacent toes. The two forms of turtle swimming differ in the direction of movement of the foot during the powerstroke. The powerstroke of the forward swim occurs while the foot is moving backward (retracting) (Davenport et al. 1984
; Lennard and Stein 1977
; Stein 1978
; Zug 1971
); the powerstroke of back-paddling occurs while the foot is moving forward (protracting) (Lennard and Stein 1977
; Stein 1978
). Stein (1983)
suggested that there were kinematic similarities between rostral scratching and forward swimming and, in addition, between caudal scratching and backward swimming (back-paddling). In the present paper, we provide evidence that supports these suggestions and demonstrate additional similarities in these behaviors.
; Currie and Stein 1989
; Stein et al. 1995
) and bilateral interneuronal activity (Berkowitz and Stein 1994a
,b
). In addition, bilateral stimulation of mirror-image sites in the left and right rostral scratch receptive fields elicits bilaterally coordinated fictive rostral scratching (Stein et al. 1995
). In the present paper we compare the intralimb kinematics during unilaterally evoked scratching, during bilaterally evoked scratching, and during swimming. The companion paper describes the interlimb kinematics during bilaterally evoked scratching and during swimming (Field and Stein 1997
). Some of the results in this paper were previously presented in an abstract (Field and Stein 1994
) and in a doctoral thesis (Field 1995
).
METHODS
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
). The turtle was placed in crushed ice for
1 h to obtain hypothermic anesthesia (Melby and Altman 1974
) and was maintained on ice for the duration of the surgical procedure. A midline channel was drilled in the carapace overlying the upper dorsal segments of the spinal cord. The spinal cord was completely transected between the second (D2) and third (D3) postcervical spinal segments just posterior to the forelimb enlargement. The turtle was removed from the ice and allowed to return to room temperature. Portions of the plastron in the ventral aspect of the pocket shell region were removed to provide better visualization of the hindlimb from below.
and Mortin and Stein (1990)
. One of two methods was used to evoke scratching behaviors. Mechanical stimulation was delivered by means of insect pins inserted into a specific site(s); scratch responses were evoked by touching the pin(s) with a fingertip and gently moving the pin(s) back and forth. Electrical stimulation was delivered via pairs of pin electrodes (interelectrode distance 3 mm) inserted in a specific site(s); electrical pulses of 2-10 V, 1 ms were delivered at 3 Hz for 5-10 s. Stimulus trials were performed in blocks of six in the following order: left stimulation, right stimulation, bilateral stimulation, bilateral stimulation, left stimulation, right stimulation.
View larger version (33K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 1.
Ventral view of the turtle, illustrating stimulus sites that evoke scratching and joint angle definitions. Stimulus points used were SP2 (stimulus point 2) in the rostral scratch receptive field, Fem 5 (Femoral 5) in the pocket scratch receptive field, and Anal 5 in the caudal scratch receptive field. Hip angle is defined as the angle formed by the thigh and the ventral midline of the body. Knee angle is defined as the angle formed by the thigh and the shank. Increasing angle values indicate the joint is extending; decreasing angle values indicate the joint is flexing. This line drawing of the turtle is reproduced from the image reflected in a mirror that was placed beneath the turtle at a 45° angle. As such, the right limbs are on the right of the figure; the left limbs are on the left of the figure.
. Hip angle was defined as the angle formed by the thigh and the ventral midline of the body. Knee angle was defined as the angle formed by the thigh and the shank. The value of each joint angle increases when the joint is extending and decreases when the joint is flexing.
) reporting interlimb kinematics. The primary focus of the companion paper was on bilateral coupling during absolute (1:1) coordination. For both papers, therefore, we selected for detailed analyses those scratch episodes consisting of at least five cycles from those turtles in which unilateral stimuli produced ipsilateral responses that were robust for both the left and the right stimulus conditions. In addition, we only analyzed groups of episodes in which the cycle period of the left hindlimb in response to a left unilateral stimulus had a value that was near that of the cycle period of the right hindlimb in response to a right unilateral stimulus; in particular, we analyzed only those groups in which the cycle period on the "slow" side was <1.5 times the cycle period on the "fast" side. In addition, for studies of rostral scratching in both papers, we restricted the data set further: only cycles in which the maximum value of hip extension was >60° were analyzed. This additional criterion was used because cycles with limited hip extension are one of two variations of the rostral scratch termed "omissions" or "deletions" (Robertson et al. 1985
). In an omission, the hip flexor burst occurs and is followed by a period of hip flexor quiescence, but hip extensor activity is absent. In a deletion, the hip flexor burst is followed immediately by another hip flexor burst with no intervening hip flexor quiescence and no hip extensor burst. In either case, these cycles differed from the normal rostral scratch kinematically. When they occurred, such variant cycles usually took place within the first one to three cycles of an episode. In addition to the analyses described above, data recorded during specific episodes of stable 2:1 coordination were analyzed separately and are reported in a section of our companion paper (Field and Stein 1997
).
) offers a dependable temporal reference for scratch phase analyses (Berkowitz and Stein 1994b
; Stein et al. 1995
). The onset of hip flexion is the movement parameter most closely associated with onset of the burst of activity in the VP-HP muscle (Mortin et al. 1985
; Robertson et al. 1985
). The onset of hip flexion was defined as the point at which the joint angle displacement trace reached a maximum and thereafter reversed direction; the onset of hip extension was defined as the time at which the joint angle displacement trace reached a minimum and thereafter reversed direction. In cycles in which the hip joint angle remained within 10° of the maximum or minimum angle for >333 ms, the midpoint of this period was defined as the time of onset of flexion or extension, respectively. Analogous criteria were applied to define the onsets of knee flexion and extension.
of an event that occurred during hip flexion was defined as the latency of the event from the onset of hip flexion divided by twice the duration of hip flexion; the phase
of an event that occurred during hip extension was defined as the latency of the event from the onset of hip extension, divided by twice the duration of hip extension, plus 0.5. An analogous method has been used to study fictive scratching in the turtle (Berkowitz and Stein 1994b
; Stein et al. 1995
). Similar dual-reference techniques have been employed by Orlovsky (1972)
in characterizing the phase of vestibulospinal neuron activity during locomotion in the cat and by Burns and Usherwood (1979)
in the assessment of motoneuron activity during locomotion in locusts and grasshoppers.
). Each phase
(0
1) was converted to a two-dimensional unit vector with a length of 1 and an angle of 2
in radians. These unit vectors were averaged with the use of vector addition techniques. The angle of the mean vector in radians divided by 2
was defined as the mean phase (which varied between 0 and 1). The length of the mean vector (which varied between 0 and 1) represented the "strength" of the preferred phase. The mean vector length was also used to calculate the mean angular deviation, a measure of the dispersion of the phase data (Batschelet 1981
). These vector data were suited to the application of circular statistics (Batschelet 1981
).
; Runyon and Haber 1984
), an unpaired, signed-rank test for linear data, was used on pooled data to compare the angle of the hip at the onset of the behavioral event in different behaviors; this test was also used to evaluate differences in hip excursion between scratch forms. The Kruskal-Wallis test (Hayes 1988
), a generalized version of the Mann-Whitney U test for multiple-group comparisons, was used to test whether the hip angle at the onset of rub was statistically different between turtles performing the same form of a behavior. The Watson U2 test (Batschelet 1981
), an unpaired, signed-rank test for circular data, was used to compare the phase data between behaviors.
). In multiple pairwise tests, this is corrected by multiplying the final P value by the number of groups being tested. When the three scratch behaviors were tested against one another, the P value was multiplied by 3. When the five scratch and swim behaviors were tested against one another, the P value was multiplied by 5.
).
RESULTS
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
). Although this trajectory was similar in the pocket and caudal forms, the toe reached a more caudal position during caudal scratch. In rostral scratch (Fig. 3A), knee extension began during hip flexion and the rub began just before the maximal knee extension. In pocket scratch (Fig. 3B), knee extension began abruptly during hip extension; the rub began soon thereafter. In caudal scratch (Fig. 3C), knee extension also began during hip extension, but the knee extended more gradually; the rub began during knee extension near the midpoint of the hip extension phase. Thus the timing of knee extension within the hip movement cycle was similar for pocket and caudal scratch (Fig. 3, B and C).
View larger version (18K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 2.
Trajectory of the 3rd toe during 1 cycle of scratch for each of the 3 forms. A: rostral scratch response (stimulation at SP2). B: pocket scratch response (stimulation at Fem 5). C: caudal scratch response (stimulation at Anal 5). In A-C, the position of the left hindlimb indicates the typical posture at the time of rub onset. Portions of the hindlimb that would be obscured by the plastron are marked with dashes. The 3 linked line segments represent, from medial to lateral, the long axis of the thigh, shank, and foot. , direction of movement of the toe trajectory.
View larger version (37K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 3.
Joint angle displacements as a function of time for the hip( ) and knee (- - -) during 4 cycles of scratching. A: rostral scratch. B: pocket scratch. C: caudal scratch.
, onset of the rub phase. Vertical dashed lines at 0.5-s intervals are included to facilitate comparison between traces.
View larger version (25K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 4.
Comparison of minimum and maximum hip angles by scratch form. , rostral scratch;
, pocket scratch;
, caudal scratch. Left edge of each rectangle: average minimum hip angle. Right edge of each rectangle: average maximum hip angle. Horizontal error bars: values of the SD for the hip angle minimum or maximum. *, rectangles joined by the dashed line are significantly different at P < 0.003;
, rectangles joined by the dashed lines are significantly different at P < 0.03 (Mann-Whitney U test with correction factor). These data are from the turtle in which all 3 different forms of scratch were measured and are representative of the data from the other turtles in the study.
) were performed bilaterally by a freely moving turtle, the animal propelled itself forward with a linear trajectory. In mixed-form turning swimming, one hindlimb displayed the forward swim form; the other hindlimb displayed the back-paddle form of swimming (Fig. 5B). In performance of back-paddling, force is exerted against the water during hip flexion. Interlimb coordination during same-form forward swimming and mixed-form turning swimming is described in the companion article (Field and Stein 1997
).
View larger version (36K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 5.
Joint angle displacements as a function of time for the hip( ) and knee (- - -) during 4 cycles of spontaneous swimming. A: forward swim. B: back-paddle.
, onset of the powerstroke phase.
); in contrast, in the forward swim, the duration of the powerstroke was longer; it began early in the hip extension phase and continued throughout most of the hip extension phase (Davenport et al. 1984
; Lennard and Stein 1977
; Zug 1971
).
View this table:
TABLE 1.
Summary of pairwise statistical comparisons for pooled data for each of three forms of scratch and two forms of swim
).
View larger version (41K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 6.
Angular position of the hip at the onset of the behavioral event (rub for scratch, powerstroke for swim) for each of the 5 behaviors. For scratch data, data are titled by the episode number of the 1st episode of a block of 6 (see METHODS for sequence of stimulus presentation in blocks); for swim data, data are titled by the specific episode number. Filled bars: episodes of bilateral scratch behavior. Striped bars: episodes of unilateral scratch behavior. Shaded bars: swim behavior. , data from a mixed-form trial in which 1 hindlimb performed one form and the other hindlimb performed another form (rostral and pocket responses, respectively, in the case of the scratch trials, or forward swimming and back-paddle, respectively, in the case of the swim trials). Vertical error bars: SD of the hip angle at event onset for the episodes. *, hip angle at event onset was statistically different between the bilateral and unilateral conditions (Mann-Whitney U test,P < 0.001).
in Fig. 6]; the phase of the onset of knee extension (Watson U2 test, not significant); the phase of the onset of the behavioral event (Watson U2 test, not significant); and the minimum and the maximum hip angles (Mann-Whitney U test, not significant).
View larger version (41K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 7.
Contralateral activity accompanying unilateral scratch stimulation: joint angle displacements as a function of time for the hip ( ) and knee (- - -). A: rostral scratch stimulus. B: pocket scratch stimulus. C: caudal scratch stimulus. Only the caudal response exhibited a true rub phase in the hindlimb contralateral to the stimulus.
DISCUSSION
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
). In cats, some of the variability may be attributed to differences in body posture (Abraham and Loeb 1985
; Kuhta and Smith 1994
). In turtles, however, body posture is a constrained parameter because of the rigid nature of the shell. The kinematic differences between the three scratch forms in the turtle may therefore reflect the diverse requirements of the behavioral goals of reaching diverse locations on a rigid body surface.
suggested that in turtles there are kinematic similarities 1) between forward swim and rostral scratch and 2) between back-paddle and caudal scratch. The present results support those suggestions and in addition demonstrate that back-paddle was similar to both pocket scratch and caudal scratch. These movement observations lead to suggestions about the organization of neuronal circuits that control these movements. In particular, is there some sharing of circuitry for rostral scratch and forward swim? In addition, is there some sharing among the circuits for pocket scratch, caudal scratch, and back-paddling? Previous work with single-unit recordings supports the concept of shared circuitry among left and right rostral scratch and pocket scratch circuitry (Berkowitz and Stein 1994a
,b
). Additional work with a hindlimb hemienlargement preparation provides further support for shared circuitry between left and right rostral scratch circuitry (Stein et al. 1995
). The present paper provides indications for additional sharing of circuitry controlling hindlimb motor rhythms.
). The present study demonstrates, however, that there may be rhythmic movement of the hindlimb contralateral to the stimulus. Rhythmic activity during ipsilateral scratching has also been observed in contralateral motor output (Berkowitz and Stein 1994a
; Currie and Stein 1989
; Deliagina et al. 1981
; O'Donovan et al. 1982
; Stein et al. 1995
) and in contralateral interneurons (Berkowitz and Stein 1994a
,b
). For most episodes of rostral and pocket scratch in this study, the contralateral hindlimb did not perform a functional rub during these movements. In contrast, for the caudal scratch, the contralateral limb often rubbed a site on that side that was the mirror image of the point being stimulated. This bilateral response for the caudal scratch may be due, in part, to the proximity of the left and the right caudal scratch receptive fields.
; Stein et al. 1986
). Kinematic measurements in the present study, as well as in earlier studies (Mortin et al. 1985
), demonstrate rhythmic alternation between hip flexion and hip extension. The present study adds to these observations with the discovery of a specific minimum hip angle for each form and a distinct maximum hip angle for the rostral scratch compared with each of the other forms. These kinematic observations indicate that analyses of the amplitude of hip flexor and extensor motor output may be useful in future studies of motor patterns during turtle scratching.
; Stein and Johnstone 1986
). For both behaviors, there is similar timing of knee extensor activity in the cycle of hip EMG activities: the offset of knee extensor activity occurs near the offset of hip flexor activity. The present study establishes kinematic similarities between these two behaviors as well.
). In contrast, EMG data establish that the rub of the rostral scratch takes place during hip flexor EMG activity and before hip extensor EMG activity (Fig. 1A of Robertson et al. 1985
). Future quantitative studies of simultaneous measures of kinematics and motor outputs for the behaviors reported in the present paper are now needed.
![]() |
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS |
---|
We thank Dr. G. Perry for software development, Dr. A. Berkowitz for editorial assistance, and M. McCullough and G. Earhart for comments.
This work was supported by National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant NS-30786 to P.S.G. Stein and a Doctoral Research Award from the Foundation for Physical Therapy Research to E. C. Field.
![]() |
FOOTNOTES |
---|
Present address of E. C. Field: University of Miami School of Medicine, Division of Physical Therapy, 5915 Ponce de Leon Blvd., 5th Floor, Coral Gables, FL 33146.
Address for reprint requests: P.S.G. Stein, Dept. of Biology, Washington University, St. Louis, MO 63130.
Received 11 February 1997; accepted in final form 22 May 1997.
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|