Department of Physiology, Tohoku University School of Medicine, Sendai 980, Japan
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Fujii, Naotaka, Hajime Mushiake, and Jun Tanji. Rostrocaudal Distinction of the Dorsal Premotor Area Based on Oculomotor Involvement. J. Neurophysiol. 83: 1764-1769, 2000. To investigate functional differences between the rostral and caudal parts of the dorsal premotor cortex (PMd), we first examined the effects of intracortical microstimulation (ICMS) while monkeys were performing oculomotor and limb motor tasks or while they were at rest. We found that saccades were evoked from the rostral part (PMdr) whereas ICMS in the caudal part (PMdc) predominantly produced forelimb or body movements. Subsequently, we examined neuronal activity in relation to the performance of visually cued and memorized saccades while monkeys reached an arm toward a visual target. We found that roughly equal numbers of PMdr neurons were active during performance of the oculomotor and limb motor tasks. In contrast, the majority of PMdc neurons were related preferentially to arm movements and not to saccades. In the subsequent analysis, we found that the oculomotor effects evoked in the PMdr differ from the effects evoked in either the frontal eye field (FEF) or supplementary eye field (SEF). These findings suggest that the PMdr is involved in oculomotor as well as limb motor behavior. However, the oculomotor involvement of the PMdr seems to have a functional aspect different from that operating in the FEF and SEF.
![]() |
INTRODUCTION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
The premotor cortex is divided into dorsal and
ventral parts termed the PMd and PMv, respectively. Anatomic studies
suggest that the PMd can be further divided into rostral and caudal
subregions. Cytoarchitectonic and myeloarchitectonic studies show that
the anatomic connectivity in these two areas is different. Barbas and
Pandya (1987) called the rostral area the PMdr (approximately corresponding to F7 of Matelli et al. 1998
) and the caudal area the PMdc (F2 of Matelli et al. 1998
). Only the PMdc sends its output to the primary motor cortex (MI) and spinal cord (Dum and Strick 1991
; He et al. 1993
) whereas only the
PMdr receives prominent input from the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
(Lu et al. 1994
). The parietal inputs to the PMdr and
PMdc are also different (Ghosh and Gattera 1995
;
Matelli et al. 1998
). Although these anatomic differences have been identified, little is known about the functional or physiological differences between the two areas. It appears as
though neurons in the PMdr may be characterized by their responses to
visual signals and activities related to motor preparation (Johnson et al. 1996
) or the anticipation of visual cues
(di Pellegrino and Wise 1991
).
In this study, we compared the oculomotor involvement of the PMdr and the PMdc. We found profound differences in the oculomotor effects of intracortical microstimulation (ICMS) and in the neuronal activity related to visually cued saccades and arm movements.
![]() |
METHODS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Two monkeys (Macaca fuscata) were trained to perform
a delayed saccade task and an arm-reaching task. A monkey sat in a
primate chair facing a panel containing five light-emitting diodes
(LEDs); one LED was located in the center of the panel and the other
four were positioned 20° above, below, right, and left of the central LED. The monkey's eye position was monitored with an infrared reflecting monitor system with a resolution of 4 ms. The task began
when the monkey placed its hand on the holding switch and fixated on
the LED that was illuminated as the fixation point. The monkey
was required to fixate on the LED for 1.5-2 s (precue period). In the
saccade task, a second LED, which served as the cue for the future
saccade target, was illuminated for 100 ms after the precue fixation
period. If the animal maintained fixation on the original LED for an
additional 500-800 ms (delay period), the original LED was dimmed.
This served as the GO signal. The monkey was rewarded if the second LED
was captured with a saccade within 400 ms of the GO signal. In the
arm-reaching task, the color of the fixation target was changed from
red to green after the monkey had maintained fixation on the fixation
target for 1.5-2.0 s (precue period). This change in color served as
the GO signal for the arm-reaching task; the monkey was required to release the hold switch (movement onset) and touch the fixation target
within 500 ms. The saccade and arm-reaching tasks were randomly
selected so that the monkey did not know the type of task or the target
until the precue period was over. While the monkey performed the
saccade task, we applied ICMS (330 Hz, 10-50 pulses, 0.2 ms duration)
under two different conditions. The first condition was that ICMS was
delivered 500 ms after the onset of fixation (fixation ICMS). The
second condition was that the initiation of ICMS was timed to occur
during the delay period and before the GO signal (pre-GO ICMS). We used
conventional electrophysiological techniques to record single unit
activity and ICMS (Fujii et al. 1998a,b
;
Mushiake et al. 1996
).
During the initial sessions of this study, we explored the MI and PMd to identify and map the arm and body representation areas. For this purpose, we examined ICMS effects while a monkey sat quietly in the chair with unrestrained limbs, at times consuming fruit or vegetables. We used stimulus currents of 5-40 µA in the MI and 40-80 µA in the PMd. ICMS was delivered at 500-µm intervals from the surface. The current with which skeletal or eye movements were evoked with 50% probability was defined as the threshold at each site.
In subsequent sessions, we let the same monkeys perform the trained motor task and examined the entire PMd, supplementary eye field (SEF), and frontal eye field (FEF) by delivering ICMS (40-80 µA in the PMd and SEF, 20-40 µA in the FEF). We explored the lateral surface of the frontal cortex from the central sulcus to the anterior edge of the superior limb of the arcuate sulcus at intervals of 0.5-1 mm. After examining the ICMS effects, we recorded the discharges of single cells during performance of the trained task using the same electrode as was used for the ICMS studies.
We statistically analyzed the neuronal activity related to either the saccade or arm-reaching movement. First, we calculated the mean firing rate and its standard deviation during a control period of 1 s at the end of the fixation period. If the mean firing rate during the premovement period (from the GO signal to the start of the eye or arm movement) exceeded the mean firing rate during the control period by more than two standard deviations, we identified the neuronal activity as premovement-related. We then compared the premovement-related neuronal discharges during the saccade and arm-reaching tasks. The number of discharges during the premovement period was analyzed statistically (Mann-Whitney U test).
If statistically significant activity changes were observed during the premovement period under only one of the two conditions, the PMd neurons were defined as being related to that condition only (saccade only or reach only). If the changes in activity were significantly different under both task conditions (P < 0.01, Mann-Whitney U test), the PMd neurons were defined as being predominantly related to saccades (saccade > reach) or predominantly related to arm-reaching (reach > saccade). The remaining PMd neurons for which premovement-related activity was not statistically different under both task conditions were defined as being related to both movements (both).
![]() |
RESULTS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Before recording neuronal activity from the PMd, we identified the
MI, FEF, and SEF on the basis of ICMS effects. The FEF was located in
the anterior wall of the arcuate sulcus. The SEF was found in the
dorsal frontal cortex approaching the medial wall (Fujii et
al.1995; Schlag and Schlag-Rey 1987
). We also
found a site near the spur of the arcuate sulcus where ICMS evoked slow eye movements. Weak currents in the MI evoked skeletomotor responses in
the forelimb (<40 µA, 10 pulses) whereas relatively stronger currents (>40 µA) and longer pulse trains (>20 pulses) were
required in the PMd to evoke skeletomotor responses in the forelimb and body. Figure 1A shows the
effects of ICMS in the left PMd, which is defined as the dorsal
convexity of the frontal cortex rostral to the MI and caudal to the
anterior edge of the upper limb of the arcuate sulcus, excluding the
anterior and posterior banks of the arcuate sulcus (cf. Fig. 2 in Russo
and Bruce 1993
). The border between the PMd and MI was drawn according
to previously proposed physiological and cytoarchitectonic criteria
(Kurata and Tanji 1986
; Weinrich
1982
). According to these criteria, muscle contractions
are readily observed caudal to the border between the PMd and the MI
with stimulus currents <40 µA. This border approximately corresponds
to the cytoarchitectonic border based on the concentration of
large-diameter pyramidal cells in layer V. In the PMd, we defined the
effect of ICMS as positive when ICMS evoked eye movements or
skeletomotor responses with >50% probability. We looked for both
skeletal and eye movements in all penetrations with currents
80 µA.
In the PMd, the threshold that was needed to elicit eye movements
ranged from 60 to 80 µA and the threshold for skeletal movements
ranged from 40 to 60 µA. Within this range of stimulus currents, we
did not find any stimulus sites from which ICMS evoked both eye and
skeletal movements. The PMd extended from the area just rostral to the
MI to the rostral end of the upper limb of the arcuate sulcus,
excluding the posterior wall of the arcuate sulcus. As shown in Fig.
1A, the effects of ICMS in the rostral part of the PMd
differed from the effects in the caudal part. The rostral part evoked
saccades whereas the caudal part evoked predominantly limb or body
movements. The genu of the arcuate sulcus corresponded approximately to
the border between the two regions. The evoked eye movements were
saccadic and not slow and their velocities exceeded 700°/s (Fig.
1C). The evoked saccades were always directed toward the
contralateral side (Fig. 1B) and were little influenced
by eye position. Based on these ICMS results, we divided the PMd into
the PMdr and the PMdc.
|
In a subsequent study, we examined whether or not ICMS evoked different
effects in the PMdr than those evoked in the FEF and SEF. When ICMS was
applied during the fixation period, we found no differences in the
saccades evoked in the PMd, FEF, and SEF. Constant-vector-type
saccades were evoked and the gaze returned to the fixation point.
However, when ICMS was applied during the delay period and before the
onset of the GO signal (pre-GO ICMS), we found that the effects of ICMS
differed. Specifically, we found a difference in the return movement
from the evoked saccades. When we applied ICMS to the PMdr 100 ms
before the GO signal, the evoked saccades starting from the fixation
point were followed by saccades aimed at the target specified by the
cue LED (Fig. 2, top). The
target was captured in 93% of the cases. The average latency of return
saccades was 173 ± 49 ms (SD) after the termination of saccades
evoked by ICMS. In contrast, when ICMS was delivered to the FEF, the
return saccades brought the line of sight to the fixation point in 63%
of the cases (Fig. 2, middle). The mean latency of the
return saccade was 152 ± 48 ms after termination of the saccades
evoked by ICMS. On the other hand, when ICMS was delivered to the SEF
during the delay period, saccades were evoked directly to the saccade
target (Fig. 2, bottom), as shown in Fujii et
al. (1995). These data demonstrated that the effects of
ICMS to the PMdr differed from those of ICMS to the FEF and SEF.
|
In addition to the analysis of ICMS effects, we examined neuronal activity in the PMd of the same animals, focusing on the appearance of saccade-related, as opposed to arm-movement-related, activity during performance of the trained task. We classified PMd neurons into five types: 1) exclusively related to saccades (Fig. 3A), 2) exclusively related to arm reaching (Fig. 3B), 3) predominantly related to saccades, 4) predominantly related to arm reaching, and 5) related to both movements (Fig. 3C). We recorded movement-related neuronal activity from 194 PMdr neurons and 347 PMdc neurons. As shown in Table 1, as many as 43% of the neurons in the PMdr showed a preference for the saccade task. In contrast, most (95%) of the PMdr neurons showed a preference for the arm-reaching task.
|
|
![]() |
DISCUSSION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
In the present study, we found that the effects of ICMS with low
currents appeared to differ in the PMdr and the PMdc. The former evoked
saccades whereas the latter evoked predominantly limb and body
movements. Furthermore, we found that the properties of neuronal
activity in relation to the trained oculomotor and arm motor tasks also
differed in the two areas. Roughly equal numbers of neurons were active
in the PMdr while the eye and arm tasks were being performed whereas
most neurons in the PMdc were primarily related to the arm-reaching
task. The neuronal activity may indicate further involvement of the
PMdr in oculomotor tasks, although we admit that other differences in
our task conditions may have contributed to the activity differences
(e.g., the saccade task involved an instruction delay whereas there was
no delay in the arm-reaching task). These findings provide a
physiological basis for dividing the PMd into rostral and caudal parts.
This accords with the anatomic view that the PMd includes two
cytoarchitectonically separable areas, defined as PMdr and PMdc
(Barbas and Pandya 1987).
In the present study, the cortical territory in the PMdr that evoked
saccades was separated from the SEF by 5-6 mm of cortex where ICMS
evoked no movements, at least with the low current that we employed.
Thus the saccade-evoking area in the PMd appears to be a cortical
territory distinct from the SEF. A report by Shook et al. (1991) also
supports the view that the the PMdr and the SEF are separate entities.
They demonstrated that these two areas have separate anatomic
connections with the thalamus, striatum, and forebrain nuclei. The PMdr
is also separate from the FEF, which is located in the bank of the
arcuate sulcus. It is important to note that the oculomotor effects
evoked in the PMdr in our study were different from the effects evoked
in the SEF and FEF. This suggests that the PMdr is involved in
oculomotor behavior in a manner different from the SEF and the FEF.
We interpret the ICMS effects in the three areas in the following
way: ICMS to the FEF evokes stimulus-site-specific saccades that
override instructed saccades. In contrast, ICMS to the SEF evokes
instructed saccades that override stimulus-site-specific saccades. In
the PMdr, the stimulus evokes the stimulus-site-specific saccade
first, followed by saccades to instructed targets. Mitz and Godschalk
(1989) hinted at the oculomotor involvement of the PMdr. However, they
evoked saccades primarily from a region rostral to the PMd. In their
cortical map, the saccade-evoking sites were located on the medial part
of the prefrontal cortex, rostral to the anterior end of the upper limb
of the arcuate sulcus. More recently, Boussaoud et al. (1998)
found
that eye position modulated neuronal activity in the PMdr; however, the
role of the PMd in oculomotor control was not studied. Interestingly,
in the cebus monkey, ICMS of the area dorsal to the upper limb of the
arcuate sulcus was reported to evoke slow eye movements (Tian
and Lynch 1996
, 1997
). In macaque monkeys,
however, the cortical site from which slow eye movements are evoked is
located immediately posterior to the arcuate sulcus, close to its spur
(Gottlieb et al. 1993
, 1994
;
Tanaka and Fukushima 1998
). There appears to be a
species difference; the slow-eye part of the FEF in the cebus monkey is located dorsal to the upper limb of the arcuate sulcus and the PMdr is
located elsewhere.
Previous reports on the corticocortical connections to the two portions
of the PMd revealed that the PMdr, but not the PMdc, receives the
output from the FEF (Ghosh and Gattera 1995;
Stanton et al. 1993
) whereas the parietal cortex caudal
to the intraparietal sulcus projects to the PMdr but not to the PMdc
(Ghosh and Gattera 1995
; Johnson et al.
1996
). These reports on anatomic connectivity accord with our
findings of oculomotor involvement of the PMdr. We hypothesize that the
PMdr, with additional input from the prefrontal cortex (periprincipal
sulcus part) (Arikuni et al. 1988
; Barbas and Pandya 1987
; Lu et al.
1994
), plays a role in coordinating eye and arm movements in a context
that requires cognitive behavioral control. Further studies are needed
to reveal the actual aspects of behavioral factors that call for the
involvement of this area.
![]() |
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS |
---|
We thank M. Kurama and Y. Takahashi for technical assistance.
This work was supported by Japan Science and Technology Corporation (Core Research for Evolutional Science and Technology), the Ministry of Education, Science and Culture of Japan (09680809, 11145205, and 11170205), and The Cooperation Research Program of Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University.
![]() |
FOOTNOTES |
---|
The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. The article must therefore be hereby marked "advertisement" in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
Received 26 July 1999; accepted in final form 30 November 1999.
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|