1Program in Physical Therapy, 2Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, and 3Department of Neurology and Neurological Surgery, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Missouri 63108
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Bastian, A. J., K. M. Zackowski, and W. T. Thach. Cerebellar Ataxia: Torque Deficiency or Torque Mismatch Between Joints?. J. Neurophysiol. 83: 3019-3030, 2000. Prior work has shown that cerebellar subjects have difficulty adjusting for interaction torques that occur during multi-jointed movements. The purpose of this study was to determine whether this deficit is due to a general inability to generate sufficient levels of phasic torque inability or due to an inability to generate muscle torques that predict and compensate for interaction torques. A second purpose was to determine whether reducing the number of moving joints by external mechanical fixation could improve cerebellar subjects' targeted limb movements. We studied control and cerebellar subjects making elbow flexion movements to touch a target under two conditions: 1) a shoulder free condition, which required only elbow flexion, although the shoulder joint was unconstrained and 2) a shoulder fixed condition, where the shoulder joint was mechanically stabilized so it could not move. We measured joint positions of the arm in the sagittal plane and electromyograms (EMGs) of shoulder and elbow muscles. Elbow and shoulder torques were estimated using inverse dynamics equations. In the shoulder free condition, cerebellar subjects made greater endpoint errors (primarily overshoots) than did controls. Cerebellar subjects' overshoot errors were largely due to unwanted flexion at the shoulder. The excessive shoulder flexion resulted from a torque mismatch, where larger shoulder muscle torques were produced at higher rates than would be appropriate for a given elbow movement. In the shoulder fixed condition, endpoint errors of cerebellar subjects and controls were comparable. The improved accuracy of cerebellar subjects was accompanied by reduced shoulder flexor muscle activity. Most of the correct cerebellar trials in the shoulder fixed condition were movements made using only muscles that flex the elbow. Our findings suggest that cerebellar subjects' poor shoulder control is due to an inability to generate muscle torques that predict and compensate for interaction torques, and not due to a general inability to generate sufficient levels of phasic torque. In addition, reducing the number of muscles to be controlled improved cerebellar ataxia.
![]() |
INTRODUCTION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Recent work has shown that cerebellar ataxia
during multi-jointed reaching may be due to an inability to adjust for
dynamic interaction torques (Bastian et al. 1996;
Topka et al. 1998a
,b
). Interaction torques are the
mechanical consequence of moving limb segments that are linked
together. During a reach, elbow movement causes an interaction torque
to occur at the shoulder, and shoulder movement causes an interaction
torque to occur at the elbow. Interaction torques may assist or oppose
the desired movement at each joint depending on direction, velocity and
acceleration of joint movements. Cerebellar subjects have difficulty
offsetting interaction torques especially during fast, multi-jointed
reaching movements (Bastian et al. 1996
; Topka et
al. 1998b
). Specifically, the torque produced by the muscles
did not counter the interaction torque appropriately, allowing
interaction torques to contribute excessively to the generation of the
movement (Bastian et al. 1996
). This resulted in an
abnormal pattern of reaching, with the elbow and shoulder joints moving
at inappropriate rates relative to one another and the fingertip
overshooting the target (Bastian et al. 1996
;
Topka et al. 1998a
).
When asked to move slowly and accurately, cerebellar subjects reached
using patterns that were either similar to controls (Topka et
al. 1998a,b
) or were "decomposed" (Bastian et al.
1996
). Decomposition means that subjects moved the shoulder
while fixing the elbow, then moved the elbow while fixing the shoulder
(Holmes 1939
). Decomposition was hypothesized to
represent a compensatory strategy to improve accuracy by voluntarily
fixing all but one joint. The decomposition would have reduced the
interaction torques occurring at the moving joint, although interaction
torques would still have occurred at the fixed joint (Bastian et
al. 1996
).
Based on these studies, it has been suggested that a major role of the
cerebellum is in the generation of muscle torques that predict and
compensate for interaction torques caused by other moving joints
(Bastian et al. 1996; Schweighofer et al.
1998a
,b
). Under this hypothesis, we propose that cerebellar
subjects should have difficulty with fast movements that produce
greater interaction torques because they cannot precisely activate
multiple muscles to offset or accommodate the anticipated interaction
torques. Thus cerebellar subjects theoretically could under- or
overcompensate for interaction torques. Cerebellar deficits, such as
dysmetria, should be also be greatly improved when interaction torques
are eliminated.
Others have proposed that this impairment is a result of a generalized
inability to quickly generate the appropriate muscle torque levels
(Boose et al. 1999). If this second hypothesis is correct, then cerebellar subjects should have a general deficit in
producing phasic torques, which would account for their inability to
offset interaction torques. Overall, one may expect that cerebellar subjects would consistently produce smaller muscle torques at much
lower rates than controls. This reduction in torque rate should account
for the types of errors observed during the movement.
Either of these two mechanisms could conceivably explain an inability to adjust for the larger interaction torques generated during fast reaching movements. To distinguish between these possibilities, we studied controls and cerebellar subjects making targeted elbow flexion movements in two different conditions. In the shoulder free condition, subjects pointed to a target located at a position that required movement at the elbow joint only, although the shoulder was free to move. Movements in the shoulder fixed condition were to the same target position, and the shoulder was mechanically stabilized so it could not move. In the shoulder free condition, cerebellar subjects over- and undershot the target; in shoulder fixed condition, cerebellar subjects made errors comparable to controls. Cerebellar overshoot errors in the shoulder free condition were primarily due to excessive shoulder flexion. The excessive shoulder flexion resulted from larger shoulder muscle torques produced at higher rates than would be appropriate for a given elbow movement. This mismatch occurred early in the movement (within the 1st 100 ms). These findings are consistent with the first hypothesis, that the cerebellum is involved in generating muscle torques that predict and compensate for interaction torques. It is not consistent with the second hypothesis because cerebellar subjects often produced greater shoulder muscle torques at a similar rate as controls.
Improvement in cerebellar subjects' performance in the shoulder fixed
condition was accompanied by a significant reduction in shoulder flexor
electromyogram (EMG) compared with controls. Our findings indicate that
cerebellar subjects have greater difficulty in moving accurately when
they have to control the muscles acting at more than one joint. As a
consequence, reducing the number of muscles to be controlled appears to
improve cerebellar ataxia. Preliminary results of this work have been
previously reported (Bastian et al. 1997).
![]() |
METHODS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Subjects
Eight control subjects and eight subjects with cerebellar cortical atrophy participated in this study. Presence of cerebellar damage was assessed by neurological examination and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan. In addition, results of genetic testing were sometimes available. A clinical neurological examination was done on each subject to check for involvement of other systems and to assess the severity of ataxia (Table 1). One subject (CBL02) showed a Babinski's reflex on the right side, but no other clinical signs of corticospinal involvement (e.g., no increased muscle tone of arms and legs). One subject with olivopontocerebellar atrophy (OPCA) had probable degeneration in the cerebellum, pons, and inferior olive. Two other subjects had hereditary spinocerebellar ataxias (SCA6 and SCA7), which have exclusively or predominantly cerebellar involvement. The subject with SCA7 had no chorea or pyramidal signs. Control subjects were matched for age, sex, handedness, and arm used to perform the movement. Informed consent was acquired prior to testing all subjects.
|
Paradigm
All subjects performed elbow flexion movements in the sagittal plane under shoulder free and shoulder fixed conditions (Fig. 1). Prior to testing, reflective markers were placed on the tip of index finger, hand (index metacarpalphalangeal joint), wrist (dorsal joint surface), elbow (lateral epicondyle), and shoulder (tip of acromion process). Bipolar, surface EMG electrodes were placed over the anterior deltoid (AD), posterior deltoid (PD), biceps (BI), brachioradialis (BR), and triceps (TRI). Two 4-cm ball markers suspended from flexible plastic wires indicated the start and target positions for the reaching movements under both conditions.
|
SHOULDER FREE CONDITION. Subjects were seated with their back supported and the arm positioned at the side with the shoulder in 0-10° of flexion, elbow in 80-90° flexion, and forearm in neutral pronation-supination (Fig. 1A). The target was located at a position that required 35° of elbow flexion and no shoulder movement. During this condition the shoulder was free to move. All subjects were asked to reach out and touch the target as fast as they could, while trying to stop on the target. Subjects were instructed to "move only the arm without leaning forward." Subjects were not specifically told to hold the shoulder joint steady. Three to 5 practice trials were given prior to 10 test trials.
SHOULDER FIXED CONDITION. This condition was identical to the shoulder free condition, except that the elbow rested on a flat surface (Fig. 1B). This position served to mechanically constrain movement at the shoulder and allow only the elbow to move. Three to 5 practice trials were given prior to 10 test trials.
Data collection
All movements were videotaped at 60 fields per second. The video camera was positioned to record the movement in the sagittal plane and zoomed in to provide the largest image possible while maintaining all markers in the field of view. Video data were digitized at 60 Hz to determine marker displacements (Peak Performance Technologies). EMG signals were time locked to the video data, amplified to obtain a strong signal, and collected at 600 Hz.
Analysis
KINEMATICS.
We analyzed five test trials from each subject moving in each
condition. Equal numbers of movements were used from each control and
cerebellar subject in the overall analysis. When possible, we chose the
first five trials because these would be the trials least likely
affected by fatigue. During digitization of the trials, we occasionally
found a movement that was clearly out of the sagittal plane, and this
movement was then rejected. This was determined by measuring the
apparent length change of the forearm during the movement. Apparent
length changes of up to 10% were considered acceptable for this type
of analysis (Hoy et al. 1985). Few trials were rejected
for this reason (<7 in both groups across both conditions).
TORQUE ANALYSIS.
Using videotaped kinematic data together with estimates of the inertial
mass of the different limb segments (Winter 1990), we
employed inverse dynamics equations to calculate the time series of the
1) net torque, 2) gravitational torque,
3) dynamic interaction torque, and 4) muscle
(residual) torque about both the elbow and shoulder joints
(Bastian et al. 1996). We define these terms in the
following way.
EMG. Raw EMG signals from all muscles were rectified and low-pass filtered at 100 Hz. A preliminary analysis was then done to determine whether each muscle was active during a given trial. A muscle was considered active if it increased 2 SD above a baseline measure (taken 500 ms prior to the go signal) for more than 40 ms. Further analysis determined differences in the muscle activity levels of the AD, BI, and BR between shoulder free and shoulder fixed conditions. We limited our analysis to these three muscles because 1) the BI and BR are the agonists for elbow flexion and were active 100% of the time, 2) the AD must be co-active with BI and BR to counteract shoulder interaction torques caused by early elbow flexion in the shoulder free condition, and 3) the PD and TRI (antagonists) were inconsistently active in both groups for both conditions (Table 2, see RESULTS). We integrated EMG signals in the AD, BI, and BR for the first 100 ms of movement, and then calculated the percent change in muscle activity from the shoulder free to the shoulder fixed conditions. Student's t-tests were used to test whether the percent change in muscle activity was different between the control and cerebellar groups.
|
![]() |
RESULTS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Endpoint error
Endpoint errors for control and cerebellar groups are shown in Fig. 2. Control subjects made small endpoint errors that were similar in both conditions. Cerebellar subjects often produced large endpoint errors in the shoulder free condition, but were able to perform at nearly control levels in the shoulder fixed condition. There was a significant group × condition interaction for endpoint error magnitude (P < 0.0005).
|
The majority of cerebellar errors in the shoulder free condition were overshoots. The cerebellar group produced 40% OS errors, 17% US errors, and 43% classified as No Error. Note that three of eight cerebellar subjects produced both over- and undershoot errors. These subjects tended to overshoot the target on the first trial and then undershoot the target on the subsequent trial. Cerebellar endpoint errors were dramatically reduced in the shoulder fixed condition, with only 15% OS errors, 7% US errors, and 78% classified as No Error.
Movement patterns associated with errors
KINEMATICS.
Figure 3A shows elbow ()
and shoulder (
) angular errors made in the shoulder free condition
and Fig. 3B shows elbow angular errors made in the shoulder
fixed condition. Angular errors at both joints were normalized so that
the desired end angle was 0°. Flexor deviations from the desired
angle are represented as positive values, and extensor deviations are
represented as negative values. In the shoulder free condition, control
subjects flexed the shoulder slightly even though movement of this
joint was not required (Fig. 3A,
). Cerebellar subjects
flexed the shoulder excessively when they made OS errors, but were
comparable to controls in all other categories. Shoulder flexion
movements in the OS category were significantly larger than those made
in all other categories (all P < 0.0005). Figure
4 shows that shoulder flexion began
concurrently with elbow flexion and was not a reaction to an early
shoulder extension movement. At the elbow, cerebellar subjects often
flexed too much when they made OS errors and flexed too little when
they made US errors (Fig. 3A,
). Elbow movements in the
OS and US categories were significantly different from each other, and
from all other categories (all P < 0.0005). In the
shoulder fixed condition, cerebellar subjects flexed the elbow too much
when they made OS errors and too little when they made US errors (Fig.
3B,
). Elbow movements in the OS and US categories were
significantly different from each other, and from all other categories
(all P < 0.0005).
|
|
TORQUE ANALYSIS. The cerebellar group produced the most abnormal torque magnitudes in the shoulder free condition. The major abnormality was at the shoulder joint when they made OS errors. Figure 5 shows single trials of torques (normalized to body weight) and EMG from a control subject and a cerebellar subject in the OS category. Trials are matched for peak tangential wrist velocity. The control subject's shoulder muscle torque is nearly equal and opposite the interaction torque in shape, and offset by an amount that counteracts the nearly constant gravitational torque (Fig. 5A). Because of this, the control subject produced a fairly flat net shoulder torque. The cerebellar subject produced a peak shoulder muscle torque that was too large and slightly too late to counter the interaction torque (Fig. 5B). This abnormality occurred within the first 100 ms of the movement. Because of this early imbalance, cerebellar subjects produced a substantial net shoulder torque, and flexed excessively at the shoulder. Also illustrated in Fig. 5 are the EMG patterns recorded from the same trials. Note that the control subject produced an initial burst of activity in the AD that coincided in time with the initial burst of activity in the BI and BR (Fig. 5C). The cerebellar subject produced more tonic levels of AD activity that did not coincide with the initiation of BI/BR activity (Fig. 5D). Analysis of EMG changes from the shoulder free to shoulder fixed conditions will be presented in a later section.
|
|
|
|
Torque magnitude and rate as a function of velocity
It is possible that our analysis of torques by error category could have failed to uncover a velocity-dependent impairment of torque generation in the cerebellar group. To test this, we compared the 1) peak flexor muscle torque magnitudes and 2) flexor muscle torque rates of control and cerebellar subjects moving at comparable velocities. We used the peak tangential wrist velocity as an index for dividing cerebellar and control trials into five different velocity categories: 40-70 cm/s, 71-100 cm/s, 101-130 cm/s, 131-160 cm/s, and 161+ cm/s. We then determined whether the elbow and shoulder torque magnitudes and rates were comparable in the different velocity categories across groups.
Figure 8A shows that control subjects scaled elbow and shoulder muscle torque magnitudes similarly with increasing velocity. Figure 8B shows that cerebellar subjects generated greater muscle torques at the shoulder compared with the elbow across velocity categories. This was most pronounced in the higher velocity categories, with the peak shoulder muscle torque greatly exceeding the peak elbow muscle torque. ANOVA showed that the difference between elbow and shoulder torque magnitudes was greater in the cerebellar group compared with controls (P < 0.001). Two findings were responsible for elbow-shoulder torque mismatch: 1) cerebellar subjects produced greater shoulder muscle torques than controls (P < 0.005) and 2) cerebellar subjects produced lower peak elbow muscle torques than controls (P < 0.05).
|
Figure 8C shows that control subjects also scaled elbow and shoulder muscle torque rates similarly with increasing velocity. In contrast, Fig. 8D shows that cerebellar subjects produced higher torque rates at the shoulder compared with the elbow, and this difference increased with increasing velocity. ANOVA showed that the difference between elbow and shoulder torque rates was greater in the cerebellar group compared with controls (P < 0.0001). This was due to the fact that cerebellar subjects produced lower elbow torque rates than controls (P < 0.001), and shoulder torque rates that were slightly higher or comparable to controls.
EMG differences between shoulder free and shoulder fixed conditions
As stated earlier, cerebellar endpoint error was reduced to near control levels when they moved in the shoulder fixed condition (78% of trials were within control variability). Cerebellar subjects also showed a striking difference in the magnitude of EMG activity in the AD muscle. Figure 9A shows a single EMG trace from a control subject and a cerebellar subject moving in both conditions. All traces are aligned on start of movement (time 0) and matched for peak tangential wrist velocity. The cerebellar subject shown is highly representative of the group, with prolonged AD activity (either tonic or multiple phasic bursts) in the shoulder free condition, and nearly absent AD activity in the shoulder fixed condition. Control subjects strongly activated the AD in both conditions, despite shoulder fixation in the second condition. Figure 9C shows the percent change in integrated EMG activity of the AD, BI, and BR from the shoulder free to shoulder fixed conditions. Averages are shown for the control group and cerebellar group. Note that the control subjects increased the magnitude of activity in all three muscles. Cerebellar subjects increased EMG magnitude in the BI muscles, showed little change in the BR, and greatly decreased activity in the AD. AD activity was decreased by 58% in cerebellar group and increased by 10% in the control group (P < 0.05). Reduced AD activity did not appear to be due to a general slowing of the reach in the cerebellar group. The average peak tangential wrist velocity for the entire cerebellar group decreased by about 12% from the shoulder free (105 ± 12 cm/s) to shoulder fixed (92 ± 10 cm/s) conditions. Controls produced comparable peak wrist velocities in both conditions (shoulder free 110 ± 7 cm/s; shoulder fixed 110 ± 9 cm/s).
|
We did not systematically analyze the magnitude of PD and TRI activity because these muscles were inconsistently active in both groups moving under both conditions (Table 2). We speculate that these extensor muscles were not active in many trials because the force of gravity provided a sufficient extensor torque to decelerate the limb at the end of the flexor movement. We also found that the BI and BR (elbow flexors) were the only muscles active for the majority (75%) of the cerebellar trials with no errors in the shoulder fixed condition. In contrast, more muscles (in addition to the elbow flexors) were active for five of six of the overshoot trials in the shoulder fixed condition.
![]() |
DISCUSSION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Many studies have investigated potential mechanisms for cerebellar
limb ataxia (Bastian et al. 1996; Brown et al.
1990
; Flament and Hore 1986
; Goodkin et
al. 1993
; Hallett et al. 1975
; Hallett and Massaquoi 1993
; Hore et al. 1991
;
Manto et al. 1994
; Massaquoi and Hallett
1996
; Topka et al. 1998a
,b
). One strategy has
been to study a specific aspect of ataxia (e.g., hypermetria,
dysdiadochokinesia) using a single jointed movement paradigm
(Brown et al.1990
; Flament and Hore 1986
;
Hallett et al. 1975
, 1991
; Hore et
al. 1991
; Manto et al. 1994
). Studies of single
jointed movements have revealed several parameters that the cerebellum
may control, including timing and/or amplitude scaling in either the
agonist, antagonist, or both muscle groups. However, no single deficit
uncovered can be generalized across studies to explain the different
features of ataxia. Instead, cerebellar damage appears to disrupt
different aspects of a movement depending on task conditions. The one
common feature across these studies is that the movement deficits were always attributed to an imbalance of agonist-antagonist activity.
An important feature of cerebellar control may be in predictively
scaling the relative activities of different muscles in relation to the
mechanical demands (e.g., inertia, interaction torques) during the
movement. If this is the case, then cerebellar ataxia should be more
pronounced during movements that require coordination of many muscles
(Thach et al. 1992a) and/or have complex mechanical
demands (Bastian et al. 1996
; Massaquoi and Hallett 1996
; Topka et al. 1998b
). Evidence from
both humans and monkeys indicate that cerebellar damage impairs
multi-jointed movements to a greater extent than would be expected
based on single jointed deficits (Goodkin et al. 1993
;
Thach et al. 1992a
,b
). Goodkin and colleagues reported a
cerebellar subject who could make relatively normal single-jointed
wrist movements, but had impaired multi-jointed reaching movements
(Goodkin et al. 1993
). Similarly, monkeys with
inactivations of the cerebellar dentate nucleus have been shown to have
mild impairments of single-jointed wrist movements, but gross
impairments of reaching movements (Thach et al. 1992b
).
In the present study, we found that cerebellar subjects can make nearly normal arm movements when they were constrained to move at only one joint in the shoulder fixed condition. This error reduction could not be explained by an appreciable slowing of the movement. Reduction of the most prevalent error type (overshoot) was due to a decrease in unwanted shoulder flexion movements caused by over-compensation for interaction torques. Reduction of undershoot errors was due to an increase in elbow flexion movements. We also found that, unlike controls, cerebellar subjects substantially reduced shoulder muscle activity when moving in the shoulder fixed condition. We speculate that the improvement in the cerebellar groups performance in the shoulder fixed condition could be due to the absence of interaction torques, reduction of the number of muscles to be controlled, or a combination of these two factors.
Interaction torques: under- versus over-compensation
Interaction torques are a mechanical complexity inherent in
multi-jointed movements. It has been suggested that the role of the
cerebellum may be in generating muscle torques that predict and
compensate for interaction torques (Bastian et al. 1996;
Schweighofer et al. 1998a
,b
). This is supported by the
finding that subjects with cerebellar damage have difficulty adjusting
their motor output to offset interaction torques during fast, reaching
movements (Bastian et al. 1996
; Topka et al.
1998b
). In these studies, cerebellar subjects produced
muscle torques that under-compensated for the interaction torques that
occurred late in the movement. Because of this under-compensation,
interaction torques contributed more to the net torque and determined
the course of the movement. This resulted in target overshoot and
abnormalities in elbow-shoulder coordination (Bastian et al.
1996
).
When compared with controls, cerebellar subjects also are impaired when
volitionally stabilizing the shoulder joint while moving the elbow
joint (Boose et al. 1999). In a recent study, subjects
were asked to flex only the elbow to a remembered final position (no
visual target) with both elbow and shoulder joints unconstrained. Both
controls and cerebellar subjects were unable to volitionally maintain a
static shoulder position. This was especially true of cerebellar
subjects who tended to move the shoulder more than controls. Unwanted
shoulder movement increased with higher velocities of elbow movement.
When compared with controls, cerebellar subjects also had a lower
correlation between the unwanted shoulder movement and the interaction
torque occurring at the shoulder (Boose et al. 1999
).
The authors speculated that the cerebellar subjects could not generate
sufficient phasic shoulder muscle torque, based on the velocity
dependence of the unwanted shoulder movement. However, the magnitude
and rate of muscle torque production was not assessed. If the
cerebellar subjects' only problem was insufficient muscle torques in
countering interaction torques (i.e., under-compensation), one would
have expected a higher correlation between the unwanted shoulder
movement and the interaction torque. This did not appear to be the case.
We have found that cerebellar subjects can over- or under-compensate
for interaction torques, depending on the demands of the task. During
our previous study of fast reaching, the deficit that we observed was
that of under-compensation for interaction torques (Bastian et
al. 1996). In the present study, we have observed that,
compared with controls, cerebellar subjects often over-compensate for
interaction torques at the shoulder, producing excessive shoulder flexion. This was due to the fact that, compared with controls, cerebellar subjects produced larger shoulder muscle torques at higher
rates than those produced at the elbow. This torque "mismatch" became most apparent at higher velocities of movement and appeared to
be the primary cause of the deficit. This mismatch also occurred early
in the movement (within the 1st 100 ms). Finally, we found timing
differences for peak muscle and interaction torques at the shoulder
joint between the control and cerebellar groups. These results indicate
that the primary cerebellar deficit may not simply be deficiency of
phasic muscle torques. Instead, we suggest that the deficit is an
inability to produce the appropriate muscle torque for the expected
interaction torques. It remains to be determined why over-compensation
for interaction torques occurs during some movements, and
under-compensation during others.
Control of multiple muscles
Cerebellar subjects produced irregular EMG patterns, often with prolonged muscle activity that was great or lesser in magnitude compared with controls. One of the striking findings in the present study was the difference in EMG patterns produced by controls and cerebellar subjects moving in the shoulder fixed condition. For controls, mechanically stabilizing the shoulder did not dramatically change the pattern of muscle activity. Control subjects typically activated the shoulder flexor (AD) concurrently with the elbow flexors (BI, BR) in both the shoulder free and shoulder fixed conditions. In contrast, for the cerebellar subjects, mechanically stabilizing the shoulder greatly altered the pattern of muscle activity used by the cerebellar subjects. In the shoulder fixed condition, cerebellar subjects reduced or ceased activity in the shoulder flexor (AD), but continued to strongly activate the elbow flexors (BI, BR). The reduction in AD activity was most often to resting baseline levels, and almost always present on the first test trial. We do not know whether it was diminished on a trial-by-trial basis during the three to five unrecorded test trials in the shoulder fixed condition.
We suspect that the reduced shoulder muscle activity observed in the shoulder fixed condition could be explained by two possible mechanisms. One possibility is that the reduced activity of shoulder muscles represents a strategy used by cerebellar subjects to further simplify control of the reaching movement. During reaching, multiple joints and multiple muscles must be controlled to hit the target. We imposed one simplification on the reaching movement by constraining the number of joints to be controlled in the shoulder fixed condition. However, the pattern of muscle activity was still free to vary, even though activity at the shoulder would have no effect on the movement. We speculate that the cerebellar subjects may have reduced activity of all muscles except the elbow flexors to further simplify control of the reach in the shoulder fixed condition. However, they would still have to combine the appropriate levels of activity in the two elbow flexors that were active (BI and BR). Errors in this combination could lead to endpoint errors in the shoulder fixed condition.
A second possibility is that cerebellar subjects had a loss of
associated muscle activity due to the cerebellar disease (Holmes 1939). If this were the case, the shoulder free condition would require cerebellar subjects to attempt a volitional muscle activation at the shoulder to compensate for the interaction torques. This volitional mechanism may not be appropriate to account for the interaction torques at the shoulder, and the cerebellar subjects' attempted shoulder stabilization would fail. In the shoulder fixed condition, the loss of associated shoulder muscle activity would not
interfere with the performance of the elbow movement, and the movement
would be performed more normally.
Given the time course of our subject's disease, we expect that we are measuring both the movement deficits and compensatory behavior in these individuals. Because of this, it is difficult to know whether the reduction in shoulder muscle activity during the shoulder fixed condition is a result of a learned compensation strategy, or the primary deficit. Future studies of subjects with very acute lesions may help to more clearly distinguish between these two possibilities.
Cerebellar contributions to arm movement
Based on this and other studies, we suggest that the cerebellum is
involved in scaling the relative activities of multiple muscles to
account for various forces (including inertial and interaction torques)
that oppose or assist the intended movement (Bastian et al.
1996; Schweighofer et al. 1998a
,b
). This
contribution would be critical to ensure that the limb segments are
moved at the correct rate and extent relative to one another. This type of control would also have to be predictive in nature, given that many
movements are too fast for feedback-dependent processes to operate.
Several lines of evidence are consistent with these ideas. First,
cerebellar subjects have difficulty scaling the relative activities of
agonist and antagonist muscles to move a single segment (Flament
and Hore 1986
; Hallett et al. 1975
; Hore
et al. 1991
). These single joint deficits worsen when inertia
is added to the moving segment (Manto et al. 1994
).
Second, much more striking deficits are observed when multiple muscles
are used to control a multi-jointed reaching movement. Multi-jointed
movement deficits are more than one would expect given the single joint
deficits, implying that the single joint deficits are not simply
summing. We believe that the additional mechanical complexity of
interaction torques during multi-jointed movements may account for this
difference. Finally, we suggest that a more general inability to
produce phasic muscle activity cannot explain these deficits. The
current study demonstrates that cerebellar errors in multi-jointed
movements can be due to excessive muscle torques produced at comparable rates compared with controls. Thus cerebellar patients can produce muscle torques that under- or over-compensate for interaction torques
during multi-jointed movements.
![]() |
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS |
---|
We thank E. Connor and C. Lang for helpful comments and discussions. We also thank A. Dromerick, J. Mink, J. Perlmutter, and B. Racette for patient referral.
This work was supported by National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grants NS-01199 to A. J. Bastian and NS-12777 to W. T. Thach.
![]() |
FOOTNOTES |
---|
Address for reprint requests: A. J. Bastian, Program in Physical Therapy, Washington University School of Medicine, 4444 Forest Park Blvd., Rm. 1101, Campus Box 8502, St. Louis, MO 63108.
The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. The article must therefore be hereby marked "advertisement" in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
Received 15 September 1999; accepted in final form 15 February 2000.
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|