1 Department of Biokinesiology, 2 Department of Neurology, and 3 PET Imaging Science Center, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California 90033
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Winstein, Carolee J., Scott T. Grafton, and Patricia S. Pohl. Motor task difficulty and brain activity: investigation of goal-directed reciprocal aiming using positron emission tomography. J. Neurophysiol. 77: 1581-1594, 1997. Differences in the kinematics and pattern of relative regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) during goal-directed arm aiming were investigated with the use of a Fitts continuous aiming paradigm with three difficulty conditions (index of difficulty, ID) and two aiming types (transport vs. targeting) in six healthy right-handed young participants with the use of video-based movement trajectory analysis and positron emission tomography. Movement time and kinematic characteristics were analyzed together with the magnitude of cerebral blood flow to identify areas of brain activity proportionate to task and movement variables. Significant differences in rCBF between task conditions were determined by analysis of variance with planned comparisons of means with the use of group mean weighted linear contrasts. Data were first analyzed for the group. Then individual subject differences for the movement versus no movement and task difficulty comparisons were related to each individual subjects' anatomy by magnetic resonance imaging. Significant differences in rCBF during reciprocal aiming compared with no-movement conditions were found in a mosaic of well-known cortical and subcortical areas associated with the planning and execution of goal-directed movements. These included cortical areas in the left sensorimotor, dorsal premotor, and ventral premotor cortices, caudal supplementary motor area (SMA) proper, and parietal cortex, and subcortical areas in the left putamen, globus pallidus, red nucleus, thalamus, and anterior cerebellum. As aiming task difficulty (ID) increased, rCBF increased in areas associated with the planning of more complex movements requiring greater visuomotor processing. These included bilateral occipital, left inferior parietal, and left dorsal cingulate corticescaudal SMA proper and right dorsal premotor area. These same areas showed significant increases or decreases, respectively, when contrast means were compared with the use of movement time or relative acceleration time, respectively, as the weighting factor. Analysis of individual subject differences revealed a correspondence between the spatial extent of rCBF changes as a function of task ID and the individuals' movement times. As task ID decreased, significant increases in rCBF were evident in the right anterior cerebellum, left middle occipital gyrus, and right ventral premotor area. Functionally, these areas are associated with aiming conditions in which the motor execution demands are high (i.e., coordination of rapid reversals) and precise trajectory planning is minimal. These same areas showed significant increases or decreases, respectively, when contrast means were compared with the use of movement time or relative acceleration time, respectively, as the weighting factor. A functional dissociation resulted from the weighted linear contrasts between larger (limb transport) or smaller (endpoint targeting) type amplitude/target width aiming conditions. Areas with significantly greater rCBF for targeting were the left motor cortex, left intraparietal sulcus, and left caudate. In contrast, those areas with greater rCBFassociated with limb transport included bilateral occipital lingual gyri and the right anterior cerebellum. Various theoretical explanations for the speed/accuracy tradeoffs of rapid aiming movements have been proposed since the original information theory hypothesis of Fitts. This is the first report to relate the predictable variations in motor control under changing task constraints with the functional anatomy of these rapid goal-directed aiming movements. Differences in unimanual aiming task difficulty lead to dissociable activation of cortical-subcortical networks. Further, these data suggest that when more precise targeting is required, independent of task difficulty, a cortical-subcortical loop composed of the contralateral motor cortex, intraparietal sulcus, and caudate is activated. This is consistent with the role of motor cortex for controlling direction of movement on the basis of population encoding.
Chronometric approaches to the study of the control of goal-directed hand aiming movements show that the precision or complexity requirements of the aiming task can be generally described through the effect that the index of difficulty (ID) has on movement time (Fitts 1954
INTRODUCTION
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
; Fitts and Peterson 1964
). Movement time increases as the width of the target decreases or when the distance to the target increases. The formulation of Fitts (1954)
for this linear relationship is given by the following equation
where MT is the movement time between two targets for discrete aiming or average movement time for continuous,reciprocal aiming, and a and b are empirically derivedconstants. In Fitts' law, the ID is represented by the term[Log2 (2A/W)], where A is the distance between the center of each target and W is the width of each target in the plane of movement. The time dependency characterizes the speed-accuracy tradeoff for rapid, goal-directed aiming movements. This highly reproducible movement time effect is in part an outcome of increasing motor planning and requisite visual feedback as task constraints increase and accuracy is maintained (Keele 1968
(1)
; Meyer et al. 1988
; Wallace and Newell 1983
; see Keele 1986
for a review).
; Georgopoulos et al. 1986
; Schwartz et al. 1988
; for review see Georgopoulos 1995
). These studies have not examined neural activity in relation to a task dimension such as the ID. Previous positron emission tomography (PET) studies in humans also implicate dorsal premotor, motor, and superior parietal cortex for control of reaching, grasping, or pointing movements (Grafton et al. 1992
, 1996b
). These experiments are typically categorical comparisons of movement versus control/no movement. As in the electrophysiology studies, a parametric analysis in which task difficulty was the parameter of interest was not examined. A primary goal of this experiment was to determine the functional neural anatomy associated with the control of continuous goal-directed aiming movements. In addition to completing a categorical comparison of movement versus no movement, the experiment was primarily designed to examine the effect of task complexity by testing subjects at different IDs. Differences along this dimension would identify areas more involved in planning as task constraints of directed arm movements increase.
). We used high-speed video recordings to characterize the kinematics of these arm aiming movements. For the purposes of PET imaging, the Fitts aiming task was modified only slightly so that blocks of continuous, reciprocal aiming movements were executed in the vertical dimension. In a prior PET study of limb pointing (Grafton et al. 1996b
) we observed a correlation of blood flow increases in motor areas with the time required to complete each reaching movement (i.e., mean limb velocity since the reaching distance was constant). However, blood flow increases in these same areas also correlated inversely with the total amount of movement made during each scan. This was because subjects that reached quickly spent more dwell time waiting for the next go cue in this task with a fixed intertrial interval. Thus a direct relationship of the kinematic parameter of interest (limb velocity) and cerebral blood flow (CBF) responses could not be established. Therefore in the current study the task was controlled so that the proportion of time with movements (and also dwell time) remained constant across all scans. Brain areas with blood flow responses proportionate to the average time to complete a reaching movement, peak velocity, number of hits, and time to peak velocity (acceleration) could then be identified. We were particularly interested to see which of these dependent measures corresponded best to CBF increases in motor areas. We did not attempt to dissociate eye and limb movements. Instead, oculomotor and limb control were treated as a unitary functional system.
![]() |
METHODS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Subjects
Six right-handed (Oldfield 1971) young adults (23 ± 3.8 yr, mean ± SD; 3 male, 3 female) participated in the study after informed consent was obtained in accordance with the Institutional Review Board of the University of Southern California. All subjects were normal by medical interview.
Apparatus and behavioral task
Equipment for the aiming task included a lightweight hand-held stylus with a globe-shaped reflective marker mounted 10 mm from the tip, a millisecond timer that emitted an audible tone at the end of a trial, and a vertically mounted tapping board with metal plates distinguishing two targets, surrounded by error regions. Video recording (Panasonic AG7350) of the trajectory of the stylus was performed with a Pulnix 120-Hz video camera. A 300-W floodlamp was directed toward the stylus reflective marker during filming. Video analysis equipment included Peak Performance Technologies software and hardware with a Unipaq 386 computer interfaced with the video recorder.
). At each ID there were two task types (i.e., amplitude/target width combinations) resulting in six aiming (movement) conditions. Amplitude/target width combinations were 10/4 and 20/8 cm for the lowest task difficulty (ID = 2.32 bits); 10/1 and 20/2 cm for the moderate-difficulty (ID = 4.32 bits); and 10/0.25 and 20/0.50 cm for the highest-difficulty(ID = 6.32 bits).
Scanning procedure
Each subject was positioned supine with the head in the scanner and the right arm free to move in a vertical direction, as shown in Fig. 1. The tapping board was mounted vertically to the right, within view and within reach of the right arm. The video camera was positioned orthogonal to the plane of movement (Fig. 1, inset). Movement of the stylus during each 90-s scan was filmed at a rate of 120 Hz. During each 90-s movement scan, the subject performed five 10-s continuous aiming trials interpolated with 5-s rests. A tone sounded after each 10-s trial, triggering the 5-s rest period. The experimenter prompted each subsequent trial with a verbal cue. Although a wide range of movement speeds was generated across task conditions and subjects, each movement scan included the same proportion of limb movement and rest.
|
10 min apart to allow for sufficient radioactive decay of 15O.
Imaging
PET images of regional CBF (rCBF) were acquired with the Siemens 953/A tomograph. The device collects 31 contiguous planes covering a 105-mm field of view. The nominal axial resolution is 4.3 mm at full width half maximum (FWHM) and the transaxial resolution is 5.5 mm FWHM as measured with a line source. The tomograph was oriented 15° more steeply than the canthomeatal line, so the field of view did not include the orbitofrontal cortex.
; Raichle et al. 1983
). A bolus of H215O was injected intravenously into the left arm commensurate with the start of scanning. The movement task was initiated 10 s after tracer injection. A 90-s scan was acquired and reconstructed with the use of calculated attenuation correction, with boundaries derived from each emission scan sinogram. Arterial blood samples were not obtained. Images of radioactive counts were used to estimate rCBF as described previously (Fox et al. 1984
; Mazziotta et al. 1985
).
Kinematics analysis
The position of the reflective marker during the three middle trials (2-4) of each movement scan was digitized and resultanttwo-dimensional coordinates were determined. Data weresmoothed with the use of a Butterworth low-pass filter (8 Hz cutoff), and velocities were derived. Trajectory data were analyzed by cycle (liftoff to liftoff). Data from each cycle were averaged within and then across trials for each of the six task conditions to determine the number of target hits per 10-s trial, average movement time per cycle (MT), peak resultant velocity per cycle, and the percent of MT in acceleration per cycle [i.e., (time to peak velocity/cycle MT) × 100].
rCBF analysis
Image processing was performed on a SUN 10-41 SPARC workstation. All rCBF images were aligned in a common stereotactic reference frame to determine a mean image for each subject. First, a within-subject alignment of PET scans was performed with the use of an automated registration algorithm (Woods et al. 1992). A mean image of the registered and resliced images was calculated for each subject. The mean PET image from each individual was coregistered to the same subject's MRI scan with the use of an automated algorithm (Woods et al. 1993a
). MRI scans were then coregistered to a reference atlas centered in Talairach coordinates with the use of an affine transformation with 12 degrees of freedom (Grafton et al. 1994
; Talairach and Tournoux 1988
; Woods et al. 1993
b). The parameters to fit were three translations, three rotations, and three rescalers oriented in a direction specified by the last three parameters. Combined registration matrices were then used to reslice all raw PET data to the final Talairach coordinate system. The resultant rCBF images were masked at a threshold of 10% of maximum; areas above this cutoff were smoothed to a final isotropic resolution of 20 mm FWHM (as verified with a line source). Previous investigations demonstrate that this magnitude of smoothing enhances signal detection (Friston et al. 1991
; Grafton et al. 1990
; Worsley et al. 1992
). All 72 (12 scans × 6 subjects) smoothed images were normalized to each other with the use of proportionate scaling calculated from the global activity of each scan. Normalization was performed with the use of a common volume mask, to avoid global normalization errors associated with missing data.
; Woods et al. 1996
). To account for the between-subjects variance, a randomized blocking design with subjects as a blocking effect was used for all comparisons. A t map image of significant effects was calculated on a pixel-by-pixel basis by weighting the scans as a function of a particular dependent variable of interest. Peak sites on the t map above a threshold of P < 0.005 were localized and maximal t and P values and mean rCBF values were tabulated for each comparison. The following six comparisons were evaluated.
MOVEMENT.
A three-way ANOVA was used to identify areas demonstrating a categorical difference between directed arm movements and no arm movement with the use of all 72 scans. The three effects were task (n = 2, movement scans were weighted 1 vs. no-movement scans weighted 1), amplitude/target condition (n = 6), and subject (n = 6). A threshold was set at t = 2.787, P < 0.005, df = 25.
TASK DIFFICULTY. A three-way ANOVA was used to identify areas demonstrating different activity as a function of task difficulty. Only the scans during movement were used (n = 36). The three effects in this statistical model were ID (n = 3, each ID scan was weighted by the derived Fitts ID level), type of difficulty(n = 2), and subject (n = 6). A threshold was set for t = 3.169,P < 0.005, df = 10. Sites with greater or lesser CBF corresponding to task difficulty were then localized.
TYPE OF DIFFICULTY: TRANSPORT/TARGETING.
A three-way ANOVA was used to identify areas more active as a function of type of difficulty. The "type" of difficulty referred to either the smaller (10/4, 10/1, 10/0.25 cm) or the larger (20/8, 20/2, 20/0.5 cm) amplitude/target width ID conditions. The three effects and t and P thresholds in this statistical model were the same as those used for the task difficulty comparison, except that the two task type scans were weighted 1 and 1. Sites where there were CBF changes in association with the use of relatively smaller or larger amplitude/target ratios were then localized. Sites with greater rCBF in association with smaller amplitude/target combinations correspond to areas involved in planning more precise aiming (endpoint targeting), whereas those showing greater relative rCBF in association with larger amplitude/target combinations correspond to areas more involved in limb movement (transport).
Movement time and kinematics
Movement time and kinematic results are summarized in Table 1 and Fig. 2 and are presented first to illustrate the degree of correlation with task difficulty (ID) and type of aiming. More importantly, these results are presented because they provide the basis for the predictions regarding the CBF data.
AIMING TASK DIFFICULTY (ID) INFLUENCES MT AND KINEMATICS IN A PREDICTABLE MANNER.
As task difficulty increases (from 2.32 to 6.32 bits), average MT increases from 237 to 1,145 ms/cycle, representing approximately a fivefold change (P < 0.003). Individual subject MTs are presented in Table 1 to illustrate the consistency of the MT change with ID across subjects, and the between-subject variability in the rate of that change (i.e., MT/ID slope). Subject 2 shows the largest effect of increasing task difficulty on MT, whereas subject 1 shows the smallest.
AIMING TASK TYPE INFLUENCES MT AND KINEMATICS.
In contrast to the predictions of Fitts' law (Fitts 1954 rCBF
MOVEMENT VERSUS NO MOVEMENT.
This categorical comparison identifies brain regions demonstrating increasedactivity during continuous reciprocal aiming (Table 2). Consistent with previous functional imaging studies, CBF increased in well-known areas associated with the planning and execution of goal-directed arm movements (Grafton et al. 1992
EFFECT OF INCREASING TASK DIFFICULTY (ID) AND RELATED KINEMATICS.
Table 3, bottom right, summarizes the results of the t map contrasts that identify areas of increasing rCBF in association with increasing ID or MT. Functionally, activation in these areas is most likely associated with the visuomotor demands of aiming as accuracy constraints are increased. When contrast means were compared with the use of ID as the weighting factor, there were significant increases of rCBF in bilateral occipital (fusiform gyri), left inferior parietal, and left mesial frontal cortex, and right dorsal premotor areas (Fig. 4, top). The mesial frontal response extended into both the dorsal cingulate cortex and caudal SMA proper. Because separate maxima could not be identified in these two sites, this activation is described as the dorsal cingulate/caudal SMA proper site. The results from the individual subject analysis of the ID effect are shown in Fig. 5. The right premotor and left dorsal cingulate/caudal SMA proper activations could be readily identified in four of five subjects and the rostral inferior parietal activations could be localized in five of five subjects.
EFFECT OF DECREASING TASK DIFFICULTY (ID) AND RELATED KINEMATICS.
Table 3, top right, summarizes the results of the t map contrasts where decreasing ID was associated with significant increases in blood flow. There were significant increases in right anterior cerebellum, left middle occipital gyrus, and right ventral premotor areas (Fig. 4, bottom). As predicted, these same areas showed significant increases of rCBF when contrast means were compared with the use of MT as the weighting factor. Functionally, activation in these areas is associated with aiming where the motor execution demands are relatively high (e.g., rapid reversals; see below), the trajectory planning demands are minimal, and the task requires the coordination of rapid alternating movements without the kind of visuomotor integration required of precise targeting.
TARGETING VERSUS LIMB TRANSPORT.
Fitts' law (Fitts 1954
The control of rapid target-directed movements has been of interest since before the time of the well-known review by Woodworth (1899) The authors thank S. Hailes, K. Hamley, and B. Fisher for technical assistance, and Dr. Roger Woods for providing image analysis software.
This work was supported in part by grants from the Human Frontiers in Science Program, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grants NS-01568 and NS-33504 to S. Grafton, and The Foundation for Physical Therapy to C. Winstein.
Present addresses: S. Grafton, Dept. of Neurology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30303; P. Pohl, Center on Aging, The University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, KS 66160.
Address for reprint requests: C. Winstein, Dept. of Biokinesiology and Physical Therapy, 1540 E. Alcazar St., CHP 155, Los Angeles, CA 90033. Received 19 April 1996; accepted in final form 1 November 1996.
View this table:
TABLE 1.
Kinematic variables for the group and by subject for movement time
RESULTS
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
View larger version (29K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 2.
Phase plane plots of horizontal position (cm) by velocity (cm/s) for 1 of each of the 10-s trials in the 6 aiming task conditions for 1 subject (subject 4). Labels correspond to the amplitude/target width (cm) constraints; thus plots from left to right are the 3 index of difficulty (ID) conditions (2.32, 4.32, 6.32 bits) and rows represent the task type conditions (top: large; bottom: small). Arrows in the top left plot: direction of movement. The trajectory above the 0-velocity line indicates movement outward away from the target (X,Y = 0,0) and the trajectory below the line indicates movement inward toward the target. Note the prevalence of 0-velocity crossings during the inward, homing-in phase for both high-ID condition (right) and the medium ID-small type condition (10/1 cm), where more precise targeting is required.
1·cycle
1, representing a nearly twofold reduction in speed (P < 0.0005). Finally, the percentage of MT spent in acceleration (time to peak resultant velocity) decreases as task difficulty increases (P < 0.0001). In other words, the time spent in the deceleration phase as the target is approached increases from 49% to 80%, representing an increase of >1.5-fold. Thus, as predicted by Fitts' law (Fitts 1954
), MT increases linearly as ID increases (r = 0.82, P < 0.0001). Further, and consistent with previous work, percent acceleration time covaries reciprocally with ID (MacKenzie et al. 1987
; Milner and Izaz 1990; Pohl et al. 1996
; Winstein and Pohl 1995
). Change in peak velocity also covaries reciprocally with ID, but has been shown to be more affected by changes in movement amplitude (see results below) and thus would be expected to covary negatively with movement type in this paradigm (MacKenzie et al. 1987
). Therefore we predict that brain areas showing increased activation with increasing task complexity (ID) would also show activation patterns that covary positively with MT and covary negatively with percent acceleration time. Conversely, those brain areas showing increased activation with decreasing task complexity would show activation patterns that also covary negatively with MT and positively with percent acceleration time.
), there was a significant ID-by-type (small vs. large amplitude/target width) interaction such that in the 4.32 ID condition where the amplitude/target width ratios were the same, MT was longer (140-ms difference) in the small relative to the large type condition (P < 0.04). Similarly, in the 4.32 ID condition, the relative acceleration time in the small type condition was less (37%) than that for the large type condition (44%), thus allowing a larger proportion of MT for targeting in the deceleration phase in the small type condition. In contrast, MT and kinematic measures for the other two ID conditions at the extremes of task difficulty were not influenced by task type (P > 0.05). These quantitative differences can be seen qualitatively in the phase plane plots of the horizontal movement component from individual trials (Fig. 2). Data from one representative subject show obvious zero-velocity crossings during the deceleration phase in the two high-ID (6.32) conditions (right) and the small type medium-ID (4.32) condition (bottom middle). These zero-velocity crossings reflect discrete time-consuming adjustments in the trajectory during the target approach phase just before target impact.
for similar findings). Therefore we predict that brain areas showing increased activation with the small (type) amplitude/target width conditions would also show activation patterns that covary negatively with peak velocity.
; Roland et al. 1982
). Coregistered MRI anatomic data were available for five of the subjects. The locations of cortical activation sites with respect to local gyral anatomy in these five subjects are shown in Fig. 3. Common responses are identifiable in left sensorimotor, dorsal and ventral premotor, mesial frontal, and parietal cortex. Two sites could be distinguished in the mesial frontal area, one centered in the dorsal cingulate cortex and the other in the caudal portion of the supplementary motor area (SMA) proper. On the lateral surface, note the close proximity of responses in precentral, central, and postcentral sulci near the somatotopic arm area in all five subjects. These responses were also identifiable in a different set of subjects performing a reach and grasp task reported previously (Grafton et al. 1996b
). An activation in ventral precentral gyrus or adjacent precentral sulcus (ventral premotor cortex) could be identified in four of the five subjects. Involvement of the intraparietal sulcus was less consistent, with three subjects showing relative increases in rostral intraparietal sulcus and three subjects showing increases in caudal intraparietal sulcus.
View this table:
TABLE 2.
Movement-related brain areas during reciprocal aiming
View larger version (148K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 3.
Movement vs. no movement. A categorical comparison of all movement scans with no-movement control scans was performed for each subject separately. Rows correspond to S1-S5 of Table 1. Pixels reaching a threshold of P < 0.05 are displayed in red in 3 dimensions on the same subject's coregistered magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans. For all subjects, the superior views (left) localize responses in the inter hemispheric fissure [caudal supplementary motor area (SMA) proper]. Across all subjects there is consistent activation of the central sulcus and adjacent pre- and postcentral sulcus in the somatotopic region of the arm area of motor cortex, best seen on the left superior oblique views (middle). Additional activations are noted in the superior parietal lobule for all subjects. The lateral views (right) identify an inferior precentral gyrus/sulcus response (ventral premotor cortex) in subjects 1, 3, and 5. Lateral occipital responses can also be identified in the same subjects.
). As in many previous PET studies, cerebellar responses were maximal in ipsilateral anterior lobe parasagittal cerebellum. The response was large and extended into underlying cerebellar nuclei.
View this table:
TABLE 3.
Task difficulty and kinematic related changes in brain activity
View larger version (125K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 4.
Functional anatomy of Fitts' ID. Top: brain areas with greater activity during performance of reciprocal aiming under a greater ID. Differences are present in bilateral occipital cortex (left, Z = 10 mm), left parietal cortex (middle, Z = 34 mm), and cingulate cortex (right, Z = 46 mm). Bottom: brain areas with greater activity during performance of the reciprocal aiming under a lower ID. Differences are present in right anterior cerebellum (left, Z =
12 mm), left dorsal occipital cortex (middle,Z = 8 mm), and right inferior precentral gyrus, ventral premotor cortex (right, Z = 18 mm). Positron emission tomography (PET) results (P < 0.005) are superimposed on a population average MRI atlas in Talairach coordinates.
View larger version (146K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 5.
Individual subject differences in cerebral blood flow (CBF) for increasing task difficulty (ID). Areas showing greater regional CBF (rCBF) as a function of increasing ID are shown in red on the same subjects' MRI scans. Left: superior view; arrows indicate location of right premotor cortex response identifiable in 4/5 subjects. Middle: left superior oblique view; significant response is seen in the left ventral premotor cortex in subjects 1 and 2; however, this site was not significant in the more stringent group analysis. Middle and right (left lateral view) demonstrate in all 5 subjects a consistent response located in the left rostral inferior parietal lobule (indicated by blue arrows in right). There is a disparity in the relative spatial extent of the rCBF differences across subjects. rCBF changes in subject 2 (row 2) are qualitatively larger than all of the other subjects. This subject had the greatest difficulty in performing the reciprocal aiming task (particularly in the higher-ID conditions).
). The cortex anterior to the axis has been termed "pre-SMA" (Picard and Strick 1996
; Tanji 1994
). These four areas showed changes in rCBF only with respect to peak velocity of these aiming movements.
View this table:
TABLE 4.
Localization of brain areas processing transport and targeting
) predicts that aiming movements performed under conditions with the same amplitude/target width ratio should exhibit the same MTs, but no predictions are made about requisite motor planning and associated neural activation patterns. Our experimental design allowed us to dissociate planning related to limb transport from that related to endpoint targeting across the range of IDs. Table 4 summarizes the results of the weighted linear contrasts, showing areas with CBF changes in association with larger (limb transport) or smaller (endpoint targeting) type amplitude/target width combinations. Areas that showed significantly greater activity with limb transport included bilateral occipital lingual gyri and the right anterior cerebellum. In contrast, the three areas with significantly greater activity for end reach targeting were located in the left motor cortex, left intraparietal sulcus, and left caudate. These cortical areas are illustrated in relation to local gyral anatomy in Fig. 6.
View larger version (114K):
[in a new window]
FIG. 6.
Cortical areas for control of endpoint targeting. A comparison of the 2 target/width ratios across the 3 levels of movement difficulty was used to dissociate difficulty related to endpoint targeting (shown in black) from that primarily related to limb transport. Accurate endpoint targeting is associated with greater relative activity in the left motor cortex (bottom arrow), centered at the fundus of the central sulcus and extending into the adjacent precentral sulcus (top arrow) and also in the left intraparietal sulcus.
DISCUSSION
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
, which describes the speed-accuracy tradeoffs in a variety of aiming tasks. Although movement scientists have long known that movement time depends both on the distance moved and on the endpoint precision as demanded by the size of the target, it was Fitts (1954)
who was the first to formally define this relationship in terms of information capacity in closed systems (see Kelso 1992
for recent perspectives). Numerous mathematical and theoretical explanations for Fitts' law have been offered since its inception, including the iterative-corrections model (Crossman and Goodeve 1983
; Keele 1968
), the impulse variability model (Schmidt et al. 1979
), the optimized initial impulse model (Meyer et al. 1988
), the vector-integration-to-endpoint (VITE) model (Bullock and Grossberg 1988
), and, most recently, the kinematic model based on a log-normal law (Plamondon 1995a
,b
). Although each new theoretical explanation has provided a more parsimonious and valid account of the mechanisms underlying the speed-accuracy tradeoff, the empirical law has survived the test of time across a variety of different aiming tasks, environmental conditions, and subject populations. Here, in our experimental design, we have used Fitts' law, the continuous aiming paradigm on which it was based, and the dimension of task difficulty as indexed by ID to identify brain areas involved in the planning of goal-directed limb movements. As Jeannerod (1994)
recently argued, "Fitts' law seems to pertain, not to the execution stage of movements (as this classical explanation would hold), but to the preparation stage. . . . The relation between duration and accuracy would thus result from neural coding of the movement during the preparatory stage. . . ." (p. 196).
) and movement selection tasks (e.g., Deiber et al. 1991
). The former assumes that the explicit operation of imagining movements is analogous to the planning of normal movements (which are typically performed without conscious thought). The latter experiments typically require working memory by subjects to retain conditional stimulus response maps. Again, this is a thoughtful process that diverges from the automatic and implicit planning of normal movements (see Jeannerod 1994
for recent discussion). Our experimental design identifies brain areas involved in motor planning under different levels of difficulty in a primarily procedural (implicit) task that requires little declarative (explicit) cognitive processing.
; Rao et al. 1993
) and neurophysiological recordings in nonhuman primates (e.g., Alexander and Crutcher 1990; Fu et al. 1993
; Riehle and Requin 1989
; Rizzolatti et al. 1987
). A new finding was an activation in ventral premotor cortex. This may be a putative homologue of area "F4" in monkeys (Rizzolatti et al. 1987
). Neurons in F4 are particularly active during reaching movements. Activation of subcortical regions including the putamen and globus pallidus has been inconsistently seen in previous PET experiments during goal-directed aiming movements. These responses are typically small in size and magnitude. With the greater "signal averaging" from a large number of trials in the present experiment, these sites could be readily identified. This is consistent with neurophysiological data from nonhuman primates (Alexander et al. 1990
; Georgopoulos et al. 1983
) implicating a motor circuit involving a cortical-subcortical network. Of further importance to an understanding of motor planning and the neural coding associated with changes in aiming task difficulty is that a different activation pattern emerges when comparisons are made within the movement conditions.
4.58 bits (Wallace and Newell 1983
). These results have been interpreted as evidence for the use of visual feedback for accuracy as ID increases. Further, this visual feedback time has been used to account for the prolongation in MT as ID increases. However, the task difficulty/MT relation has been demonstrated even in aiming conditions when vision is not allowed, suggesting that the increase in MT with ID is more a consequence of the motor plan than visual feedback processing time (e.g., Meyer et al. 1988
; Prablanc et al. 1979
; Schmidt et al. 1979
). Our activation pattern associated with increases in ID seems to support both arguments. First, we see a pronounced increase in bilateral fusiform gyrus (Brodmann's area 18) activation, suggesting an increase in visual information processing as ID increases. These visual areas are most likely at the origin of a dorsal route linking striate areas to posterior parietal (contralateral inferior parietal) areas known to be involved in visual-spatial integration for action (Goodale et al. 1991
; Taira et al. 1990
).
; Grafton et al. 1992
; Orgogozo and Larsen 1979
; Remy et al. 1994
; Shibasaki et al. 1993
). Furthermore, the dorsal cingulate/caudal SMA proper, rostral SMA proper, and pre-SMA sites have also been shown to have different activation patterns for real, imagined, and observed movements (Grafton et al. 1996a
; Stephan et al. 1995
; Tyszka et al. 1994
). The more rostral areas are associated with imagined movements, whereas the caudal areas are associated with real movements.
; Smith and Zernicke 1987
) even though the task demands for dealing with aiming accuracy are lower. Recent work with individuals with cerebellar disease suggests a role for the cerebellum in generating feedforward signals to adjust for interaction torques during reaching movements (Bastian et al. 1996
). Previous recordings of dentate neurons in nonhuman primates during an alternating target aiming task before and after temporary dentate cooling showed a decrease in continuous movements and inadequate modulation of force (Brooks et al. 1973
). Examination of the phase plane plots for the low-ID conditions (Fig. 2, left) reveals a remarkable predominance of continuous movement trajectories exhibiting pendular dynamics, with only two velocity crossings at peak outward displacement and target hit. Altogether, this suggests that the task demands in this low-ID condition are those for which the cerebellum and its unique cortical and deep nuclear circuitry is particularly suited. Of course, we cannot rule out the possibility that in the low-ID conditions the demands for continuous sensory updating might be higher than in the high-ID conditions. Recent proposals regarding the role of the cerebellum in the acquisition of sensory data (proprioceptive and cutaneous) cannot be ruled out from the present findings (Bower 1997
).
, 1995). With this, it is likely that in aiming situations in which more precise targeting is required, the motor cortex neurons are recruited to define this trajectory. We can only speculate as to whether a greater PET activation corresponds to more activity in a fixed number of neurons or whether a greater number of neurons is used to encode the intended movement. Given the covariation of movement velocity and force with ID, we expected that motor cortex activation would increase as force (velocity) increased. However, across ID conditions, the task type effect revealed a preferential activation in primary motor cortex for the lower-force (i.e., small type) aiming condition compared with the higher-force (i.e., large type) conditions. This comparison allowed a dissociation of the normal velocity/force covariation with ID and suggests that the primary motor cortex was preferentially activated in relation to the precise direction of force application and not simply the magnitude of force application.
to account for planned arm aiming movements uses the cell properties in the precentral motor cortex to provide continuous difference vector computations between the present position and the target position commands. With the use of this model, the speed-accuracy properties for aiming trajectories can be simulated. Consistent with our findings, this model suggests that where more precise targeting is required, the demands for this particular motor cortex cell computation would be greater.
).
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
FOOTNOTES
REFERENCES
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
0022-3077/97 $5.00 Copyright ©1997 The American Physiological Society