From the Department of Microbiology and Molecular Genetics, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, California 92697-4025
The role of the voltage sensor positive charges in fast and slow inactivation of the rat brain IIA sodium channel was investigated by mutating the second and fourth conserved positive charges in the S4 segments
of all four homologous domains. Both charge-neutralizing mutations (by glutamine substitution) and charge-conserving mutations were constructed in a cDNA encoding the sodium channel subunit. To determine if fast inactivation altered the effects of the mutations on slow inactivation, the mutations were also constructed in a channel
that had fast inactivation removed by the incorporation of the IFMQ3 mutation in the III-IV linker (West, J.W.,
D.E. Patton, T. Scheuer, Y. Wang, A.L. Goldin, and W.A. Catterall. 1992. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 89:10910-
10914). Most of the mutations shifted the v1/2 of fast inactivation in the negative direction, with the largest effects
resulting from mutations in domains I and II. These shifts were in the opposite direction compared with those observed for activation. The effects of the mutations on slow inactivation depended on whether fast inactivation was
intact or not. When fast inactivation was eliminated, most of the mutations resulted in positive shifts in the v1/2 of
slow inactivation. The largest effects again resulted from mutations in domains I and II. When fast inactivation was
intact, the mutations in domains II and III resulted in negative shifts in the v1/2 of slow inactivation. Neutralization
of the fourth charge in domain I or II resulted in the appearance of a second component in the voltage dependence of slow inactivation that was only observable when fast inactivation was intact. These results suggest the S4
regions of all four domains of the sodium channel are involved in the voltage dependence of inactivation, but to
varying extents. Fast inactivation is not strictly coupled to activation, but it derives some independent voltage sensitivity from the charges in the S4 domains. Finally, there is an interaction between the fast and slow inactivation processes.
The voltage-gated sodium channel is essential for the
initiation of action potentials in most electrically excitable cells. The channel undergoes at least two distinct
gating events, voltage-dependent activation and fast inactivation. Fast inactivation is thought to have only
slight voltage dependence, with the apparent voltage dependence resulting from the activation process (Bezanilla and Armstrong, 1977; Nonner, 1980
; Aldrich
and Stevens, 1987
; Zagotta and Aldrich, 1990
). Part of
the evidence for the lack of voltage dependence is that
fast inactivation is not associated with any component
of gating current, although it does immobilize about two-thirds of the activation gating charge (Armstrong
and Bezanilla, 1977
). Fast inactivation is thought to occur by a ball-and-chain-type mechanism (Armstrong
and Bezanilla, 1977
), in which a portion of the channel
on the cytoplasmic side occludes the pore after activation. This model has been verified for the Shaker potassium channel, in which the amino terminus functions
as the inactivating particle (Hoshi et al., 1990
; Zagotta
et al., 1990
). In the sodium channel, the cytoplasmic
linker connecting domains III and IV is essential for
fast inactivation, and it has been proposed that this
linker functions as the inactivating particle (Patton et al., 1992
; West et al., 1992
).
In addition to fast inactivation that occurs over milliseconds, sodium channels can be inactivated by processes that occur over a much longer time scale, from
hundreds of milliseconds to minutes (Simoncini and
Stühmer, 1987; Ruff et al., 1987
, 1988
; Ruben et al.,
1990
). Slow inactivation can be a critical determinant of membrane excitability by decreasing the availability
of sodium channels (Ruff et al., 1988
). Slow inactivation is voltage dependent (Ruff et al., 1987
; Ruben et
al., 1990
), and it is likely that the voltage dependence is
derived, at least in part, from voltage sensors that are
not as important for activation. Slow inactivation can
occur in the absence of fast inactivation, but the two
processes do interact (Featherstone et al., 1996
). Disorders of both fast and slow inactivation in either the skeletal muscle or cardiac sodium channel are the cause of
a number of human neurological disorders, including
hyperkalemic periodic paralysis, paramyotonia congenita, sodium channel myotonia (reviewed by Barchi,
1995
; Cannon, 1996
) and long QT syndrome (Wang et
al., 1995a
, 1995
b; Wang et al., 1996
).
The roles of some of the S4 charges during activation
and inactivation have been investigated previously in
both sodium and potassium channels. Stühmer et al.
(1989) found that neutralization of S4 positive charges
in domains I and II of the rat brain II sodium channel
shifted the voltage dependence of inactivation and altered the slope factor, but not necessarily in the same direction as the shifts of activation. Analyses of the effects of S4 mutations in the potassium channel have
demonstrated a much stronger concordance between
the effects on activation and inactivation (Papazian et
al., 1991
; Lopez et al., 1991
). These results support the
original hypothesis of Hodgkin and Huxley (1952)
that
potassium channels have the same gates for both activation and inactivation, whereas sodium channels have
separate activation and inactivation gates. Chen et al.
(1996)
recently examined the effects of mutations of
some of the S4 charges in all four domains of the sodium channel. They concluded that the S4 region of
domain IV plays a unique role in the coupling of activation to inactivation, consistent with previous results
from O'Leary et al. (1995)
.
To investigate the roles played by the four S4 segments in the voltage dependence of inactivation, we
constructed charge-neutralizing and -conserving substitutions of the second and fourth positive charges in the
S4 segment of each domain of the rat brain IIA (RBIIA) sodium channel. To examine the effects on
fast and slow inactivation, all of the mutations were
constructed both in a wild-type background and in a
channel containing the IFMQ3 mutation that eliminates fast inactivation (West et al., 1992). We show that
the positively charged residues in all four S4 segments
contribute unequally to the voltage dependence of inactivation, and that the effects on slow inactivation differ
depending on whether fast inactivation is intact or not.
Sodium Channel Mutations and Expression in Oocytes
The construction and nomenclature for the wild-type and mutant sodium channels are described in the accompanying paper
(Kontis et al., 1997). For the studies described in this paper, the
same S4 mutations were constructed in the wild-type sodium
channel
subunit background, with fast inactivation intact. The
procedures for in vitro transcription and expression in oocytes
are described in the accompanying paper (Kontis et al., 1997
).
Electrophysiological Recording
Sodium currents were measured by two-microelectrode voltage
clamping using an oocyte clamp (OC-725; Warner Instruments,
Hamden, CT). Data acquisition and analysis used pCLAMP software (version 6.0.3) and a TL-1 interface (Axon Instruments,
Inc., Burlingame, CA). Agarose-tipped borosilicate glass electrodes were filled with 3 M KCl and had impedances between 0.5 and 1.0 M, and currents were measured using a virtual ground
circuit. The recording bath solution contained (mM): 96 NaCl, 5 HEPES, pH 7.5, 2 KCl, 1 MgCl2, and 1.8 CaCl2. All experiments
were performed at room temperature (20-22°C). Oocytes were
clamped at
100 mV and 22°C for at least 5 min to allow temperature equilibration and full recovery from inactivation. Data were
acquired at a sampling frequency of 25 kHz with a filter frequency of 3 kHz. Depolarizations were from
90 to +50 mV in
10-mV steps from a holding potential of
100 mV, and lasted
57.5 ms. Capacitive and leak currents were eliminated by subtraction of records obtained in the presence of 400 nM tetrodotoxin for the data used to determine the time constants of fast inactivation. The series resistance varied from 1-2.0 k
.
Inactivation data were acquired using two-pulse protocols from
a holding potential of 100 mV. For fast inactivation, the conditioning pulses ranged from
90 to +5 mV in 5-mV steps and were 500 ms in duration. These were immediately followed by a test pulse to
10 mV to measure available current. The protocol for slow inactivation included a 60-s conditioning pulse ranging from
90 to +5 mV in 5-mV increments. This was followed by a
20-ms pulse to
150 mV to allow recovery from fast inactivation,
followed by a test pulse to
10 mV to measure available current.
The test potential was different for those mutants that had shifts
in the peak current-voltage relationship. The same slow inactivation protocol was used for the IFMQ3 channels and for the channels with fast inactivation intact. A similar protocol was used to measure the kinetics of slow inactivation, except that the conditioning pulse was to a constant potential of 0 mV and its duration
was varied from 1 to 30 s.
Data Analysis
The voltage dependence of inactivation was analyzed by measuring peak currents elicited during the test pulse. The current measurements were normalized to the maximum current observed
and plotted against the conditioning pulse potential. The data
were fit using the Sigmaplot program (version 4.0; Jandel Scientific, San Rafael, CA), which employs the Marquardt-Levenberg
algorithm for nonlinear regression. Normalized current-voltage
relationships were fit with a two-state Boltzmann function, I/Imax = 1/{1 + exp[(vv1/2)/a]}, where I/Imax is the normalized current,
v1/2 is the half-maximal voltage of inactivation, and a is the apparent slope factor. The slope factor is inversely related to the steepness of the voltage dependence. In cases where two components
of inactivation were observed, the data were fit with the sum of
two Boltzmann functions, I/Imax = f1/{1 + exp[(v
v1/2,1)/a1]} + f2/{1 + exp[(v
v1/2,2)/a2]}, where v1/2 and a have their usual
meanings and f1 and f2 are the fractions of the total current represented by each component of inactivation. The average and
standard deviation for each voltage-dependent parameter were
determined by individually fitting the data, and Student's unpaired t test was used to determine the statistical significance of
the differences.
The time constants of fast (h) and slow (
s) inactivation were
determined using the Chebyshev method to fit the current traces with a single or double exponential equation of the form
A1*exp[
(t
K)/
1] + A2*exp[
(t
K)/
2] + C, where A1 and
A2 represent the amplitudes at the start of the fit region of
1 and
2, which are the time constants for inactivation, K is the time
shift, and C is the steady state asymptote. The time shift was manually selected by fitting the traces at the time when the currents
were just starting to exponentially decrease.
Most of the S4 Substitutions Cause Small Hyperpolarizing Shifts in Fast Inactivation
The voltage dependence of fast sodium channel inactivation is thought to result mainly from the voltage dependence of activation (Aldrich et al., 1983; Aldrich
and Stevens, 1987
). In the context of a ball-and-chain
model for inactivation (Armstrong and Bezanilla, 1977
),
the coupling of activation and inactivation could result from conformational changes during activation that
make the docking site available to bind the inactivation
particle. Since the S4 positive charges are believed to
comprise at least part of the voltage sensor for activation, it is likely that mutations in the S4 regions would
affect fast sodium channel inactivation. We therefore
tested the effects on fast inactivation of mutations in
which charge neutralizing (glutamine) or charge conserving (arginine or lysine) substitutions were made for
the second and fourth residues of each S4 segment.
These are the same mutations that were described in
the accompanying paper (Kontis et al., 1997
), except
that they were made in the wild-type sodium channel with inactivation intact. The nomenclature is such that
substitution of the second positive charge of the domain I S4 segment with a glutamine is denoted as
1R2Q. Two double mutants were also constructed, one
that combines neutralizations of the fourth positive
charges in domains I and II (1K4Q and 2K4Q), and the
other combining neutralizations of the fourth positive
charges in domains III and IV (3R4Q and 4R4Q).
Fig. 1 shows the voltage dependence of steady state
inactivation examined with a 500-ms prepulse. The values for v1/2 and slope factor are shown in Table I. Neutralization of either the second or fourth charge in domain I, II, or III resulted in small but statistically significant negative shifts in v1/2. Neutralization of either the
second or fourth charge in domain IV had no significant effect on v1/2. Only one mutation (4R4K) shifted
the v1/2 in the positive direction. Most of the mutations
decreased the slope factor, but not to a large extent.
The combination of neutralizations in domains I and II
(1K4Q:2K4Q) shifted the v1/2 to a degree consistent with a summation of the slight shifts caused by each
substitution individually. In contrast, the 3R4Q:4R4Q
double mutation caused a large negative shift in v1/2,
which is surprising because the 3R4Q mutation caused
only a small negative shift and 4R4Q caused no apparent shift. This double mutant also demonstrated a significant reduction in the steepness of the curve (an increase in the slope factor).
Table I. Parameters of the Voltage Dependence of Fast Inactivation |
S4 Mutations of Domain IV Affect the Kinetics of Fast Inactivation
The analysis of steady state inactivation examines the
voltage dependence of inactivation from both closed
and open states. To determine if any of the mutations
specifically affects inactivation from open states, the
time constants for fast inactivation (h) were determined for all of the mutants at potentials ranging from
40 to +55 mV (Fig. 2). Most of the mutations had no
significant effects on the kinetics of fast inactivation.
The time constants for all of the substitutions in domains I, II, and III were comparable with those observed for the wild-type channel, as were the time constants for all but one of the single substitution mutations in domain IV. However, neutralization of the
fourth positive charge in domain IV (4R4Q) resulted in
significantly faster inactivation at negative potentials,
with a shallower voltage dependence. The double mutant in which the fourth charges in domains III and IV were neutralized was comparable with 4R4Q, demonstrating that this effect depended only on neutralization of the charge in the fourth position. Therefore,
neutralization of the fourth positive charge in domain
IV decreased the voltage dependence of
h, resulting in
faster inactivation than that of the wild-type channel at
potentials more negative than
20 mV.
Neutralization of Charges in Domains I or II Causes a Significant Shift in the v1/2 of Slow Inactivation in a Channel with Fast Inactivation Removed
To investigate the role of the S4 positive charges in slow
inactivation, the voltage dependence of slow inactivation was measured using the S4 mutants in the IFMQ3
background, with fast inactivation removed. Fig. 3
shows the voltage command protocol and representative traces recorded from an oocyte injected with RNA
encoding the wild-type sodium channel with the IFMQ3
mutation. The number of data points between 0 and 60 s
is small due to the slow sampling rate used during this
portion of the protocol. Peak currents were measured,
normalized to the maximum current, and plotted
against the conditioning potential. The voltage dependence of slow inactivation curves for the wild-type and
mutant channels are shown in Fig. 4, and the values for
v1/2 and slope factor are included in Table II.
Table II. Parameters of the Voltage Dependence of Slow Inactivation |
Neutralization of the fourth positive charge in either domain I (1K4Q) or II (2K4Q) resulted in the largest positive shifts in v1/2. Neither mutation caused a significant change in the slope factor. Combining the two mutations was essentially additive, resulting in a larger positive shift in v1/2 without any significant change in slope. In contrast, neutralization of the second charge in domain II (2R2Q) shifted v1/2 in the negative direction, with an increase in the slope factor. The comparable mutation in domain I had no effect on v1/2, but it decreased the slope factor.
All of the mutations in domain III resulted in slow inactivation curves that were very similar to that of the wild-type channel. Although a number of differences were statistically significant, they were small. The mutations in domain IV also had relatively modest effects. Neutralization of either the second (4R2Q) or fourth (4R4Q) charge in domain IV shifted v1/2 in the negative direction. Combining the mutations that neutralized the fourth charge in domain III or IV (3R4Q:4R4Q) resulted in a small negative shift in v1/2, confirming the lack of dramatic effects with mutations in these two domains.
S4 Mutations Have Different Effects on Slow Inactivation in a Channel with Fast Inactivation Intact
Featherstone et al. (1996) have recently shown that
slow inactivation in the rat skeletal muscle sodium
channel occurs more quickly when fast inactivation is
removed, suggesting that there is an interaction between the two processes. Because of this interaction, the S4 mutations might have different effects on slow
inactivation if fast inactivation were intact. To evaluate
this possibility, we analyzed slow inactivation of the S4
mutants in a wild-type background with fast inactivation intact. We used the protocol depicted in Fig. 3,
which includes a 20-ms hyperpolarization to
150 mV
to allow recovery from fast inactivation before measuring available current with the test pulse. The voltage dependence of slow inactivation curves for the wild-type
and mutant channels are shown in Fig. 5, and the values for v1/2 and slope factor are included in Table III.
In the absence of fast inactivation for the wild-type
channel, the v1/2 of slow inactivation was more positive
and the slope factor was smaller than when fast inactivation was intact. Therefore, the presence of a functional fast inactivation gate caused a small negative shift
in the v1/2 and a decrease in the steepness of the voltage dependence of slow inactivation.
Table III. Parameters of the Voltage Dependence of Slow Inactivation with Fast Inactivation Intact |
Substitution of the S4 positive charges had different effects on slow inactivation when fast inactivation was intact compared with the results when fast inactivation was removed. Most of the mutations shifted the v1/2 for slow inactivation in the negative direction and decreased the steepness of the curve. For example, neutralization of the second charge in domains I, II, or III shifted the v1/2 in the negative direction. However, the charge-conserving substitutions of the second residue in domains I and III resulted in negative shifts of similar magnitude, indicating that the shifts in v1/2 did not result from reduction of the charge in the S4 segment. Neutralization of the second charge in domain IV (4R2Q) had no significant effect on the v1/2 of slow inactivation.
Neutralization of the fourth charge in domain III or
IV resulted in small or insignificant negative shifts in v1/2.
However, neutralization of the fourth charge in domain I (1K4Q) or II (2K4Q) caused a marked positive
shift in v1/2 with a decrease in slope, leading to a curve
that could no longer be fit with a single Boltzmann function. Similar results were observed when the fourth
charge in domain I was replaced with arginine (1K4R),
or when the two charge neutralization mutations were
combined (1K4Q:2K4Q). Each of the curves for these
mutants could be fit very well with a two-component
Boltzmann function, as shown for the 1K4Q and 1K4Q: 2K4Q mutations in Fig. 6. In each case, there is a component of slow inactivation with a v1/2 slightly more
negative than that of the wild-type channel, and a second component with a more positive v1/2 and a shallower slope.
1K4Q Mutation Primarily Affects One Component of Slow Inactivation
The presence of two components in the voltage dependence of slow inactivation for the 1K4Q and 2K4Q mutants suggests that slow inactivation is not a single process. To evaluate this possibility, the kinetics of slow inactivation were determined for the wild-type, IFMQ3,
and 1K4Q mutant channels with and without fast inactivation. For the channels without fast inactivation, the
onset of slow inactivation was measured during a 60-s
depolarization to 0 mV (Fig. 7 A). Both IFMQ3 and
1K4Q channels inactivated with kinetics that were best
fit with two exponential time constants. The slower time constant was not significantly different for the two
channels, but the faster time constant for 1K4Q was
about half that for IFMQ3 (time constant values are
presented in Fig. 7). The presence of two time constants is consistent with the hypothesis that slow inactivation consists of at least two processes.
For the channels with fast inactivation intact, the onset of slow inactivation was determined using the protocol shown in Fig. 3, modified so that the prepulse varied in length from 0 to 30 s at a constant voltage of 0 mV. The presence of fast inactivation significantly delayed the onset of slow inactivation (Fig. 7 B), as previously shown by Featherstone et al. (1996). Both the
wild-type and 1K4Q channels with fast inactivation intact demonstrated two exponential time constants for
slow inactivation. As was the case for the channels without fast inactivation, the slower time constant was not
significantly different between the two channels, but the faster time constant for 1K4Q was much smaller
than that for the wild-type channel. In addition, the
steady state asymptote for the 1K4Q channel represented 27% of the current, consistent with the finding
that the 1K4Q channels did not completely slow inactivate at 0 mV (Fig. 6). These results indicate that slow inactivation consists of at least two components, and that
the 1K4Q mutation primarily affects the faster of the
two components and causes slow inactivation to be incomplete when fast inactivation is intact.
This study examines the effects of mutations in the four
putative voltage sensors on the voltage dependence of
fast and slow sodium channel inactivation. The effects
on fast inactivation have also been studied by Stühmer
et al. (1989) and Chen et al. (1996)
. Stühmer et al.
(1989)
examined the effects of mutations in domains I
and II in the rat brain II channel on steady state inactivation assessed with a 100-ms prepulse. Chen et al.
(1996)
investigated the effects of mutations of the first
and third charges in all four domains of the human
heart 1 channel on steady state inactivation assessed
with a 500-ms prepulse, which is equivalent to the protocol used in our study. The results of the three studies are summarized in Table IV. Most of the neutralizations in domain I resulted in small negative shifts in v1/2.
The only exception is 1R3Q assessed with a 100-ms
prepulse. Similarly, all but one of the neutralizations in
domain II resulted in negative shifts in v1/2, although
these were larger than those observed for the domain I
mutations. The one exception is 2R1Q, which demonstrated a significant positive shift. The effects were
more pronounced for three of the mutations in domain III, and again all of the shifts in v1/2 were in the
negative direction. Three of the neutralizations in domain IV caused very minor shifts, but the other mutation (4R3Q) resulted in a dramatic negative shift in v1/2.
The overall trend appears to be that neutralization of
S4 positive charges results in negative shifts in v1/2 and
small decreases in the slope factor of fast inactivation.
Table IV. Summary of Charge Neutralization Mutation Effects on Fast Inactivation |
Chen et al. (1996) also examined the effects of the S4
positive charge mutations on the kinetics of fast inactivation (
h). They observed that mutations in domains I,
II, and III did not significantly affect the kinetics of fast
inactivation, similar to our results. With respect to domain IV, Chen et al. (1996)
noted that some substitutions of the first and third positive charges decreased
the voltage dependence of
h, resulting in slower inactivation at potentials more positive than
40 mV. Our
finding that neutralization of the fourth charge in domain IV decreased the voltage dependence of
h is similar to the observations of Chen et al. (1996)
. However,
the net effect of the mutation we studied was to speed
up inactivation at potentials more negative than
20
mV, in contrast to the slowing down observed by Chen et al. (1996)
. We did not observe any significant effects
on
h when the second positive charge in domain IV
was substituted. These results suggest that the fourth
domain plays a unique role in determining inactivation, as concluded by Chen et al. (1996)
, but indicate
that all of the charged residues in the fourth domain are not equivalent.
The effects of the mutations on the voltage dependence of fast inactivation contrast with their effects on
activation, as presented in the accompanying manuscript (Kontis et al., 1997) and also examined by Stühmer et al. (1989)
and Chen et al. (1996)
. Most of the S4
mutations in domains I, II, and IV resulted in significant shifts in the v1/2 of activation in the positive direction (Table V). In contrast, the mutations in domains I
and II resulted in negative shifts in fast inactivation,
and most of the mutations in domain IV had no significant effects on fast inactivation (Table V). There is not
much better concordance for the effects of the mutations in domain III. Neutralization of the second
(3R2Q) or fourth (3R4Q) charge caused a negative
shift in activation and fast inactivation, but 3R2Q resulted in a larger shift in fast inactivation and 3R4Q
caused a larger shift in activation. These discrepancies
argue against the hypothesis that most of the voltage dependence of fast inactivation in the rat brain sodium
channel is derived from activation (Bezanilla and Armstrong, 1977
; Nonner, 1980
; Aldrich and Stevens, 1987
;
Zagotta and Aldrich, 1990
). Instead, the voltage dependence of fast inactivation may be derived more directly
from movement of a subset of the voltage sensors. Consistent with this hypothesis, Chen et al. (1996)
observed
that the S4 segment of domain IV was uniquely involved in the voltage dependence of inactivation.
Table V. Comparison of Mutation Effects on Activation and Inactivation |
The effects of the S4 mutations on slow inactivation depended on whether fast inactivation was intact or not. When fast inactivation was removed by inclusion of the IFMQ3 mutation, most of the mutations resulted in positive shifts in v1/2 (Table V). The effects on slow inactivation of the mutations in domains I and II generally corresponded with the effects of those mutations on activation. For example, neutralization of the fourth residue in either domain I or II shifted both activation and slow inactivation in the positive direction, and combining the two mutations resulted in shifts in both activation and slow inactivation that were essentially additive (Table V). The mutations in domain III had essentially no effect on slow inactivation but did have significant effects on activation, and the mutations in domain IV caused small shifts in slow inactivation but had no significant effects on activation. These results suggest that activation and slow inactivation both derive their voltage dependence from movements of the S4 regions, which is not surprising since both processes most likely represent voltage initiated conformational changes in the channel.
The effects of the mutations on slow inactivation were quite different when fast inactivation was intact. Most of the mutations caused significant shifts of v1/2 in the negative direction, but the magnitude of the effects did not correlate with the reduction in charge. For example, neutralization of the second residue in either domain I or III resulted in negative shifts of slow inactivation when fast inactivation was intact (Table V). The charge-conserving substitutions at the same positions had comparable effects, demonstrating that the shifts were not caused by elimination of the charge. These results indicate that the structure of the residues at these positions in S4 have more important roles in the transition to the slow inactivated state when fast inactivation is present than when it is absent.
The most dramatic effects were observed with mutations of the fourth positive charge in domain I or II
(1K4Q, 1K4R, and 2K4Q). Each of these mutations resulted in a second component in the voltage dependence of slow inactivation, but only when fast inactivation was intact. The first component had a v1/2 that was
slightly more negative than that of the wild-type channel, and the second component had a pronounced
shift in the positive direction and a marked increase in
slope factor (Table III). Featherstone et al. (1996) have
previously reported that slow inactivation in the skeletal muscle sodium channel differs depending on the
presence or absence of fast inactivation. They observed
that slow inactivation has a comparable v1/2 in both situations, but that it is only ~80% complete when fast inactivation is present. Their results are consistent with a
model in which fast and slow inactivation are not mutually exclusive, and that fast inactivation interferes with
slow inactivation. Our data with the wild-type rat brain IIA channel were somewhat different in that slow inactivation was complete even when fast inactivation was
intact, but there was a small negative shift in the v1/2 of
slow inactivation.
The second component in the voltage dependence of slow inactivation suggests that slow inactivation does not represent a single process. Consistent with this hypothesis, the kinetics of slow inactivation were best fit with two exponential time constants, regardless of the presence of fast inactivation (Fig. 7). The 1K4Q mutation appeared to affect only the faster component, decreasing the magnitude of the time constant. In addition, this mutation caused incomplete slow inactivation at 0 mV when fast inactivation was intact, as did all of the mutations that caused a second component in the voltage dependence of slow inactivation. These mutations also caused positive voltage shifts in slow inactivation when fast inactivation was eliminated (Table V). One hypothesis to explain these results is that the positive voltage shift resulted in slow inactivation occurring in a voltage region in which fast inactivation was more complete. Therefore, more extensive fast inactivation during the conditioning pulse caused more interference with slow inactivation. The net result would be that some percentage of the channels do not undergo slow inactivation until a more positive membrane potential is reached.
In summary, the data are consistent with a model in which the S4 regions of all four domains of the sodium channel are involved in both activation and inactivation gating. Each S4 segment has a different role in both processes, however, so that mutations in any one domain differentially affect each process. Fast inactivation is not completely coupled to activation, but may derive its voltage dependence from movements of the S4 segments that are not rate limiting for activation. The true test of the importance of individual charges as voltage sensing residues for both activation and inactivation will be to examine the effects of S4 mutations on the gating currents of the channel.
Address correspondence to Dr. Alan L. Goldin, Department of Microbiology and Molecular Genetics, University of California, Irvine, Irvine, California 92697-4025. Fax: 714-824-8598; E-mail: agoldin{at}uci.edu
Received for publication 26 February 1997 and accepted in revised form 23 July 1997.
Dr. Kontis' present address is Hycor Biomedical Inc., Garden Grove, CA 92841.We thank Dr. Raymond Smith, Dr. Michael Pugsley, Ted Shih, Marianne Smith, and Dan Allen for helpful discussions during the course of this work, Dan Allen and Ian Jester for help with the experiments, and Mimi Reyes for excellent technical assistance.
This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NS-26729) and the National Science Foundation (IBN9221984). A.L. Goldin is an Established Investigator of the American Heart Association.
1. | Aldrich, R.W., D.P. Corey, and C.F. Stevens. 1983. A reinterpretation of mammalian sodium channel gating based on single channel recording. Nature (Lond.). 306: 436-441 [Medline]. |
2. | Aldrich, R.W., and C.F. Stevens. 1987. Voltage-dependent gating of single sodium channels from mammalian neuroblastoma cells. J. Neurosci. 7: 418-431 [Abstract]. |
3. | Armstrong, C.M., and F. Bezanilla. 1977. Inactivation of the sodium channel. II. Gating current experiments. J. Gen. Physiol. 70: 567-590 [Abstract]. |
4. | Barchi, R.L.. 1995. Molecular pathology of the skeletal muscle sodium channel. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 57: 355-385 [Medline]. |
5. |
Bezanilla, F., and
C.M. Armstrong.
1977.
Inactivation of the sodium
channel-sodium current experiments.
J. Gen. Physiol.
70:
549-566
|
6. | Cannon, S.C.. 1996. Ion-channel defects and aberrant excitability in myotonia and periodic paralysis. Trends Neurosci. 19: 3-10 [Medline]. |
7. | Chen, L.-Q., V. Santarelli, R. Horn, and R.G. Kallen. 1996. A unique role for the S4 segment of domain 4 in the inactivation of sodium channels. J. Gen. Physiol. 108: 549-556 [Abstract]. |
8. | Featherstone, D.E., J.E. Richmond, and P.C. Ruben. 1996. Interaction between fast and slow inactivation in Skm1 sodium channels. Biophys. J. 71: 3098-3109 [Abstract]. |
9. | Hodgkin, A.L., and A.F. Huxley. 1952. A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve. J. Physiol. (Cambr.). 117: 500-544 [Medline]. |
10. | Hoshi, T., W.N. Zagotta, and R.W. Aldrich. 1990. Biophysical and molecular mechanisms of Shaker potassium channel inactivation. Science (Wash. DC). 250: 533-538 [Medline]. |
11. |
Kontis, K.J.,
A. Rounaghi, and
A.L. Goldin.
1997.
Sodium channel
activation gating is affected by substitutions of voltage sensor positive charges in all four domains.
J. Gen. Physiol.
110:
391-401
|
12. | Lopez, G.A., Y.N. Jan, and L.Y. Jan. 1991. Hydrophobic substitution mutations in the S4 sequence alter voltage-dependent gating in Shaker K+ channels. Neuron. 7: 327-336 [Medline]. |
13. | Nonner, W.. 1980. Relations between the inactivation of sodium channels and the immobilization of gating charge in frog myelinated nerve. J. Physiol. (Cambr.). 299: 573-603 [Medline]. |
14. | O'Leary, M.E., L.-Q. Chen, R.G. Kallen, and R. Horn. 1995. A molecular link between activation and inactivation of sodium channels. J. Gen. Physiol. 106: 641-658 [Abstract]. |
15. | Papazian, D.M., L.C. Timpe, Y.N. Jan, and L.Y. Jan. 1991. Alteration of voltage-dependence of Shaker potassium channel by mutations in the S4 sequence. Nature (Lond.). 349: 305-310 [Medline]. |
16. | Patton, D.E., J.W. West, W.A. Catterall, and A.L. Goldin. 1992. Amino acid residues required for fast sodium channel inactivation. Charge neutralizations and deletions in the III-IV linker. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 89: 10905-10909 [Abstract]. |
17. | Ruben, P.C., J.G. Starkus, and M.D. Rayner. 1990. Holding potential affects the apparent voltage-sensitivity of sodium channel activation in crayfish giant axons. Biophys. J. 58: 1169-1181 [Abstract]. |
18. | Ruff, R.L., L. Simoncini, and W. Stühmer. 1987. Comparison between slow sodium channel inactivation in rat slow- and fast-twitch muscle. J. Physiol. 383: 339-348 [Abstract]. |
19. | Ruff, R.L., L. Simoncini, and W. Stühmer. 1988. Slow sodium channel inactivation in mammalian muscle: a possible role in regulating excitability. Muscle Nerve. 11: 502-510 [Medline]. |
20. | Simoncini, L., and W. Stühmer. 1987. Slow sodium channel inactivation in rat fast-twitch muscle. J. Physiol. 383: 327-337 [Abstract]. |
21. | Stühmer, W., F. Conti, H. Suzuki, X. Wang, M. Noda, N. Yahagi, H. Kubo, and S. Numa. 1989. Structural parts involved in activation and inactivation of the sodium channel. Nature (Lond.). 339: 597-603 [Medline]. |
22. |
Wang, D.W.,
K. Yazawa,
A.L. George Jr., and
P.B. Bennett.
1996.
Characterization of human cardiac Na+ channel mutations in
the congenital long QT syndrome.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA.
93:
13200-13205
|
23. | Wang, Q., J. Shen, Z. Li, K. Timothy, G.M. Vincent, S.G. Priori, P.J. Schwartz, and M.T. Keating. 1995a. Cardiac sodium channel mutations in patients with long QT syndrome, an inherited cardiac arrhythmia. Hum. Mol. Genet. 4: 1603-1607 [Abstract]. |
24. | Wang, Q., J. Shen, I. Splawski, D. Atkinson, Z. Li, J.L. Robinson, A.J. Moss, J.A. Towbin, and M.T. Keating. 1995. b. SCN5A mutations associated with an inherited cardiac arrhythmia, long QT syndrome. Cell. 80: 805-811 [Medline]. |
25. | West, J.W., D.E. Patton, T. Scheuer, Y. Wang, A.L. Goldin, and W.A. Catterall. 1992. A cluster of hydrophobic amino acid residues required for fast Na+ channel inactivation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 89: 10910-10914 [Abstract]. |
26. | Zagotta, W.N., T. Hoshi, and R.W. Aldrich. 1990. Restoration of inactivation in mutants of Shaker potassium channels by a peptide derived from ShB. Science (Wash. DC). 250: 568-571 [Medline]. |
27. | Zagotta, W.N., and R.W. Aldrich. 1990. Voltage-dependent gating of Shaker A-type potassium channels in Drosophila muscle. J. Gen. Physiol. 98: 29-60 . |