Timing of praying mantis evasive responses during simulated bat attack sequences
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA
* Author for correspondence at present address: Department of Biology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA 30303, USA (e-mail: biojdt{at}langate.gsu.edu)
Accepted 23 February 2005
![]() |
Summary |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Key words: mantis, bat, escape, insect, hearing
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Certain night-flying insects possess auditory systems sensitive to the
echolocation calls of bats that prey upon them
(Hoy, 1998;
Yager, 1999
). These insects
perform evasive maneuvers that are effective in eluding their bat predators.
For moths (Roeder, 1967
),
lacewings (Miller and Olesen,
1979
), and mantids (Yager et
al., 1990
), these maneuvers vary depending on whether the bat
represents a low-level threat (i.e. gradual turns when the bat is far away) or
a high-level threat (i.e. dramatic loops and dives when the bat is very
close). The insect's nervous system must detect the bat, determine the level
of danger, and initiate the proper response at the appropriate time for the
maneuver to be effective. Furthermore, these actions must occur on the scale
of tens of milliseconds to succeed.
Bat vocalizations emitted during an attack sequence undergo a variety of
stereotypic changes in pulse repetition rate (PRR), pulse duration (PD), pulse
frequency and pulse intensity during attack sequences
(Griffin, 1958;
Simmons et al., 1979
;
Kick, 1982
;
Surlykke and Moss, 2000
).
These changes provide a variety of cues that insects could use to determine
the level of danger posed by a bat and guide decisions such as whether to
respond, the type of response, and when to initiate the response. This is the
case for the triggering of `last chance' responses in two different insects.
High PRRs [>100 pulses s-1 (pps)] trigger ultrasonic `click'
production in dogbane tiger moths (Fullard
et al., 1994
). This finding supported the hypothesis that the
`clicks' may be attempts to startle
(Möhl and Miller, 1976
)
or confuse (Fullard et al.,
1979
) the bat moments before capture. Similarly, green lacewings
passively fall in response to bat echolocation calls and high PRRs further
elicit `wing-flips' in the passively falling lacewings. `Wing-flips' alter the
ballistic trajectory of the passive fall, causing the bats to miss the insect
(Miller and Olesen, 1979
).
Praying mantids possess an auditory system that is sensitive to bat
vocalizations, with auditory tuning most sensitive between 20-60 kHz in most
species (Yager and Hoy, 1989;
Triblehorn and Yager, 2001
).
The single ear, located ventrally between the metathoracic legs, consists of
two tympana within a midline chamber. Physiological and behavioral tests have
demonstrated that the auditory system is nondirectional. Mantids respond to
low-level threats by gradually turning and high-level threats by entering a
power dive (Yager et al.,
1990
). These maneuvers are highly effective in preventing capture
(Yager et al., 1990
;
Triblehorn, 2003
).
Mantis auditory interneuron 501-T3 is a likely interneuron for triggering
these responses since it is ultrasound-sensitive (best sensitivity between
25-45 kHz), responds with a short latency (8-12 ms minimum), and has a fast
conduction velocity of 4 m s-1
(Yager and Hoy, 1989).
Implanted electrodes recording 501-T3 activity during flying bat attacks
(Triblehorn and Yager, 2002
)
demonstrated that this interneuron tracks bat vocalizations very well as the
bat approaches the mantis and begins to increase its PRR during its attack
(following PRRs up to 55 pps on average). However, 501-T3 ceased producing
multi-spike bursts during the attack as the bat continued to increase its PRR
before completely shutting down for the remainder of the attack. Although
mantids have other auditory interneurons, 501-T3 responses were the only
auditory interneuron activity observed during the bat attack. If 501-T3 is the
trigger interneuron, the results from that study suggest that evasive
responses must be triggered prior to 501-T3 shutdown.
This study uses a tethered flight paradigm to answer two pertinent questions: (1) when does the mantis initiate its power dive during simulated bat attack sequences; and (2) what parameters in the bat attack vocalization sequence are important for triggering the power dive response? The results demonstrate that bat emission patterns influence the triggering of the mantis evasive response in a way that potentially affects the mantis' chance of escape.
![]() |
Materials and methods |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Simulated bat attack sequences
Experimental stimuli consisted of pulse trains simulating the PRRs and PDs
of echolocation vocalizations emitted during the last 1200 ms of a successful
bat attack sequence (i.e. capture). All pulses had a 0.1 ms rise/fall time.
Each sequence (5 in total) was based on the temporal pattern (i.e. PRRs and
PDs) of actual bat (Eptesicus fuscus) attack sequences recorded
during a previous experiment conducted in a flight room designed for
behavioral studies of bat echolocation behavior
(Triblehorn and Yager, 2002).
The temporal patterns of the attack sequences were typical of flight room
echolocation behavior, including the absence of search phase vocalizations
(Surlykke and Moss, 2000
;
Triblehorn and Yager, 2002
).
The temporal patterns of these sequences are similar to the most common type
of echolocation pattern, those emitted by bats using frequency-modulated
vocalizations (e.g. FM bats) as opposed to other echolocation strategies (e.g.
constant frequency or gleaning bats; for a review, see
Schnitzler and Kalko, 2001
).
During free flight encounters, mantids perform effective power dives in
response to attacking, echolocating Eptesicus fuscus
(Triblehorn, 2003
).
Each sequence (Fig. 1) began with low, stable PRRs (around 12 pps, PDs=3-4 ms; traditionally called the `approach phase') followed by a period of continuously increasing PRRs before reaching a consistent high PRR (130-150 pps, PDs <1 ms; called the `terminal buzz') just before `contact.' In this study, the period of continuously increasing PRRs (shaded regions in Fig. 1) is referred to as the transition period. The slope of the transition period (pps 10 ms-1) provided a measure of how quickly the bat switched from low to high PRRs during an attack. Three of the sequences, termed rapid transition (RT) sequences, contained fast increases in PRR over a short transition period (slopes of 5.37, 8.25 and 14.01 pps ms-1). The other two stimuli, termed gradual transition (GT) sequences, contained slower PRR increases over a longer transition period, indicated by lower slopes (1.78 and 3.45 pps 10 ms-1) in Fig. 1.
|
Sequences were presented using 35 kHz tones, the best frequency for 501-T3
in P. agrionina, at 85 dB sound pressure level (SPL)
except where noted. At 35 kHz, P. agrionina's behavioral threshold is
about 70 dB SPL (Yager and May,
1990) and 501-T3's threshold is 10 dB lower
(Yager and Hoy, 1989
). Stimuli
25 dB over threshold elicits maximum or near-maximum responses in 501-T3,
(including spikes/stimulus and instantaneous firing frequency) at minimum
firing latencies (Yager and Hoy,
1989
) and evokes the full escape response (see below). Thus, bat
attack sequences presented at this intensity should elicit the earliest mantis
responses for each attack sequence and thus provide the `best case' scenario
for the mantis. Louder stimuli produce similar responses in 501-T3 (i.e.
spikes/stimulus and instantaneous firing frequency do not decrease at higher
intensities, up to 50 dB over threshold;
Yager and Hoy, 1989
), but
higher intensities could habituate the behavioral response with repeated
exposures.
Unlike the simulated trains in this study, bats vary the intensity of their
emissions during attack sequences (Boonman
and Jones, 2002). To ensure the simulated sequences produced
similar 501-T3 activity as flying bat attack sequences, 501-T3 responses to
the simulated trains (fixed intensity) were compared to the responses recorded
during the corresponding flying bat attacks (changing intensity;
Triblehorn and Yager, 2002
).
For all five sequences, 501-T3 responses during the first 400 ms of the
sequence were greater (i.e. more spikes/stimulus) for the simulated sequences.
In four of the five sequences, 501-T3 activity was similar (i.e. similar
spikes/stimulus) during the last 700-800 ms of the attack sequence. The fifth
simulated sequence (RT1) elicited more 501-T3 spikes/stimulus than during the
corresponding bat attack sequence until the last 200 ms, when 501-T3 responses
shut down in response to both simulated and flying bat sequences. Since 501-T3
responses were similar during the flying bat (changing intensity) and
simulated sequences (fixed intensity) during the last 700-800 ms (containing
the transition period and post-transition period leading up to capture), the
simulated sequences should elicit behavioral responses similar to those that
the flying attacks would have elicited.
Experimental setup
All experiments were conducted in a chamber (2.0 mx0.8 mx0.8 m)
lined with acoustic foam and a speaker (EAS10TH400B leaf tweeter, Technics,
Secaucus, NJ, USA) at one end. SuperScope I with a MACADIOS board generated
the train stimuli by gating a sine wave generator (Model DS345, Stanford
Research Systems, Sunnyvale, CA, USA). Custom-built electronics shaped the
sine wave pulses. A Hewlett-Packard 350D attenuator (Palo Alto, CA, USA)
controlled intensity and a Harmon Kardon HK-655 power amplifier amplified the
signal to the speakers. Stimulus calibration followed the procedure described
in Triblehorn and Yager
(2001), in which the stimuli
were 300 ms pulses (0.1 ms rise/fall). The calibration was referenced to a
Brüel & Kjaer 4135 6.36 mm microphone (Nærum, Denmark).
Mantids were first pre-tethered with wax (Kerr brand sticky wax,
Emeryville, CA, USA) on the dorsal surface of the pronotum as far caudal as
possible (toward the mantid's center of mass). Thread melted in the wax and a
touch of Superglue held the pre-tether in place. The tether (a thin wire
tipped with sticky wax) attached to the pre-tether by melting the wax
together. The mantis was positioned 0.7 m from the speaker, 11 cm from the
floor of the chamber. A fan provided a gentle headwind that induced flight
initiation as well as promoted longer bouts of stable flight. Stable flight
was defined as the mantis assuming standard flight posture (with all three
pairs of legs tucked in and still; Yager
and May, 1990) and both pairs of wings beating continuously (i.e.
without brief or long pauses). Both conditions had to be present for several
seconds before stimulus presentation.
Sequences were presented in blocks of five trials, with each sequence presented once per block. Interstimulus intervals of 1 min separated each sequence presentation. Each mantis was exposed to 3-5 trial blocks, with 5 min separating each block. Thus, each mantis was exposed to each attack sequence 3-5 times. Attack sequences were presented in a different order in each block, but the order was consistent across mantids. In between trials, the mantis rested on a platform without headwind stimulation. During trials, stimulus presentation occurred only after the mantis achieved a stable flight pattern. For an individual mantis in a single day, testing ended if the mantis appeared fatigued (demonstrated by short flight bouts and/or unstable flight patterns). If this occurred in the middle of a block, the data from that entire block was discarded. In many cases, testing resumed on another day. For each mantis, a control sequence representing the vocalization pattern preceding the transition phase (PRR=12 pps, PD 4 ms for 1200-2000 ms, 85, 90, and 95 dB SPL) was presented.
Behavioral responses and predicted trigger points
Pulse trains of ultrasound elicit the mantis evasive response while in
tethered flight. The response is a multi-component behavior consisting of: (1)
foreleg extension, (2) abdominal dorsiflexion, (3) wing-beat phase and
frequency changes and (4) a head roll
(Yager and May, 1990). To
determine when the behavioral response occurred during a simulated bat attack
sequence, a laser directed at a photocell was positioned in front of the
mantis so the forelegs would break the beam when extended during evasive
response production (Fig. 2A).
Although the beginning of foreleg extension is the fastest component of the
evasive response (66 ms; Yager and May,
1990
), the positioning of the laser measured nearly full foreleg
extension that provided latencies (typically between 130-210 ms) that were
closer to the latencies for changes in flight paths during free flight
(125-230 ms; Yager et al.,
1990
).
|
Determining the trigger point is more difficult since there is only one
known variable (the behavioral response) and two unknowns (when the attack
sequence triggers the response and the latency between the trigger point and
the behavioral response). To provide an estimate of the latency between the
trigger point and the behavior, the following procedure preceded simulated bat
attack sequence presentations for each mantis (illustrated in
Fig. 2B,C). After tethering the
mantis and positioning the laser (Fig.
2B), a standard 300 ms train (PRR=50 pps, PD=10 ms, 35 kHz, 85 dB
SPL) presentation evoked an evasive response in the flying mantis.
This is the typical pulse train stimulus used by our laboratory for eliciting
consistent, strong evasive responses during tethered flight across mantis
species (see Triblehorn and Yager,
2001). Since the standard train had consistent parameters with a
known onset, the time between the beginning of standard train and the
behavioral response provided the response latency
(Fig. 2B). The average results
from five standard stimulus presentations provided an average response
latency. Trigger point predictions were calculated by subtracting the average
response latency from the time of the behavioral response to obtain the
predicted trigger point (Fig.
2C). These individually calculated predicted trigger points were
averaged to obtain the average predicted trigger point for each individual
mantis. If testing occurred over different days, a new response latency
estimate was determined for that day's data.
The effects of stimulus intensity on the timing of behavioral responses were determined by testing two sequences (RT1 and GT1) presented at 75-95 dB SPL in 5 dB intervals in increasing order. A 1 min interstimulus interval separated each trial within a block and a 5 min period separated each block. Six mantids were tested with each sequence. Three mantids were tested using both sequences while six other mantids were tested with only one of the sequences (data from nine mantids total). For mantids exposed to both sequences, responses were collected on separate days. For both sequences, some of the mantids (one for GT1 and two for RT1) were tested only between 75-90 dB SPL.
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Fig. 3 illustrates the results for the behavioral responses to the three RT attack sequences. Each symbol (diamonds for behavioral responses, triangles for predicted trigger points) represents the results from a single mantis (N=8, each individual represented by a color) and is the average from 3-5 trials. The deviation between the behavioral response and trigger point data differ because the `standard' latency used in predicting the trigger points differed across individuals. For all mantids, behavioral responses occurred very close to the `contact' time, i.e. the time the bat reached and captured the target during the actual attack sequence. Behavioral responses occurred between 50-150 ms (mean=114.0 ms, S.D.=42.4 ms) before `contact,' when the `bat' reached its maximum PRR after the transition period (PRR >120 pps in the three sequences). Behavioral responses did not systematically decline during the course of the experiment, indicating that the presentation paradigm effectively prevented habituation.
|
Trigger point predictions fell between 250-370 ms (mean=287.4 ms, S.D.=47.2 ms) before `contact.' In the majority of the cases, this corresponded to the transition period of the bat attack, shortly after PRR began to increase. For another individual (purple triangle, RT3), the prediction fell just after the transition period, when the PRR reached 120 pps. In the majority of individuals, however, the trigger point predictions indicated that only a moderate increase in PRR early in the transition phase was necessary for triggering the mantis power dive. From a stable PRR of 12 pps prior to the transition phase, 73.92% of the trials predicted that PRRs between 15-40 pps triggered the responses.
Bat attack sequences containing gradual transitions (GT bat attack sequences) elicited a very different pattern in the timing of the behavioral response (shown in Fig. 4). GT1 elicited behavioral responses 260-400 ms (mean=308.2 ms, S.D.=99.3 ms) before `contact,' earlier than responses elicited by the RT sequences. This period corresponded to the transition phase of the attack sequence. GT2 contained a shallower overall transition than any of the RT sequences, but was steeper than GT1. All mantids responded to the GT2 sequence 100-225 ms (mean=154 ms, S.D.=60 ms) before `contact.' Half of the mantids responded after the transition period (as seen in the RT sequences) while the rest responded during the transition period (similar to the responses to the GT1 sequence, though not as early). Mantis behavioral responses also exhibited greater individual variation to GT sequences than to RT sequences (standard deviations for behavioral responses were 44.8, 45.3 and 36.2 for RT1-3 and 99.3 and 60 for GT1-2).
|
Since GT sequences generally elicited behavioral responses earlier than RT sequences, the predicted trigger times naturally occurred earlier relative to `contact' time. Despite these earlier predictions, GT attack sequences triggered mantis responses at similar PRRs to the RT attack sequences (77.02% of trials predicted PRRs between 15-40 pps triggered the behavioral response). The trigger point predictions exhibited less individual variation than the behavioral response times.
It is important to note that mantids never responded prior to the transition period (i.e. the `approach phase') in any of the five simulated bat attack sequences, when the PRR was low and stable (PRR=12 pps). To ensure that such vocalization sequences fail to elicit the mantis power dive, a static pulse sequence (PRR=12 pps, PD=4 ms) was presented for 2000 ms at 90 or 95 dB SPL. These presentations never elicited a power dive response in tethered flying mantids.
Effects of attack sequence intensity on mantis behavioral responses
Previous studies using static pulse trains demonstrated that louder stimuli
elicited a stronger behavioral response (greater yaw:
Yager and May, 1990; higher
percentage of power dives: Yager et al.,
1990
) and decreased the latency of the response
(Yager and May, 1990
). To
determine whether intensity affects when simulated bat attack sequences elicit
responses, two different sequences (GT1 and RT1) were presented at five
different intensities (75, 80, 85, 90 and 95 dB SPL) to tethered flying
mantids.
Fig. 5 shows the average behavioral response times (relative to the response time elicited at 85 dB SPL) for all six mantids for both sequences. Comparing the average responses elicited by 75 and 95 dB SPL (75 and 90 dB SPL for gray and black data for RT1 and blue data for GT1), increasing intensity had a tendency to evoke behavioral responses earlier in the sequence for most of the mantids (the exception is green data for RT1). Across individuals, the difference in the average response times at 75 vs 95 dB SPL (or 75 vs 90 dB SPL) ranged from 29-218 ms (median=129.5 ms, omitting the green data) for RT1. For GT1, this difference ranged from 55-303 ms (median=188 ms).
|
The effect of intensity on behavioral response times was not equivalent across the range of intensities tested. For mantids tested on the full range of intensities (75-95 dB SPL) during GT1 presentations, the difference in the response times elicited by 75 vs 85 dB SPL (100-300 ms differences) were greater than the differences in the response times elicited by 85 vs 95 dB (11-81 ms) for four of the five mantids (80%; pink data the exception). For RT1, this was true in two of the four mantids (50%, red and blue data, 150 ms vs 48 and 64 ms, respectively) and a third mantis showed no difference (green data).
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
The results of the present study further suggest that the characteristics
of this transition period could be an important factor influencing the outcome
of a bat-mantis interaction. Longer transition periods (such as in GT1 and
GT2) can `tip off' the mantis, evoking a power dive before the bat is very
close, allowing the mantis to escape. On the other hand, very short transition
periods could circumvent the mantis auditory defense, allowing the bat to
capture the mantis. Although not specifically analyzed in previous studies,
examples of echolocation sequences show that transition periods do vary (e.g.
Kalko and Schnitzler, 1989;
Kalko, 1995
). The transition
period may also affect other insect ultrasound-mediated evasive responses as
well. If so, not only mantids, but also other insects, could benefit from
having a back-up system for situations when the auditory defense fails.
It may be the case that rate of PRR change (the `slope' of the transition
period) and not absolute PRR (or both together) may be an important parameter
for triggering evasive responses in mantids and other insects. Most studies
only consider physiological and behavioral responses to static PRR trains
(Fullard, 1984;
Moiseff and Hoy, 1983
; Faure
and Hoy,
2000a
,b
)
or to a single attack sequence containing changes in PRR (Fullard et al.,
1994
,
2003
). The results of the
current study identify: (1) the importance of the transition phase and pulse
train stimuli that include this phase, which more closely resemble natural bat
echolocation sequences, and (2) the importance of testing multiple attack
sequences. Both should be considered in future studies of insect physiological
and behavioral auditory responses.
Risk assessment and the triggering of the power dive
The nature of echolocation requires bats to perceive the world in a
strobe-like manner, as bats must produce a vocalization and wait for the
returning echo to obtain information about the environment. When tracking and
hunting an insect, a bat must continuously update its information concerning
the insect's location and these updates need to occur more often as the bat
comes closer to capturing the insect. Thus, PRRs increase closer to capture
and PDs decrease to prevent overlap between the next vocalization and the
returning echo from the previous emission
(Schnitzler et al., 1980;
Simmons and Grinnell, 1980
;
Kalko, 1995
).
Assuming 501-T3 is the interneuron primarily responsible for triggering the
mantis evasive response, the decision to dive will be based on two factors:
how often 501-T3 fires and how many spikes comprise each response. 501-T3 is
not spontaneously active and only produces spikes in response to ultrasound
(Yager and Hoy, 1989).
Triblehorn and Yager (2002
)
confirmed this was true during flying bat attacks and demonstrated that 501-T3
burst rates followed the bat's PRR reliably into the transition phase.
Therefore, how often 501-T3 fires (501-T3 burst rate) determines the rate of
information provided to the mantis, dictated by the bat's PRR. The number of
spikes comprising each 501-T3 response is a function of the `effective
strength' of each pulse in eliciting behavioral responses. A variety of
parameters, including vocalization duration and intensity, influences 501-T3's
response. Two examples illustrate that both factors of 501-T3's response are
important for triggering the mantis evasive response.
First, pulse trains simulating bat vocalization patterns prior to the
transition phase (low PRR around 12 pps with PDs around 4 ms presented at 85
dB SPL or higher) failed to elicit evasive responses. However,
physiological recordings in non-flying mantids reveal that 501-T3 responses to
such pulse trains typically contain 5-6 spikes (J. D. Triblehorn, unpublished
data), but this activity fails to trigger an evasive response. The low 501-T3
burst rate likely causes this failure. However, this is beneficial as it
limits false alarms and prevents the evasive response from occurring too
early, possibly allowing the bat to adjust to the mantid's maneuver. Second,
pulse trains simulating bat vocalization patterns after the transition period
(very high PRRs >100 pps with PDs <1 ms) also fail to elicit power dives
(J. D. Triblehorn, personal observation). In this case, the high PRR
potentially provides a large amount of input, but 501-T3 responds either
weakly (single spikes) or not all to each vocalization
(Yager and Hoy, 1989;
Yager et al., 2001
). As the
average mantis power dive latency is 242 ms, responses initiated after the
transition period would generally be ineffective anyway as the bat would
capture the mantis before the power dive is fully developed.
The transition period of the bat attack sequence provides the best
indication to the mantis that it is in danger. Although a bat can break off an
attack during this period, it does represent a level of commitment by the bat
that is absent earlier in the attack sequence (when the bat vocalizes using
low PRRs). Although it may lead to some false alarm responses in the mantis,
waiting for an indication of the late stage of the bat attack (i.e. PRRs
>100 pps) would not leave the mantis enough time to escape. Fullard
(1984) used similar reasoning
to explain why PRRs between 30-50 pps optimally elicit clicking behavior in
the arctiid moth Cycnia tenera. Low pulse rates indicate a distant
bat that may trigger early defenses, but not close range defenses while very
high pulse rates are similar to non-predator sounds such as singing
insects.
The transition period and 501-T3's responses may not only affect the
initiation of the power dive response, but also the magnitude of the response.
This would not be unusual. In crickets, the magnitude of Int-1's response
directly correlates with the strength of the response in the dorsal
longitudinal muscles, and thus the magnitude of the evasive turn
(Nolen and Hoy, 1984). In
mantids, the magnitude of the power dive may, at least in part, be attributed
to how long 501-T3 continues responding to bat vocalizations after initiating
a response. If so, then attack sequences with rapid transitions would not only
evoke later power dives, but also weaker ones, while gradual transitions would
evoke strong power dive responses.
The implications of transition rates on the outcomes of mantis-bat encounters
The proposed design for initiating the mantis power dive based on the
current results suggests how the bat vocalization pattern (and, more
specifically, the transition period) can significantly influence the chance of
a mantis successfully escaping an attacking bat. Attack sequences containing
longer transition periods (such as GT1 and GT2) trigger the response before
the bat gets too close, thus preventing any chance of the bat catching the
mantis.
As bats increase their PRR, they may exhibit a phenomenon known as
strobing, where the bat emits several (2-4) vocalizations with short (15-20
ms) interpulse intervals separated by a single pulse with a longer interpulse
interval (30-40 ms; Moss and Surlykke,
2001). These vocalization clusters, known as strobe groups, may
facilitate the bat's perception of the auditory scene through perceptual
grouping of auditory events, especially when tracking and capturing insects
(Moss and Surlykke, 2001
).
Such grouping of perceptual stimuli is common to both visual and auditory
systems in many animals, including humans
(Bregman, 1990
). Moss and
Surlykke (2001
) demonstrated
that Eptesicus fuscus exhibit strobing behavior, typically during the
transition phase of the attack sequence, when the bat begins pursuing an
insect but before fully committing to the attack (represented by PRRs over 100
pps). Strobing bat attack sequences have transition slopes between 0.94 and
1.45 pps 10 ms-1 (based on examples in
Moss and Surlykke, 2001
).
Attack sequences containing strobe groups provide the best examples of gradual
transitions in natural attack sequences. Although our study did not include
attack sequences containing strobing behavior, GT1, with its long transition
phase (slope=1.78 pps 10 ms-1), best represents such a sequence and
GT1 clearly triggered the mantis evasive response the earliest. Though
strobing behavior could potentially reduce a bat's hunting efficiency, this
would only be true against insects that have the ability to detect the
ultrasonic emissions. Since a large number of night-flying insects lack
ultrasound-sensitivity, the potential benefits of strobing may, in the long
run, be outweighed by the drawbacks of early warning to the
ultrasound-sensitive insects.
The proposed design of the mantis evasive response trigger circuit also means that very short transition periods (as in RT1, RT2 and RT3) may circumvent the mantis auditory defense, allowing the bat to capture a mantis even though the mantis can hear the bat. This circumvention could occur in several ways. A short transition period occurring close to the point of capture is one way a bat attack sequence could thwart the mantis auditory defense. A late transition would allow the bat to close with the mantis without triggering the power dive since vocalization patterns preceding the transition do not trigger mantis power dives. A rapid, late transition to high PRRs when the bat is close to the mantis would allow the bat to capture the mantis even after triggering a power dive during the transition period due to the long latency of the mantis power dive response. A rapid transition occurring farther away could also render the mantis auditory defense ineffective. Vocalizations emitted when the bat is farther away would reduce the sound intensity reaching the mantis and, consequently, 501-T3's response. Even though the transition period normally provides a favorable rate of 501-T3 bursts, the weaker 501-T3 responses might fail to initiate a response or only elicit a weak behavioral response. A rapid transition occurring far away would require the bat to continue pursuing the mantis using very high PRRs. If the bat is capable of echolocating at very high PRRs for long durations, these vocalizations will fail to elicit a mantis response and negate the mantid's auditory defense.
Unlike gradual transitions, it is not clear under what circumstances bats
would use rapid transitions. A bat that makes a late decision to attack an
insect could potentially employ a rapid transition as it tries to capture the
insect. It may also be possible that bats continuously encountering evading
insects with auditory capabilities could potentially learn that attack
sequences with rapid transitions increase their capture success. This would be
another strategy bats could employ to circumvent insect auditory responses
(reviewed in Schnitzler and Kalko,
2001). Field research of bat-insect encounters would provide the
best method for identifying the type of transitions bats use while capturing
insects and under what conditions bats employ both gradual and rapid
transitions.
The effects of intensity on power dive behavioral responses
The results in the present study are based on simulated sequences presented
at a constant intensity of 85 dB SPL, for reasons previously
stated. However, during actual attack sequences, bats systematically change
the intensity of their vocalizations during attacks
(Griffin, 1958;
Simmons et al., 1979
;
Kick, 1982
;
Surlykke and Moss, 2000
;
Boonman and Jones, 2002
) and
the intensity of the vocalizations may exceed 85 dB SPL even during
the intensity changes (Jensen and Miller,
1999
; Boonman and Jones,
2002
; Holderied and von
Helversen, 2003
). 501-T3 does produce more spikes/stimulus in
response to louder single bat vocalizations
(Triblehorn and Yager, 2002
)
and to louder, short, pure tone pulses
(Yager et al., 2001
),
especially when the sounds are <3 ms in duration, which occurs during the
transition phase (see Fig. 1).
However, our simulated sequences (fixed intensity) did produce similar 501-T3
responses to those of actual bat attack sequences (changing intensity) and the
mantis behaviors elicited by the simulated sequences should reflect the
behavior that would be evoked by the actual sequences. Still, it is important
to consider how bat vocalization intensity might influence mantis evasive
responses.
The production of mantis power dives involves both triggering and executing the response. Triggering the power dive will be tightly linked to 501-T3 activity (and potentially other auditory interneurons), while execution involves the activity of descending neurons in the cephalic ganglia and motor neurons (as well as any interposed interneurons), and muscle activation. Our data indicate that triggering is linked to PRR and intensities over 85 dB SPL have little effect on triggering the response. This is evident from the findings that the trigger point already appears to be set to a low PRR (15-40 pps) and that control sequences with a fixed PRR and pulse duration (12 pps, 4 ms) do not trigger the power dive response, even when presented at 95 dB SPL. Since the trigger point is already set to a low PRR, any further reduction could increase the number of false alarms, causing the mantis to perform a power dive before the bat truly poses a threat. The slope of the transition phase also does not alter the triggering point.
Intensity appears to have greater influence in the execution of the
response, affecting both the latency and magnitude of the response. Yager and
May (1990) demonstrated that
higher intensity pulse trains reduce the latency for yaw production in the
mantis evasive response and increase yaw magnitude. Yager et al.
(1990
) also demonstrated that
evasive responses in free-flying mantids become more dramatic as pulse train
intensity increases (from gradual turns to shallow dives to steep power
dives). The results from our intensity experiment indicate that louder
sequences can emit earlier responses and give the mantids more time to escape.
However, this effect varied across mantids (average behavioral responses in
some mantids were unaffected by stimulus intensity) and sequences (increasing
intensity seemed to evoke earlier responses in mantids when exposed to GT1
compared to RT1). Intensities below 85 dB SPL also appear to affect
when behavioral responses occurred during simulated attack sequences more than
intensities over 85 dB SPL. As with the Yager and May
(1990
) study, our data showed
a high degree of variation, which may add an element of unpredictability in
the response that could increase its effectiveness.
Our study did not examine how the transition phase affects response magnitude. However, to address this issue, an ideal experimental paradigm would to record 501-T3 activity in tethered flying mantids while quantifying the magnitude of elicited evasive responses. It is important to know the 501-T3 response pattern that elicits evasive responses, in order to account for the variability that exists in behavioral data alone. These recordings could detect the descending motor command, which would provide more accurate information regarding the trigger point since the biomechanical factors and some of the neural delays that affect the behavioral latency would be eliminated.
Back-up systems and `last-ditch' evasive responses
The results of this study reveal the effectiveness of the mantis evasive
response, but also reveal some vulnerabilities that bats could exploit (either
as a strategy or serendipitously). Such vulnerability could explain, at least
to some extent, why insect auditory defenses are not 100% effective. It also
indicates that mantids could benefit from possessing back-up systems for
initiating `last-ditch' responses. Insects such as arctiid moths and
lacewings, have auditory systems that do mediate `last-ditch' responses,
triggered by the high PRRs emitted by bats late in the attack. However, P.
agrionina does not have an obvious sound-mediated last-chance behavior
like these. Such `last-ditch' responses could be mediated by other sensory
systems such as the mantid's wind-sensitive cercal system. Bats do produce a
substantial amount of air currents before capturing their target that is
detectable by the mantis, and cercal-directed wind puffs elicit brief
wing-beat changes in tethered flying mantids that could alter the flight path
sufficiently in free flight to make it more difficult for the bat to capture
the mantis (Triblehorn,
2003).
![]() |
Acknowledgments |
---|
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Boonman, A. and Jones, G. (2002). Intensity
control during target approach in echolocating bats; stereotypical
sensori-motor behaviour in Daubenton's bats, Myotis daubentonii.
J. Exp. Biol. 205,2865
-2874.
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Eaton, R. C. (1984). Neural Mechanisms of Startle Behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Ehrmann, R. and Roy, R. (2002). Systematische Aufstellung der Gattungen. In Mantodea: Gottesanbiterinnen der Welt (ed. R. Ehrmann), pp. 374-378. Munich: Natur und Teil-Verlag.
Faure, P. A. and Hoy, R. R. (2000a).
Neuroethology of the katydid T-cell. I. Tuning and responses to pure tones.
J. Exp. Biol. 203,3225
-3242.
Faure, P. A. and Hoy, R. R. (2000b).
Neuroethology of the katydid T-cell. II. Responses to acoustic playback of
conspecific and predatory signals. J. Exp. Biol.
203,3243
-3254.
Fullard, J. H. (1984). Listening for bats: pulse repetition rate as a cue for a defensive behavior in Cycnia tenera (Lepidoptera: Arctiidae). J. Comp. Physiol. A 154,249 -252.
Fullard, J. H., Dawson, J. W. and Jacobs, D. S.
(2003). Auditory encoding during the last moment of a moth's
life. J. Exp. Biol. 206,281
-294.
Fullard, J. H., Fenton, M. B. and Simmons, J. A. (1979). Jamming bat echolocation: the clicks of arctiid moths. Can. J. Zool. 57,647 -649.
Fullard, J. H., Simmons, J. A. and Saillant, P. A.
(1994). Jamming bat echolocation: the dogbane tiger moth
Cycnia tenera times its clicks to the terminal attack calls of the
big brown bat Eptesicus fuscus. J. Exp. Biol.
194,285
-298.
Griffin, D. R. (1958). Listening in the Dark. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Holderied, M. W. and von Helversen, O. (2003). Echolocation range and wingbeat period match in aerial-hawking bats. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270,2293 -2299.[CrossRef][Medline]
Hoy, R. R. (1998). Acute as a bug's ear: An informal discussion of hearing in insects. In Comparative Hearing: Insects (ed. R. R. Hoy, A. N. Popper, and R. R. Fay), pp.1 -17. New York: Springer.
Jensen, M. E. and Miller, L. A. (1999). Echolocation signals of the bat Eptesicus serotinus recorded using a vertical microphone array: Effect of flight altitude on searching signals. Behav. Ecol. Sociol. 47,60 -69.[CrossRef]
Kalko, E. K. V. (1995). Insect pursuit, prey capture and echolocation in pipistrelle bats (Microchiroptera). Anim. Behav. 50,861 -880.[CrossRef]
Kalko, E. K. V. and Schnitzler, H.-U. (1989). The echolocation and hunting behavior of Daubenton's bat, Myotis daubentoni. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 24,225 -238.[CrossRef]
Kick, S. A. (1982). Target detection by the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J. Comp. Physiol. A 145,431 -443.
Miller, L. A. and Olesen, J. (1979). Avoidance behavior in green lacewings. I. Behavior of free flying green lacewings to hunting bats and ultrasound. J. Comp. Physiol. 131,113 -120.[CrossRef]
Möhl, B. and Miller, L. A. (1976). Ultrasonic clicks produced by the peacock butterfly: a possible bat-repellent mechanism. J. Exp. Biol. 64,693 -644.
Moiseff, A. and Hoy, R. R. (1983). Sensitivity to ultrasound in an identified auditory interneuron in the cricket: a possible link to phonotactic behavior. J. Comp. Physiol. 152,155 -167.[CrossRef]
Moss, C. F. and Surlykke, A. (2001). Auditory scene analysis by echolocation in bats. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 110,2207 -2226.[CrossRef][Medline]
Nolen, T. G. and Hoy, R. R. (1984). Initiation of behavior by single neurons: The role of behavioral context. Science 226,992 -994.[Medline]
Roeder, K. D. (1967). Nerve Cells and Insect Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Schnitzler, H. U. and Kalko, E. K. V. (2001). Echolocation by insect-eating bats. BioScience 51,557 -569.
Schnitzler, H.-U., Kalko, E., Miller, L. and Surlykke, A. (1980). How the bat, Pipistrellus kuhli, hunts for insects. In Animal Sonar: Processes and Performance (ed. P. E. Nachtigal and P. W. B. Moore), pp.619 -623. New York: Plenum Press.
Simmons, J. A., Fenton, M. B. and O'Farrell, M. J. (1979). Echolocation and pursuit of prey by bats. Science 203,16 -21.[Medline]
Simmons, J. A. and Grinnell, A. S. (1980). The performance of echolocation: Acoustic images perceived by echolocating bats. In Animal Sonar: Processes and Performance (ed. P. E. Nachtigal and P. W. B. Moore), pp. 353-385. New York: Plenum Press.
Surlykke, A. and Moss, C. F. (2000). Echolocation behavior of big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, in the field and the laboratory. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 108,2419 -2429.[CrossRef][Medline]
Triblehorn, J. D. (2003). Multisensory integration in the ultrasound-triggered escape response of the praying mantis, Parasphendale agrionina. Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park, USA.
Triblehorn, J. D. and Yager, D. D. (2001). Broad vs. narrow auditory tuning and corresponding bat-evasive flight behavior in praying mantids. J. Zool. Lond. 254, 27-40.
Triblehorn, J. D. and Yager, D. D. (2002).
Implanted electrode recordings from a praying mantis auditory interneuron
during flying bat attacks. J. Exp. Biol.
205,307
-320.
Yager, D. D. (1999). Structure, development, and evolution of insect auditory systems. Microsc. Res. Tech. 47,380 -400.[CrossRef][Medline]
Yager, D. D. and Hoy, R. R. (1989). Audition in the praying mantis, Mantis religiosa L.: identification of an interneuron mediating ultrasonic hearing. J. Comp. Physiol. A 165,471 -493.[Medline]
Yager, D. D. and May, M. L. (1990). Ultrasound-triggered, flight-gated evasive maneuvers in the praying mantis Parasphendale agrionina. II. Tethered flight. J. Exp. Biol. 152,41 -58.[Abstract]
Yager, D. D., May, M. L. and Fenton, M. B. (1990). Ultrasound-triggered, flight-gated evasive maneuvers in the praying mantis Parasphendale agrionina. I. Free flight. J. Exp. Biol. 152,17 -39.[Abstract]
Yager, D. D., Young, A. L. and Donaldson, B. (2001). Temporal processing by an identified auditory interneuron in the praying mantis. Int. Congr. Neuroethol. Abstr. 6, 109.
Related articles in JEB: