Activation patterns of the tongue-projector muscle during feeding in the imperial cave salamander Hydromantes imperialis
Brain Research Institute, University of Bremen, 28334 Bremen, Germany
* Author for correspondence at present address: Department of Biology, 201 South Biology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT 84102, USA (e-mail: Stephen.Deban{at}utah.edu)
Accepted 12 March 2004
![]() |
Summary |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Key words: electromyography, salamander, Hydromantes imperialis, modulation, feeding, tongue
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
In ballistic tongue projection, the tongue skeleton to which the tongue pad
is attached is shot completely from the mouth of the salamander. To accomplish
this extraordinary feat, the primary tongue projector muscles, the bilaterally
paired subarcualis rectus (SAR), must not be directly attached to the
elongated epibranchials, which they encompass. They must also have a
morphology and activation pattern that allows them to apply force rapidly
enough to accelerate the tongue to high velocity in short distance. Previous
research on Hydromantes has revealed that the muscle fibers of each
SAR wrap in a complex arrangement, circumferentially, around the tapered
epibranchial cartilage (Lombard and Wake,
1977) and that the epibranchial is free to evacuate the muscle
entirely (Deban et al.,
1997
).
Articulating cartilaginous elements make up the tongue skeleton of
terrestrial salamanders. The single basibranchial lies medially in the floor
of the mouth and supports the tongue pad. On each side, a first and second
ceratobranchial articulates with the caudal half of the basibranchial. In
Hydromantes, the basibranchial is approximately twice the length of
the ceratobranchials. An epibranchial articulates with the caudal ends of the
first and second ceratobranchials. The epibranchial in Hydromantes
and bolitoglossines is greatly elongated and extends over the shoulder and
some distance down the trunk. In Hydromantes, the epibranchial is the
longest of any salamander, and tapers from rostral to caudal along its entire
length (Lombard and Wake,
1977).
During tongue projection, the tongue skeleton folds medially, becoming a compact projectile as it is pulled and squeezed forward relative to the ceratohyals. In bolitoglossines, the tongue skeleton is free from the projection muscles and can be projected completely from the mouth in a ballistic fashion. It is tethered to the body of the salamander by a bundle of tissue that includes the retractor muscles (i.e. the rectus cervicis profundus, RCP), blood vessels, nerves and a connective tissue sheath.
The SAR is the primary tongue-protraction muscle; it originates broadly
along the rostrolateral edge of the ceratohyal and travels posteriorly to the
epibranchial. The rostral portion (the subarcualis rectus anterior, SARA) has
a fiber orientation that can pull the tongue skeleton rostral relative to the
ceratohyal, and meets the caudal portion near the joint formed by the
ceratobranchials and the epibranchial. The caudal portion of the SAR
(subarcualis rectus posterior, SARP) forms an elongate sheath around the
epibranchial with a complex segmented arrangement
(Fig. 1) of short muscle fibers
(less than 1 mm long). This arrangement has been proposed to exert radial
forces during contraction that squeeze the tapered epibranchial rostrally and
thus project the tongue from the mouth
(Lombard and Wake, 1977;
Deban et al., 1997
). The
posterior portion of the SAR in Hydromantes and other bolitoglossines
is elongated compared to other taxa, encompassing the entire elongated
epibranchial and the caudal ends of the ceratobranchials, but not directly
attaching to those elements. The arrangement of muscle fibers of the SARP and
the tapered shape of the epibranchial together suggest that the SARP can
squeeze on the epibranchial anywhere along its length to push the tongue
skeleton rostrally.
|
Tongue retraction is accomplished by the RCP, which is a long strap-like
muscle that originates on the pelvis and inserts into the tongue pad. A
portion of the RCP lies in a loop in the throat just in front of the heart
when the tongue is at rest in the mouth in bolitoglossines; the extra length
accommodates long-distance tongue projection. Electromyographic activity
patterns of the RCP were reported previously
(Deban and Dicke, 1999) in
Hydromantes supramontis, a closely related species, and are not
examined in this study.
Previous research into the activation pattern of the SAR has been limited
to electromyographic (EMG) recordings from a single electrode placed at the
center of the muscle (Deban and Dicke,
1999). Given the variation in activation pattern observed, and the
anatomical complexity of the SAR, a more detailed examination of its
activation patterns is necessary to understand its operation.
The objective of the present study is to determine the pattern of activation of different regions of the SAR during tongue projection in Hydromantes. We describe the temporal and spatial pattern of activation using 34 recording sites arrayed along the length of the SAR, and we examine the effects of prey distance and prey-capture success on the pattern of activation. These data are used to test three specific hypotheses regarding the motor control of tongue projection.
The first hypothesis is that the SAR shows a high degree of variation such
that different regions are activated at different times and for different
durations. The muscle is elongated, like the epibranchial, and its morphology
and muscle fiber orientation suggest that motor units are arrayed lengthwise
such that it can apply force to the epibranchial anywhere along its length. We
hypothesize that it will be activated differentially depending on the extent
of tongue projection, with more of the muscle being recruited as greater
tongue projection is required. Previous electromyographic study of
Hydromantes, using one recording site in the SAR, has shown that the
muscle is activated prior to its antagonist, the tongue retractor RCP, and
that the relative onset time and duration of activation of these two muscles
are strongly modulated in response to prey distance
(Deban and Dicke, 1999). We
expect that this modulation extends to recruitment within the SAR.
The second hypothesis, related to the first, is that ballistic and
non-ballistic projection will display discrete muscle activity patterns. In
ballistic projection, the tongue skeleton is shot completely from the SAR
muscle and from the body of the salamander, and it is hence beyond the ability
of the SAR to exert force upon it in the later stages of projection. In
non-ballistic projection, the tongue skeleton is pulled and squeezed forward
by the SAR, but does not leave the SAR, providing an opportunity for
coordination of tongue movement by coactivity of the SAR and its antagonist,
the RCP (tongue retractor). This pattern of decreasing coactivity with
increasing prey distance has been observed in Hydromantes
(Deban and Dicke, 1999). The
greater role for momentum and the lack of coactivity in ballistic projection
leads us to predict that the activity pattern of the SAR will differ in these
two modes of tongue projection. Chameleons use ballistic projection by means
of a similar mechanism and the tongue accelerator muscle is activated all at
once (Wainwright and Bennett,
1992a
). We expect this pattern during ballistic projection in the
SAR of Hydromantes as well. In non-ballistic projection, we expect
that the SAR will be activated partially, and the duration and extent of
activity will increase with increasing distance of tongue projection.
The third hypothesis we tested is that SAR activation pattern will show no
effect of prey-capture success (i.e. misses versus captures). From a
motor control standpoint, this is important because it would indicate whether
tongue projection is controlled using sensory feedback or using a feedforward
mechanism of motor planning (sensu
Marsden et al., 1984;
Pearson, 1993
). Previously,
Hydromantes and other species of plethodontid salamanders showed
almost no effect of prey capture success on the timing of activation of the
SAR (one recording site), the RCP, and the depressor mandibulae (Deban and
Dicke, 1997). Additionally, transecting the sensory nerves to the tongue had
no effect on feeding kinematics in another plethodontid species
(Deban, 1997
). Based on these
previous results, we expect that motor patterns within the SAR will not be
influenced by capture success, and that activation of the SAR will therefore
be accomplished via feedforward control.
The goals of this project were thus to determine the electromyographic (EMG) pattern(s) within the primary tongue protractor muscle, the SAR, in a ballistic-tongued salamander, Hydromantes imperialis, and to test three hypotheses: (1) the SAR shows regional variation in the timing and duration of activation, strongly influenced by tongue-projection distance, (2) ballistic and non-ballistic projection are accomplished with discrete activation patterns of the SAR and (3) tongue projection (both ballistic and non-ballistic) is controlled via feedforward rather than feedback mechanisms, that is, SAR activation patterns are not influenced by capture success.
![]() |
Materials and methods |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Morphology
The main tongue-protractor muscle, the SAR, was chosen for EMG recordings.
To determine the approximate tongue-projection distance at which the
epibranchial leaves the posterior SAR (SARP), we cleared and double stained
(for bone and cartilage) a preserved specimen of Hydromantes
imperialis with a snoutvent length of 73 mm. We measured the
length of the tongue skeleton at 35 mm and the distance from the tongue tip to
the rostral margin of the jaws at 20 mm when the epibranchial tips were
aligned with the posterior tips of the lower jaws. This position for the
tongue skeleton approximates the position it is in during tongue projection
when the epibranchial tips are at the rostral edge of the SARP muscles. Tongue
projection beyond this distance of 20 mm relies on the momentum of the tongue
skeleton (i.e. ballistic projection) because the SARP muscles can no longer
exert force upon the epibranchials.
Electrode implantation
Formvar-coated nichrome wire of 25 µm uncoated (38 µm coated)
diameter (A-M Systems #761500, Sequim, WA, USA) was used to construct bipolar
patch electrodes. Electrodes were made by twisting together two strands of
wire approximately 60 cm long, which were then threaded through a 3 mmx3
mm piece of silicone rubber in the shape of either a half cylinder (cut from
tubing) or a flat square. Insulation was removed from approximately 2 mm of
both wires on the inner surface of the electrode. Dipole orientation of the
cylindrical patch electrodes was perpendicular to the long axis of the
half-cylinder, and thus was parallel to the outermost muscle fibers of the SAR
upon implantation.
Prior to electrode implantation, salamanders were anesthetized by immersion in a buffered 2% aqueous solution of MS-222 (3-aminobenzoic acid ethyl ester; Sigma) for 1030 min. Electrodes were implanted through three or four small incisions in the skin, at the surface of the muscles. An electrode was placed against the anterior portion of the SAR in two individuals through an incision in the skin of the throat. In all five individuals, three patch electrodes were placed against the surface of the right SAR, with the concave surface of the electrode cradling a portion of the cylindrical muscle. Electrodes were positioned with approximately equal spacing along the SAR muscle, with the anterior electrode placed at the level of the gular fold. The SAR has been observed to move rostrally during tongue projection, but because the electrodes were not attached to the muscle, the muscle was free to move relative to them. This arrangement was necessary given the small size and delicate nature of the muscle, which prohibited electrode implantation directly into the muscle. The posterior electrode was positioned several mm rostral to the caudal tip of the SAR to accommodate muscle movement (up to 0.5 cm, see below) and to remain in contact with the muscle throughout tongue projection and retraction. The middle electrode was positioned approximately midway between the anterior and posterior electrodes, such that electrodes were spaced about 7 mm apart (Fig. 2).
|
Incisions were closed with silk suture. Electrode leads were glued together with modeling glue and attached to the skin of the back with suture to prevent them from being pulled loose. The ends of the leads were soldered to an 8-pin connector, which was plugged directly into the preamplifier.
Electromyography
Salamanders fed readily after recovery from anesthesia. Recordings were
made within 3 days of recovery, after which electrode positions and spacing
were confirmed surgically. A total of 73 feedings (43 captures, 30 misses)
were recorded. Numbers of recorded captures (and misses) for each individual
were 8 (2), 18 (16), 12 (2), 4 (7) and 1 (3).
Electromyographic signals were amplified 1000 times by a custom-built differential preamplifier-amplifier. Signals were recorded on an Instrutech (Port Washington, New York, USA) CRC VR-100A digital recorder connected to a JVC HR-S700 video cassette recorder, from which they were captured at a rate of 2000 samples s1 using a National Instruments data acquisition card and LabVIEW 4.0 (National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA) software running in Windows 98. The raw signals were filtered in LabVIEW to remove 50 Hz line noise, other noise, and low-frequency movement artifacts. Filtered signals were then rectified for analysis.
Each salamander was permitted unrestrained movement in a 20 cmx20 cm plastic box with a grounded substrate of moist graph paper while EMG recordings were made. Live crickets were presented to the salamander by dropping them on the substrate at varying distances in front of the salamander. Strikes were recorded as either a successful capture or a miss. The distance from the salamander to the prey at the time of the salamander's strike was estimated, using the graph paper grid, to the nearest 5 mm.
To correlate tongue movement and EMG activity, 10 feedings from two individuals were videotaped in dorsal view at 60 Hz during EMG recording. Video and EMG were synchronized with an LED in the video field and a pulse on one channel of the EMG recording. In these individuals, the SARP and epibranchial contralateral to the instrumented muscle were exposed by pulling the free posterior end through a small incision at the shoulder. The muscle was kept moist by application of Ringer's solution. Because the muscle and the contained cartilage are normally covered only by skin and are unattached posteriorly, this procedure is unlikely to have affected their function or movement. Tongue projection and prey-capture performance were not noticeably altered by this procedure. Rostrocaudal movement of the SAR and the epibranchial within the translucent muscle were observed in this way. These were the same two individuals that had an electrode on the SARA, and the combined video and EMG data were used to determine the timing of activation at this site relative to the most posterior site in the SARP and relative to SAR and tongue movements.
Five measurements were made from the EMG burst associated with the prey-capture strike for each electrode: (1) time of the onset of activity, the time at which activity exceeded background noise levels by twofold for at least 10 ms, (2) time of the offset of activity, the time at which activity dropped below two times background noise levels for at least 10 ms, (3) burst area, the integrated area under the curve between times 1 and 2, (4) time of peak activity, the starting time of the 10 ms period between times 1 and 2 with the greatest integrated area and (5) peak amplitude, the average amplitude of the 10 ms period in (4).
Twelve timing variables were calculated from these five measurements: (1) duration of activity at each electrode, the onset time minus the offset time (three variables), (2) anteriorposterior relative duration, the duration of the posterior electrode minus the duration of the anterior electrode, (3) anteriormiddle relative duration, (4) middleposterior relative duration, (5) anteriorposterior relative onset, the onset time of the posterior electrode minus the onset time of the anterior electrode, (6) anteriormiddle relative onset, (7) middleposterior relative onset, (8) anteriorposterior relative peak, the time of peak activity of the posterior electrode minus the time of peak activity of the anterior electrode, (9) anteriormiddle relative peak and (10) middleposterior relative peak.
Statistical analyses
Statistical analyses were performed to examine effects of individual,
electrode position, capture success and initial prey distance. Analyses
included 18 variables: burst duration for each of the three electrodes (three
variables), burst area for each electrode (three variables), peak amplitude
for each electrode (three variables), relative durations between pairs of
electrodes (three variables), relative onset times between pairs of electrodes
(three variables), and relative peak times between pairs of electrodes (three
variables). These comparisons were conducted on all trials in all
individuals.
Three statistical analyses were conducted. First, to test the hypotheses
that individual, prey distance and capture success influence EMG patterns
within the SAR, we conducted an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) examining the
effects of individual and capture success, with prey distance as a covariate,
on each of the 18 variables individually. Two-way interactions (individual
x capture, prey distance x capture and individual x prey
distance) were also examined, but because none were significant they were
removed from the model to increase statistical power. The `individual' term in
the analyses accounted for differences in both the electromyographic responses
of the salamanders and the properties of the electrodes (e.g. variation in
sensitivity and exact position along the SAR) when comparing individuals. The
P0.05 significance level was adjusted in the ANCOVA using the
simultaneous Bonferroni correction for experiment-wide error
(Sokal and Rohlf, 1995
) to a
level of P
0.0028. The ANCOVA was also run with only the 43
feedings in which the prey was captured with individual and prey distance
terms in the model.
In the second analysis, burst duration, burst area and relative onset variables (all trials) were plotted against initial prey distance and least-squares regression lines were fitted, to visualize the effects of prey distance on each of the variables independently.
Third, t-tests were conducted on the two individuals with electrodes in the SARA together and separately to test the hypothesis that the time of onset, time of peak activity and duration of activity were significantly different from those of the posterior recording site (SARP Post.). One-sample tests were used to determine if relative time of onset, relative time of peak activity and relative duration averages were significantly different from zero, to test the hypotheses that the activity onset and peak were reached at different times, and that the duration of activity varied, along the length of the SARP.
Statistical analyses were performed on an Apple Macintosh PowerBook G4
using StatView software version 5.0. The sums of squares of the ANCOVAs were
calculated by the reduction technique
(Searle, 1971). The algorithms
used can be found in the StatView Reference 1998, SAS Institute
Inc.
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
The buccal SARA site was the first to show activation during feeding, and the site remained active until prey contact. After the SARA, the posterior SARP site was activated, followed by the middle SARP site, then the anterior SARP site. The relative onset times between sites diminished with increasing prey distance, until, at maximum tongue projection distance the sites were activated nearly simultaneously. The duration of activation at all sites also increased with prey distance, but the SARP sites were deactivated well before the SARA site was deactivated.
Buccal SARA activity compared to SARP activity
In the two individuals in which the SARP was exposed during feeding, the
SARP was observed to move rostrally by 0.31±0.06 cm (00.5 cm),
following the onset of activity in the SARA by 110±21 ms (23186
ms). The tongue first appeared at the mouth 123±13 ms (80180 ms)
after the onset of activation in the posterior electrode of the SARP, and
107±16 ms (66188 ms) after activity began at the anterior
electrode of the SARP. Tongue appearance occurred 2±13 ms after (58 ms
before to 40 ms after) activity ended in the SARA, with short distance
feedings (0.51.5 cm) showing activity in the SARA even after the tongue
reached the prey.
The hypothesis that the buccal portion of the SAR (the SARA) is activated separately from the posterior portion was tested with one-sample t-tests (two-tailed). The t-tests revealed that activity was detected in the SARA electrode significantly earlier (42±4 ms) than in the most posterior SARP electrode in both individuals together (t=11.529 and P<0.0001), and in each individual separately (42±5 ms; t=9.042; P<0.0001 and 43±6 ms; t=7.018; P=0.0059). The timing of peak activity, however, was not significantly different between the SARA and the posterior SARP recording site for both individuals (t=0.440; P=0.6664) or for one individual (t=0.815; P=0.4341), but was significantly different for the other individual (t=4.061; P=0.0269), with the peak occurring 28±7 ms later in the SARA. Duration of activity was significantly longer at the SARA site than in the posterior SARP site, in both individuals (80±8 ms longer; t=10.401; P<0.0001) and in each individual separately (86±8 ms; t=10.357; P<0.0001 and 61±15 ms; t=4.001; P=0.0280). The anterior site of the SARP was active the latest, on average, and the time of deactivation of the SARA was later than the time of deactivation of the anterior SARP for one of the two individuals (t=5.669 and P=0.0002).
EMG variation among individuals
The ANCOVAs revealed significant effects of individual in nine of the 18
variables (Table 1). Effects of
`individual' in the analysis account for both effects due to variation among
the individual salamanders and random effects due to variation in electrode
characteristics. Therefore, the details of which pair-wise individual
differences are driving the overall effect of individual are not informative
and are not discussed further.
|
Posterior-to-anterior activation within the SARP
Average onset time, peak time and duration of the SAR muscle were
significantly variable along its length. All relative onset and relative
duration averages were significantly different from zero, indicating the
recording sites were activated at different times and for different durations
(Table 2). The posterior
recording site showed the earliest activity, defined as t=0 ms,
followed by the middle site (t=9±1 ms, 6 to 43 ms),
followed by the anterior site (t=17±2 ms, 9 to 50 ms),
indicating that the SARP was activated in a posterior-to-anterior (i.e.
caudal-to-cranial) wave. The posterior site showed the longest duration of
activity (115±4 ms, 53194 ms), followed by the middle site
(106±4 ms, 29193 ms), and the anterior site (85±4 ms,
27169 ms). None of the relative peak variables were significantly
different from zero, indicating that different regions of the SARP reached
peak activation at essentially the same time.
|
Effects of capture success and prey distance on SARP activation
Of the 73 feeding attempts for which EMGs were recorded, 30 were strikes in
which the prey was not captured. The ANCOVA which included a `capture success'
effect revealed that missing the prey had no significant influence on any of
the 18 EMG variables (Table
1).
Unlike capture success, variation in prey distance had a significant effect on several of the variables, including durations, areas, relative durations and relative onsets. Amplitudes and relative peak variables were not affected significantly (Table 1). None of the interaction terms in the ANCOVA were significant for any variable, including the `prey distance x capture success' effect. This indicates that prey distance had no differential effect on the EMG patterns in captures versus misses.
EMG area increased significantly with increasing prey distance at the middle site (F=62.206; P<0.0001) and the anterior site (F=88.005; P<0.0001), but area from the posterior electrode did not (F=8.185; P=0.0057) (Table 1; Fig. 3Aiiii). All three sites, posterior (F=37.307; P<0.0001), middle (F=64.784; P<0.0001) and anterior (F=111.174; P<0.0001), displayed an increase in the duration of activity with increasing prey distance.
|
The relative duration of activity of the posterior site decreased with increasing prey distance compared to both the middle (F=14.820; P=0.0003) and anterior sites (F=20.020; P<0.0001), due to the fact that the duration of activity at the posterior site increased at a slower rate than the other sites with increasing prey distance (Table 1; Figs 3Ciiii, 4Aiiii). The relative duration of the middle and anterior sites did not show an effect of prey distance (F=2.300; P=0.1342). Relative onset variables were all affected significantly (P<0.0001) by prey distance, such that there was less delay in activation between recording sites as prey distance increased (Table 1; Figs 4Biiii, 5). Relative peak, on the other hand, showed no significant influence of variation in prey distance (Fig. 4Ciiii).
|
|
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Buccal SARA versus SARP activation
The SARA is activated significantly earlier than the SARP, and its activity
overlaps extensively with that of the SARP. This result supports the
hypothesis that the SAR is divided into two functional components: the
anterior SAR, which pulls the tongue skeleton forward relative to the lower
jaw and stabilizes the entire SAR against the `recoil' forces generated when
the second component, the SARP, squeezes the tongue skeleton forward and out
of the mouth. In animals that were videotaped during feeding, the SARP was
observed moving rostrally prior to the epibranchial evacuating the muscle,
which is consistent with the activation of the SARA prior to activation of the
SARP. In addition, the SARA is innervated in a closely related species,
Hydromantes italicus, by a separate branch of the glossopharyngeal
nerve (cranial nerve IX) from the SARP (G. Westhoff, personal communication),
lending anatomical support to the hypothesis that this portion is capable of
being controlled separately from the SARP.
Consistent with a stabilizing role for the SARA are three additional pieces
of evidence. First, the SARA mechanically connects the SARP to the ceratohyal
(Lombard and Wake, 1977;
Deban et al., 1997
) and
therefore must transmit any force generated by the SARP to the ceratohyal;
thus, it must be active during and prior to activation of the SARP, so as to
transmit and not absorb the energy generated by the SARP. Second, the SARA is
active earlier than the SARP and for a longer duration, completely
encompassing the time that the SARP is active
(Fig. 5). Third, the SARA
contains slow tonic as well as fast twitch muscle fibers in this portion of
the SAR in Hydromantes italicus; in the SARP, only fast twitch fibers
have been found (Dicke et al.,
1995
).
The extensive overlap in EMG activity of the SARA and SARP suggests that
the SARA may also play a role in accelerating the tongue, synergistically with
the SARP, which is consistent with the presence of fast fibers in both parts
of the muscle (Dicke et al.,
1995). Co-activation of these two parts of the SAR muscle may
contribute additively to the acceleration of the tongue skeleton, with the
SARA pulling the SARP and enclosed tongue skeleton rostrally relative to the
ceratohyals, and the SARP concurrently squeezing the epibranchial
rostrally.
No effect of capture success on SAR activation
The lack of an effect of capture success on any of the EMG variables is
consistent with previous results in Hydromantes supramontis
(Deban and Dicke, 1999), in
which muscle activity pattern was independent of capture success and the motor
program was therefore not influenced by sensory feedback. Prey capture
kinematics in another plethodontid salamander, Ensatina
eschscholtzii, were unchanged when sensory feedback was prevented by
lingual nerve transection (Deban,
1997
). These salamanders thus appear to modulate their feeding
behavior by changes in motor pattern, but in a feed-forward manner using motor
programming (Marsden et al.,
1984
; Pearson,
1993
; Deban et al.,
2001
) as opposed to using sensory or proprioceptive feedback
during the strike.
Diverse activation patterns within the SARP
Both sequential and simultaneous activation of different regions of the
SARP were observed in Hydromantes. The average activity pattern of
the SARP (with an average prey distance of 2.3±0.14 cm) is a
posterior-to-anterior wave of activation and a decreasing duration of activity
from posterior to anterior. The SARP is thus activated regionally to exert
force first on the posterior tip of the epibranchial, and subsequently on more
rostral portions of the epibranchial. Because the epibranchial moves rostrally
when the SARP squeezes it, the posterior tip of the SARP must be activated
first, because the epibranchial soon evacuates this portion of the muscle,
even in modest tongue protraction.
Sequential activation of the SARP occurs during feedings in which the tongue is projected short distances (i.e. a few cm), but the pattern changes when the tongue is projected farther. The delay in activation of segments of the SARP decreases with increasing prey distance, as do the differences in the regional durations of activity. Thus, the longest-distance tongue projections are accomplished by simultaneous activation of the entire SARP. Absolute durations of activity at all sites on the SARP also increase with prey distance. This pattern indicates that the SARP exerts force simultaneously for a longer period of time along the entire length of the epibranchial when the tongue is shot to its maximum extent. These changes are not abrupt, as we hypothesized, but gradual, suggesting that the biomechanical transition from non-ballistic to ballistic projection is not discrete.
These different patterns of activation make sense in light of observations
of Hydromantes in which short-distance tongue projection is
relatively slow and precise (even appearing `leisurely'), and the tongue is
placed accurately on the prey and rarely overshoots it (this study and
Deban and Dicke, 1999). In
short-distance feedings, tongue movement must be braked and its direction of
travel must be reversed soon after tongue protraction begins, by
co-contraction of the antagonistic rectus cervicis profundus (RCP) muscles
(Deban and Dicke, 1999
).
Applying less projection force for a briefer duration when antagonistic
muscles are soon to be activated to reverse the tongue trajectory is a
sensible motor control strategy. Sequential and orderly longitudinal
activation of the SARP may also smooth force transmission to the tongue
skeleton and, combined with modulated RCP activity, may permit more precise
control of tongue-projection force, distance and velocity in short-distance
feedings.
Long distance projection (i.e. greater than about 3 cm), on the other hand,
requires that the tongue skeleton be launched ballistically (i.e. the
epibranchial evacuating both the SARP and the mouth completely and the tongue
traveling to the prey under its own momentum; see
Deban et al., 1997). Ballistic
projection can only be accomplished by high accelerations and velocities of
the tongue skeleton, which require that the entire SARP exert force
simultaneously along the length of the epibranchial, rather than piecemeal as
in feedings at shorter distances. This conclusion is supported by the increase
in the duration and area of EMG activity with increasing prey distance
(Fig. 3). The faster movement
of the epibranchial associated with longer distance tongue projection requires
that the activation bursts along the length of the SARP `bunch up' and overlap
more extensively in time. By contracting in its entirety, rather than
piecemeal, the SARP exerts maximal force upon the epibranchial. By increasing
its duration of contraction (made possible in Hydromantes by the
lengthy epibranchial) the SARP imparts the greatest impulse (force x
time) to the tongue skeleton, which is critical for rapid acceleration of the
tongue. Also critical for maximum acceleration of the tongue is the lack of
any activity in the antagonistic RCP during projection, a pattern that has
previously been observed (Deban and Dicke,
1999
).
Separate motor units within a muscle are necessary to produce a pattern of
regional activation such as that displayed by the SARA and SARP of
Hydromantes. The motor units of the SARP are probably arranged
longitudinally, based on the longitudinal progression of muscle activation.
Although the spatial arrangement of muscle fibers is unusual and complex in
the SARP, the arrangement of motor units is not functionally unusual for a
vertebrate muscle. In the cat sartorius muscle, for example, the motor units
extend from origin to insertion (Smits et
al., 1994) and therefore each motor unit can contribute to joint
torque. Likewise, in the SARP, each putative motor unit (i.e. each
longitudinal `segment') can exert force separately upon the epibranchial and,
because the epibranchial is tapered, each can contribute to tongue
protraction.
The tapered epibranchial can serve an additional function in tongue
projection, by altering the relationship between SARP contraction and movement
of the tongue tip. Because the muscle fibers of the SARP are short and are
arranged mostly in parallel, there are relatively few sarcomeres in series.
The SARP as a whole is therefore suited to high force production and low
velocity of contraction (Josephson,
1975). Yet, tongue projection is extremely rapid. The gently
tapered epibranchial (which the SARP squeezes) reduces the mechanical
advantage of the muscle by an amount that depends on the local slope of
epibranchial taper. The high-force, low-velocity contraction of the SAR is
thus converted to low-force, high-velocity movement of the tongue skeleton.
This effect is exaggerated by the medial folding of the tongue skeleton, which
thrusts the basibranchial and the attached tongue pad forward faster than the
epibranchial moves. The strain rate of the SARP fibers, therefore, may not
need to be especially high to produce extremely fast tongue projection.
Comparison to other systems
No previous studies have examined regional activation of tongue-projector
muscles in salamanders, making it impossible to compare the results from
Hydromantes to other taxa. We suspect that the results will be
similar in other salamanders with ballistic tongues. Within the
Plethodontidae, ballistic tongue projection has evolved at least twice
(independently in the Bolitoglossini and the Hemidactyliini). The morphology
of the tongue system is highly variable among the hundreds of species of
plethodontids, providing many opportunities for future comparative
evolutionary research in this group.
Like bolitoglossine salamanders, chameleons are also capable of ballistic
tongue projection, but their tongue-projection system is reversed compared to
salamanders. The tongue-accelerator muscle leaves the mouth with the tongue,
and the cartilage it squeezes stays in the body. The accelerator muscle
(analogous to the SARP of bolitoglossines) resides inside the tongue pad and
is activated approximately 300 ms before the tongue leaves the mouth,
remaining active until then (Wainwright
and Bennett, 1992a). The delay between activation and projection
has been explained as the time required for the cylindrical accelerator muscle
to elongate so that it reaches the tip of the entoglossal cartilage
(Wainwright and Bennett,
1992b
). Once the muscle reaches the tip, it squeezes itself off
the entoglossus and thus launches the tongue pad to which it is attached. The
tongue pad does not accelerate prior to reaching the entoglossal tip, because
the entoglossus is parallel-sided and the squeezing force produced by the
muscle produces no net propulsive force. An alternative explanation for the
300 ms activation-projection delay is that the accelerator muscle changes
shape and thereby stretches elastic structures within it
(de Groot and van Leeuwen,
2004
). When the accelerator slides over the end of the
entoglossus, the elastic structures recoil, releasing the stored potential
energy as kinetic energy and thereby amplifying the power output (i.e. the
rate of energy release) of the accelerator muscle. This mechanism can explain
the extremely high power output observed during tongue projection that cannot
be attributed to direct muscular action (de
Groot and van Leeuwen, 2004
).
The delay between muscle activation and tongue projection in
Hydromantes is less than that of the chameleon, mean 123±13 ms
(80180 ms). Nonetheless, given the smaller size and consequently more
rapid movements of the salamanders compared to the chameleons, this delay is
sufficiently long to implicate elastic energy storage and release, and to
raise the possibility of power amplification. Simultaneous activation of the
entire SARP in long-distance feedings is similar to the pattern of
simultaneous activation of different regions of the accelerator muscle of the
chameleon (Wainwright and Bennett,
1992a), further suggesting biomechanical similarities between
these two taxa. In the chameleon, the latch that prevents the tongue from
protruding while the accelerator muscle changes shape is the parallel-sided
shape of the entoglossus, and the elastic structures are collaginous sheaths
that reside in the lumen of the accelerator muscle
(de Groot and van Leeuwen,
2004
). If elastic energy storage and release were also operating
in Hydromantes, as has been suggested
(van Leeuwen et al., 2000
),
both a latch and elastic structures would also be required. In
Hydromantes, the latch cannot be the epibranchial itself because it
is tapered over its entire length, rather than parallel sided. The data
presented here (particularly the >100 ms activationprojection delay)
suggest either an elastic mechanism of tongue projection in
Hydromantes, or, alternatively, a pre-loading mechanism in which
shortening of the muscle fibers is prevented until optimum tension is
achieved. To distinguish between these possibilities, future research will
need to determine if power amplification is occurring, as in the chameleon. If
so, the next step would be to clearly identify morphological structures that
can operate as a spring and a latch.
![]() |
Acknowledgments |
---|
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Deban, S. M. (1997). Modulation of prey-capture
behavior in the plethodontid salamander Ensatina eschscholtzii. J.
Exp. Biol. 200,1951
-1964.
Deban, S. M. (2002). Constraint and convergence in the evolution of salamander feeding. In Vertebrate Biomechanics and Evolution (ed. J.-P. Gasc, A. Casinos and V. L. Bels), pp.163 -180. Oxford: BIOS Scientific Publishers.
Deban, S. M. and Dicke, U. (1999). Motor
control of tongue movement during prey capture in plethodontid salamanders.
J. Exp. Biol. 202,3699
-3714.
Deban, S. M., Wake, D. B. and Roth, G. (1997). Salamander with a ballistic tongue. Nature 389, 27-28.[Medline]
Deban, S. M., O'Reilly, J. C. and Nishikawa, K. C. (2001). The evolution of the motor control of feeding in amphibians. Am. Zool. 41,1280 -1298.
de Groot, J. H. and van Leeuwen, J. L. (2004). Evidence for an elastic projection mechanism in the chameleon tongue. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271,761 -770.[CrossRef]
Dicke, U., Mühlenbrock-Lenter, S. and Roth, G. (1995). Fiber types of muscles of the feeding apparatus in plethodontid salamanders. In Proceedings of the 23rd Göttingen Neurobiology Conference; Vol.II (ed. N. Elsner and R. Menzel), p.227 . Stuttgart, New York: Georg Thieme.
Duellman, W. E. and Trueb, L. (1986). Biology of Amphibians. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Elwood, J. R. L. and Cundall, D. (1994). Morphology and behavior of the feeding apparatus in Cryptobranchus alleganiensis (Amphibia: Caudata). J. Morphol. 220, 47-70.
Erdman, S. and Cundall, D. (1984). The feeding apparatus of the salamander Amphiuma tridactylum: morphology and behavior. J. Morphol. 181,175 -204.
Josephson, R. K. (1975). Extensive and intensive factors determining the performance of striated muscle. J. Exp. Zool. 194,135 -154.[Medline]
Larsen, J. H., Jr. and Beneski, J. T., Jr. (1988). Quantitative analysis of feeding kinematics in dusky salamanders (Desmognathus). Can. J. Zool. 66,1309 -1317.
Lombard, R. E. and Wake, D. B. (1976). Tongue evolution in the lungless salamanders, Family Plethodontidae. I. Introduction, theory and a general model of dynamics. J. Morphol. 148,265 -286.[Medline]
Lombard, R. E. and Wake, D. B. (1977). Tongue evolution in the lungless salamanders, Family Plethodontidae. II. Function and evolutionary diversity. J. Morphol. 153, 39-80.[Medline]
Maglia, A. M. and Pyles, R. A. (1995). Modulation of prey-capture behavior in Plethodon cinereus (Green) (Amphibia: Caudata). J. Exp. Zool. 272,167 -183.
Marsden, C. D., Rothwell, J. C. and Day, B. L. (1984). The use of peripheral feedback in the control of movement. Trends Neurosci. 7, 253-258.
Miller, B. T. and Larsen, J. H., Jr. (1990). Comparative kinematics of terrestrial prey capture in salamanders and newts (Amphibia: Urodela: Salamandridae). J. Exp. Zool. 256,135 -153.
Pearson, K. G. (1993). Common principles of motor control in vertebrates and invertebrates. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 16,265 -297.[CrossRef][Medline]
Reilly, S. M. and Lauder, G. V. (1989). Physiological bases of feeding behavior in salamanders: do motor patterns vary with prey type? J. Exp. Biol. 141,343 -358.
Reilly, S. M. and Lauder, G. V. (1992). Morphology, behavior, and evolution: comparative kinematics of aquatic feeding in salamanders. Brain Behav. Evol. 40,182 -196.[Medline]
Searle, S. R. (1971). Linear Models. John Wiley & Sons, New York.
Shaffer, H. B. and Lauder, G. V. (1985). Patterns of variation in aquatic ambystomatid salamanders: kinematics of the feeding mechanism. Evolution 39, 83-92.
Smits, E., Rose, P. K., Gordon, T. and Richmond, F. J. R.
(1994). Organization of single motor units in feline sartorius.
J. Neurophys. 72,1885
-1896.
Sokal, R. R. and Rohlf, F. J. (1995). Biometry (third edition). New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
Thexton, A. J., Wake, D. B. and Wake, M. H. (1977). Tongue function in the salamander Bolitoglossa occidentalis. Arch. Oral Biol. 22,361 -366.[CrossRef][Medline]
van Leeuwen, J. L., De Groot, J. H. and Kier, W. M. (2000). Evolutionary mechanics of protrusible tentacles and tongues. Neth. J. Zool. 50,113 -139.[CrossRef]
Wainwright, P. C. and Bennett, A. F. (1992a). The mechanism of tongue projection in chameleons. 1. Electromyographic tests of functional hypotheses. J. Exp. Biol. 168, 1-21.
Wainwright, P. C. and Bennett, A. F. (1992b). The Mechanism of tongue projection in chameleons. 2. Role of shape change in a muscular hydrostat. J. Exp. Biol. 168, 23-40.
Related articles in JEB: