The use of viewing posture to control visual processing by lateralised mechanisms
1 Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università degli Studi di
Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35131, Padua, Italy
2 Sussex Centre for Neuroscience, School of Biological Sciences, University
of Sussex, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK
* Author for correspondence (e-mail: ltommasi{at}unipd.it )
Accepted 27 February 2002
![]() |
Summary |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
A standard set of head positions were used, particularly by SL and NOL chicks, showing that the head was aligned with some reference point, perhaps the centre of the dish. These fell into two series (used by both eyes), and in both the peaks of frequent use were 10° apart. One (20°, 10°) was probably generated by head saccades ending with the bill pointing directly at the dish (0°). The other (35°, 25°, 15°) is best explained by slight divergence of the optic axes. The 25° right eye position was consistently used by STR chicks at the beginning of approach. The STR condition requires the most difficult manipulation, and the manipulandum is most obvious from a distance. This is consistent with right eye use during establishment of a motor plan. Head postures consistent with divergence were also used when close to the dish under conditions where choice between targets was necessary. This was clear in the NOL condition, where the chick could see the food grains as it approached. Here, it is likely that both eyes are used in independent search. If so, it may be that divergence is used as a strategy during establishment of a motor plan (as in STR chicks) to increase the independence of the right eye system.
Key words: eye use, visuomotor control, lateralisation, chick, Gallus gallus
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
We recently (Andrew et al.,
2000) showed that chicks use the RE in an approach to a visible
target that they intend to manipulate with the bill, but the LE in very
similar approaches, when they cannot see the target that has to be
manipulated. Here, it seems that the processes of decision include the
establishment and maintenance of a motor plan. We argued (see also
Andrew, 2002
) that such
lateralised visual control of motor response is likely to be the most ancient
form of motor lateralisation.
We present here new experiments, and also further data and analysis from an
experiment that was briefly and partially reported in Andrew et al.
(2000). We show that different
viewing postures (within the overall use of either the LE or RE) are used when
establishing the motor plan, and when maintaining it. Further, head postures
during viewing allow us to distinguish periods when a motor plan is set up
from ones in which a decision is being carried out or a specific target for
response is sought.
![]() |
Materials and methods |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Chicks were trained singly on days 3, 4 and 5 of life. Food was removed 2h before training, which occurred in a rectangular arena (32 cmx90 cm) in a quiet experimental room. The arena (Fig. 1) was lit from above by fluorescent ceiling lights, and indirectly by incandescent bulbs on two sides of the room, arranged so that the arena was lit uniformly. A black cylindrical pot (height 2.5 cm, diameter 3 cm) containing a shallow layer of food was placed in the middle of one of the shorter walls. The chick was placed at the opposite end of the arena from the dish, at the mid point of the wall, and facing the dish, which was approximately 30 cm away along the major axis of the arena.
|
On day 3, a row of food crumbs led the chick directly to the food dish. In those conditions in which the dish was covered by a light paper lid, there were also crumbs on the lid. When the chick pecked these, the lid usually fell off, revealing the food inside the dish, from which the chick then fed readily. The amount of food present on the floor and lid was progressively reduced in later trials, until the chick ran directly to the dish, removed the lid (if one was present), and fed from the dish. There were three training sessions per day, each involving at least six runs to the dish. The chick was allowed to feed for several seconds, and then was held away from the dish with one hand whilst any crumbs that had been thrown to the floor were removed. The final session on day 5 (test session) was recorded by a video camera placed directly above the arena. This session was made up of 4-7 trials, according to the readiness of the chick to continue running to the dish.
Four conditions were used (Fig. 1). Each chick was trained and tested under only one condition. In the no lid (NOL: 16 chicks) condition the dish was uncovered. In the square lid (SL: 19 chicks) condition, the dish was covered with a square lid (3 cmx3 cm), placed symmetrically so that one corner faced the chick, and two others were at the sides of the dish. The lid projected enough for it to be removed by a peck or sideways blow at a variety of points around its perimeter. In the U-shaped lid (UL: 23 chicks) condition, the lid was circular (4 cm diameter) with a rounded indentation that was positioned directly facing the chick. The lid could most easily be removed by pushing the bill into the space provided by the indentation. In the string lid (STR: 8 chicks) condition, the dish was covered with a circular lid (3 cm diameter) that did not protrude over the edge of the dish. It could be removed most easily by grasping a piece of white string (3 cm long, 3 mm diameter) that was attached to the centre of the lid. Its tip was marked with a black dot that evoked pecking and so facilitated learning. The tip turned over and was directed towards the chick as it approached.
NOL were divided into two batches. In the first, eight NOL were compared with seven SL, and in the second, eight NOL were compared with eight STR. Data from two other batches of UL and from one batch of SL were combined with that from the first two to produce bodies of data for within-condition analyses.
The video recordings were analysed, taking measurements as the chick crossed each of three reference lines running perpendicular to the direct line from the start point to the dish centre. These were 10 cm (DIST), 20 cm (MID) and 27 cm (CLOSE) from the start point (Distant: DIST; Mid: MID; Close: CLOSE). The CLOSE point was just before the dish was reached. The measurements taken were the displacement of the centre point of the head from the direct line of approach, and the angle between the longitudinal axis of the head (bill tip to head centre) and a line from the head centre to the centre of the dish. Head angles were measured to the nearest degree and then assigned to a series of 5° bins, starting with `binocular' (3° left to 3° right), and then in successive blocks (3-7°, 8-12°, 13-17° and so on). These are termed 5°, 10° and 15° (and so on) from their central angle.
Mean displacement and mean angle were calculated for each chick for each of
the three reference lines in the approach, using data from all the test
trials. Measures of right or left bias were based on the number of trials in
which each was shown, converted to a ratio as (right/right+left). Overall
heterogeneity between lid conditions was examined with general linear model
analysis (SPSS for Windows, Release 10). In the case of preferred angular
positions of the head, the measure used for overall analysis was the number of
occurrences n of each head position (10°, 15°, 20°,
25°) in the formula
(n15+n25)-(n10+n20)
for each individual, combining data for the RE and LE. If the most probable
distribution of head positions was followed, i.e. the nearer the positions
were to pointing directly at the target (0°) the more commonly they were
used, then this measure would tend to be negative. When examining the
significance of bias within a condition, Wilcoxon signed-ranks tests
(T+) were used (Siegel
and Castellan, 1988). All probabilities are two-tailed, with the
exception of pairwise tests comparing frequency of head angles in two adjacent
bins. Here the only direction of difference of interest was that the bin
further from the binocular position should be the larger.
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|
Comparisons of a lid condition with NOL were possible within a single batch for SL and NOL, and for STR and NOL. In both cases, the lid condition differed significantly from NOL (F1,13=44.382, P<0.001; F1,14=13.892, P=0.002).
The most obvious reason for the right and left deviations from the direct line of approach is that they are a consequence of bringing the LE or RE to bear on the dish. However, this needs confirmation. It would be possible for a chick to run indirectly to the dish (for some unknown reason) and to hold its head in positions that would allow either RE or LE use.
A single mean measure of bias in eye use (R/R+L) was calculated for each point of approach for each chick, based on which eye was directed towards the dish in each of the test trials. There was overall significant variation between conditions (repeated-measures ANOVA: F3,62=12.176, P<0.001), which was due to RE use in the lid conditions and LE use in the NOL condition. All pairwise comparisons between NOL and each of the lid conditions, based on measures at all three points, were significant at P<0.05.
At the DIST point there was significant bias to RE use in all lid conditions (STR, N=7, T+=27, P=0.031; UL, z=2.903, P=0.0038; SL, z=2.495, P=0.0126). In the NOL condition there was significant bias to LE use (N=15, T+=116.5, P=0.0004). The corresponding values for MID were: STR, N=8, T+=33, P=0.039, UL, z=3.081, P=0.002; SL, z=1.825, P=0.068; NOL, N=12, T+=76, P=0.0014; and for CLOSE: STR, N=7, T+=17, NS; UL, z=2.086, P=0.037; SL, z=1.916, P=0.056; NOL, N=14, T+=74, NS.
Head positions
Chicks are known to use precise and standard head positions when viewing
objects at a distance with one or other lateral visual field. Under these
circumstances there is a narrow peak in frequency of head positions that is
centred on 62-63° from the direct line of sight to the object
(Dharmaretnam and Andrew,
1994). This degree of consistency suggests that a standard eye
position is usually assumed when viewing from a distance. It is likely that
the 62-63° peak involves the avian `primary position of gaze'
(Wallman and Pettigrew, 1985
),
in which the eyes are held diverged to a standard degree. A second standard
relative position of the optical axes in birds is binocular convergence just
before pecking, including a further series of small vergences (approximately
3° in pigeon Columba livia) as the head descends towards the
target (McFadden, 1994
).
A final point to note is that the present data often show well-defined
standard head positions relative to the centre of the dish, even when quite
close to it (a few centimetres at most, at the CLOSE point). This suggests
that the retinal area important for viewing is aligned in some standard way
with the object of interest (the dish), perhaps with its centre. A similar
alignment is probably needed to explain the standard head positions described
by Dharmaretnam and Andrew
(1994) for chicks that were
viewing objects such as a live hen or rat. Although the distance between chick
and stimulus object in that study was approximately 55 cm, the objects could
be 15 cm or more in length, when turned broad-side on. This could introduce
variation of as much as 15° if the head were turned to examine different
features, which would certainly have obscured the sharply defined and standard
peaks in frequency of use. It follows that the head and retina are aligned
with some characteristic feature of large objects of interest. This suggests
either that analysis of different local features can be carried out over a
relatively wide area of the retina, or that once the alignment is taken up
small eye movements allow scanning.
Fig. 3 shows the frequency of occurrence of particular head positions for chicks at the CLOSE point near to the dish. From these data the number of occurrences of each head position for both eyes at 10°, 15°, 20° and 25° was entered in the formula x=(n15+n25-(n10+n20) for each individual. If the most probable distribution of head positions occurred, i.e. the nearer the positions were to pointing directly at the target (0°) the more commonly they were used, then this measure would tend to be negative. The calculated values showed a clear preference for 15° and 25° in NOL (median x=2) and SL (median x=1) chicks. There was significant heterogeneity between lid conditions (univariate ANOVA, F3,62=3.256, P=0.027), because this preference occurred only in SL and NOL animals (median x=0 for UL; median x=-1 for STR). In both, the measure was positive and significantly different from random (Wilcoxon signed-ranks test; for SL, N=15, T+=106, P=0.0034; for NOL, N=16, T+=105, P=0.0001). Thus there are peaks in frequency of use at 15° and 25° in SL and NOL that are absent (or less well developed) in STR and UL. Comparison of each peak with the sector adjacent to it that was nearer to 0°, showed significant differences for both the 15° and 25° peaks (for SL, 15°, N=13, T+=80, P=0.007; and 25°, N=15, T+=95, P=0.024; for NOL, 15°, N=8, T+=36, P=0.004; and 25°, N=13, T+=86.5, P=0.001). Further, for SL the frequency at 35° was significantly greater than that at 30° (N=12, T+=66, P=0.034), suggesting periodicity of repetition, with peaks 10° apart.
|
The patterns of head position frequency for NOL and SL also resembled each other at the MID and DIST points (Figs 4, 5). At both points, the use of the dominant eye (left for NOL, right for SL condition) was so predominant that analysis was confined to these data. At the MID point 20° was used more often than 15° (N=18, T+=138.5, P=0.017), and at the DIST point 10° was used more than 5° (N=23, T+=205.5, P=0.04).
|
|
In general, STR chicks show no clear peaks in the frequency of use of head positions. However, at the DIST point (Fig. 5), there was a significant difference between 25° and 20° for the RE (N=6, T+=21, P=0.016).
At the CLOSE point further resemblances in the pattern of frequency of head positions can be seen between NOL and SL. Subordinate and dominant eyes both show a peak at 15°, but the 25° peak is largely confined to the dominant eye. In addition, in both conditions, 0° is by far the most commonly used position, whereas in UL and STR chicks the frequency of use of 0° is more similar to that of 5° (Fig. 3), even though in these latter two conditions there was a single central target at which the bill could be directed. The frequency value for the 0° bin thus was compared with the mean of the right and left 5° bins: 0° was used more frequently than the 5° bins for NOL (z=2.90, P=0.004) and SL (z=3.007, P=0.002).
At the DIST point, the pattern for UL is similar to that of SL, with suggestion of a peak in RE use at 10°. However, at MID and CLOSE, there are no clear peaks in RE use in UL chicks, in contrast to the SL condition. At the CLOSE point UL chicks made similar use of all RE bins from 0° to 15°. There is a significant difference between UL and SL chicks in the relative use of 0° and 5° for the RE (MannWhitney test, U=132.5, P=0.029).
At the MID point for UL there is no elevation of the frequency of use of 0° relative to 5° that is present in SL chicks (MannWhitney test, U=146, P=<0.001). These patterns suggest that UL chicks fail to maintain a standard fixation point relative to the dish as they approach. One explanation for this would be a shift in attention to the large notch, into which the bill will be inserted, at some point during the approach that varies between trials and individuals.
It is worth emphasising that the test arena and position and dimensions of the target dish was the same for all conditions. The observed differences in head position were due to decisions by the chick that in turn derived from the nature of the tasks.
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
The regularity of the peaks in the present data suggest that similar predictable transfer of features is important here too, but based on much smaller amplitude (10°) head saccades, made whilst attention is directed to a particular area or target.
The existence of two series of peaks, displaced relative to each other by
5°, is best explained by supposing that the 10° and 20° peaks
occur when the eyes are converged to the position used during binocular
fixation. Note that 0° is then a part of the 10° and 20° series,
with head turns towards 0° ending in binocular fixation. The series
15°, 25°, 35° is hypothesised to occur when the eye in use is
diverged by 5°. It can be seen most clearly in SL and NOL at the CLOSE
point (Fig. 3), suggesting that
such divergence occurs, in SL and NOL chicks, at or near to the CLOSE point.
In both SL and NOL animals, it is likely that selection of specific targets
occurs as the chick approaches this point. This must be true for NOL, since
the food grains could not be seen until the chick was close to the dish. In
the case of SL, there was no single obvious best place to apply the bill. The
lid could be removed by a blow at a variety of places around the rim.
Selection of a particular target for the bill thus could well have occurred
only when close to the dish. The other feature of interest of the CLOSE point
data from NOL and SL is that (at least for the 15° peak) they involve the
subordinate eye almost as much as the dominant. This is consistent with the
use of both eyes in search, so that the head turns to best use whichever eye
has detected a possible target. Both eyes are used independently when chicks
search for food grains scattered over the floor
(Rogers and Andrew, 1989).
Divergence during such searching may make it easier for each eye system to
work independently, by bringing the eyes out of binocular register.
The UL condition differs from SL and NOL in that attention is likely to shift during approach from the dish as a whole to the notch. The hint of a peak in the frequency of head positions at 10° for the DIST point suggests that at least some chicks began the approach with the alignment to the dish that was observed in SL and NOL chicks to occur up to the CLOSE point. A shift in alignment during the approach in UL birds would obscure any detailed pattern of preferred head position.
The STR task was unique in that the manipulandum was conspicuous from the start, and also required careful grasping, rather than the sideways blow of the bill that occurs when a chick cannot reach food because of an obstacle. Both are likely to have required a motor plan to be set up at the beginning of the approach. The greater use of the 25° right head position at the DIST point by STR chicks may associated with setting up such a motor plan. It has been argued above that the use of this head position in SL and NOL chicks at the CLOSE point was associated with the selection of a specific target for the bill.
The use of extreme head positions such as 25° or 35° for RE viewing may function to prevent the left eye system (LES) from attending to the target, and thus give the right eye system (RES) full control over establishing and sustaining a motor plan.
The key finding of the present study is the importance of the RES in motor
control. This may be widespread in birds. Hunt
(2000) gives indirect evidence
that New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides use the RE when making
a tool from the hooked edge of a pandanus leaf: here a series of skilled
movements are carried out under RE control. In the pigeon
(Zeigler et al., 1994
)
adjustment of gape to the size of the object to be grasped is under visual
control. If this is true also of the chick, then the evidence cited here
implies that the RES is responsible. Further aspects of RES motor control are
likely to be acting to sustain the motor plan against distraction: the use of
the 25° RE head position has already been discussed as a strategy
contributing to this.
The RES also appears to record cues to which attention is directed during
the performance of a response under visual control. When marsh tits Parus
palustris, with one eye covered, recover food items recently hoarded with
both eyes functional, they use local properties of the site itself, when
searching with the RE, but spatial position within the general environment
when using the LE (Clayton and Krebs,
1994). A comparable condition in chicks is suggested by their
tendency to turn towards objects seen in the right hemifield when searching
using local cues, but to the left hemifield when using topographical cues
(Tommasi et al., 2000
). Chicks
also use information from the general environment to determine the geometric
centre of an arena when using the LE, but use local landmarks when using the
RE (Vallortigara, 2000
;
Tommasi and Vallortigara,
2001
).
There is evidence suggesting that the RES records the outcome of the
response to a stimulus, in the chick. After feeding on one type of food, the
attractiveness of that type of food is reduced. Such `devaluation' affects
choice based on dish characteristics that are predictive of food type, but
only during RE, and not LE use (Cozzutti
and Vallortigara, 2001). Note that if the RES holds such
information, this will further improve the ability to control the motor
response correctly with the RE.
The use of the LE in the NOL condition deserves comment. Orientation by
topographical features is better with the LE than with the RE
(Rashid and Andrew, 1989);
however, this can have mattered little in the simple direct approaches to the
dish that were made in the tasks used here. Nevertheless, a default strategy
of LE use during approach to sites where no manipulandum is visible might be
advantageous. Detection of novelty is more likely when use of the LE is forced
(Vallortigara and Andrew,
1991
). Spontaneous use of the LE as a default strategy may thus
make it more likely that unexpected change will be detected
(Andrew et al., 2000
).
There has been no study of possible improvements in manipulation, when the
chick is forced to use the RE. However, chicks with one eye covered show
better performance in the pebble floor task when using the RE
(Rogers and Anson, 1979).
Here, one type of small target (familiar food grains) has to be rapidly
selected from amongst a mixture of two types of target. Pecks at the second
type, inedible and unfamiliar small pebbles, are scored as errors. There is no
special manipulation to be planned. Nevertheless, two features of the task
call for effective motor control. Firstly, this is a task in which the speed
of pecking is such that the ability to inhibit responses to incorrect targets
must be crucial. Secondly, the cues used in selection have to be derived from
immediate prior experience of the outcome of response.
We propose that the assumption of specific head angles in viewing is also functional, and that it serves to regulate the degree of control by one or other eye system and of the interaction between them. Such regulation occurs whether the RE or LE is in use. It thus forms a second and relatively independent mechanism.
Current evidence indicates that the specialisations of the RES and LES in birds may be summarised as follows: RES focuses on the target of response, controls the motor response and records both the response and its outcome. The LES uses diffuse attention in the analysis of the layout of the environment, and uses multiple cues to assess identity or novelty. It is likely that this would be largely incompatible with the control of the response. Note that it is not argued that the RES does not use information relating to spatial patterning at all. The local cues that distinguish hoarding sites for marsh tits probably could only be reliably discriminated one from another if the relative positions of features within the pattern were taken into account.
The current findings underline the importance of studies that, like the present one, allow study of lateralisation under normal conditions. They suggest that differential viewing is used to allow the eye system appropriate to the task in hand to dominate, even with both eyes readily able to see the target.
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Andrew, R. J. (2002). The earliest origins and subsequent evolution of lateralisation. In Comparative Vertebrate Lateralisation (ed. L. J. Rogers and R. J. Andrew), pp.70 -93. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Andrew, R. J. and Dharmaretnam, M. (1993). Lateralisation and strategies of viewing in the domestic chick. In Vision, Brain and Behavior in Birds (ed. H. P. Zeigler and H.-J. Bischof), pp. 319-332. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Andrew, R. J., Tommasi, L. and Ford, N. (2000). Motor control in vision and the evolution of cerebral lateralisation. Brain Lang. 73,220 -235.[Medline]
Clayton, N. S. and Krebs, J. R. (1994). Memory for spatial and object-specific cues in food storing and non-storing birds. J. Comp. Physiol. A 172,371 -379.
Cozzutti, C. and Vallortigara, G. (2001). Hemispheric memories for the content and position of food caches in the domestic chick. Behav. Neurosci. 115,305 -313.[Medline]
Dharmaretnam, M. and Andrew, R. J. (1994). Age- and stimulus-specific use of right and left eye by the domestic chick. Anim. Behav. 48,1395 -1406.
Evans, C. S., Evans, L. and Marler, P. (1993). On the meaning of alarm calls: functional reference in an avian vocal system. Anim. Behav. 46,23 -28.
Hunt, G. R. (2000). Human-like, population-level specialisation in the manufacture of pandanus tools by New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267,403 -413.[Medline]
McFadden, S. A. (1994). Binocular depth perception. In Perception and Motor Control in Birds (ed. M. N. O. Davies and P. R. Green), pp. 54-73. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
McKenzie, R., Andrew, R. J. and Jones, R. B. (1998). Lateralisation in chicks and hens: new evidence for the control of response by the right eye system. Neuropsychologia 36,5 -58.
Miklósi, A. and Andrew, R. J. (1999). Right eye use associated with decision to bite in zebrafish. Behav. Brain Res. 105,199 -205.[Medline]
Miklósi, A., Andrew, R. J. and Dharmaretnam, M. (1996). Auditory lateralisation: shifts in ear use during attachment in the domestic chick. Laterality 1, 215-224.[Medline]
Miklósi, A., Andrew, R. J. and Savage, H. (1998). Behavioural lateralisation of the tetrapod type in the zebrafish (Brachydanio rerio), as revealed by viewing patterns. Physiol. Behav. 63,127 -135.
Rashid, N. and Andrew, R. J. (1989). Right hemisphere advantage for topographical orientation in domestic chick. Neuropsychologia 27,937 -948.[Medline]
Rogers, L. J. (2002). Advantages and disadvantages of lateralisation. In Comparative Vertebrate Lateralisation (ed. L. J. Rogers and R. J. Andrew), pp.126 -154. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Rogers, L. J. and Andrew, R. J. (1989). Frontal and lateral visual field use by chicks after treatment with testosterone. Anim. Behav. 38,394 -405.
Rogers, L. J. and Anson, J. M. (1979). Lateralisation of function in the chicken forebrain. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 9,735 -740.
Siegel, S. and Castellan, N. J. (1988). Nonparametric Statistics. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Tommasi, L., Andrew R. J. and Vallortigara, G. (2000). Eye use is determined by nature of task in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). Behav. Brain. Res. 112,119 -126.[Medline]
Tommasi, L. and Vallortigara, G. (2001). Encoding of geometric and landmark information in the left and right hemispheres of the avian brain. Behav. Neurosci. 115,602 -613.[Medline]
Vallortigara, G. and Andrew, R. J. (1991). Lateralisation of response to change in social partner in chick. Anim. Behav. 41,187 -194.
Vallortigara, G. (2000). Comparative neuropsychology of the dual brain: a stroll through left and right animals' perceptual worlds. Brain Lang. 73,189 -219.[Medline]
Vallortigara, G., Cozzutti, C., Tommasi, L. and Rogers, L. J. (2001). How birds use their eyes: Opposite left-right specialization for the lateral and frontal visual hemifield in the domestic chick. Curr. Biol. 11,29 -33.[Medline]
Wallman, J. and Pettigrew, J. D. (1985). Conjugate and disjunctive saccades in two avian species with contrasting oculomotor strategies. J. Neurosci. 5,1418 -1428.[Abstract]
Zeigler, H. P., Bermejo, R. and Bout, R. (1994). Ingestive behaviour and the sensorimotor control of the jaw. In Perception and Motor Control in Birds (ed. M. N. O. Davies and P. R. Green), pp. 54-73. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Related articles in JEB: