Polarization analysis in the crayfish visual system
Department of Biochemistry and Cell Biology, Rice University, PO Box 1892, Houston, TX 77251, USA
e-mail: rmg{at}rice.edu
Accepted April 19, 2001
![]() |
Summary |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Key words: Polarization sensitivity, e-vector, crustacean, vision, interneuron, contrast sensitivity, oculomotor system.
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Motion detection is a critical feature of decapod visual systems. Behavioral studies indicate motion sensitivity spanning a velocity range of at least four orders of magnitude. Thus optomotor reflexes track a global motion at 0.005°s-1 (Sandeman, 1977) while the defense reflex exhibits coordinated responses at stimulus velocities of up to 50°s-1 (Glantz, 1974). A corresponding wealth of motion-sensitive interneurons have been described in the decapod optic tract (Wiersma and Yamaguchi, 1966; Wiersma and York, 1972; Wiersma et al., 1982) and studies of lamina monopolar neurons suggest that a foundation for motion vision is established in the lamina ganglionaris (first visual neuropile) (Glantz and Bartels, 1994) at the primary visual synapse.
In the last few years, studies in the crayfish lamina (Glantz, 1996a) and medulla externa (second visual neuropile) (Glantz, 1996b; Glantz and McIsaac, 1998) have revealed significant polarization sensitivity in four neuronal classes that form the most peripheral stages of information processing in the visual pathway. None of the cells examined is exclusively polarization-sensitive, and the polarization-relevant signal is confounded with the signals of normal contrast vision throughout the system. A possible explanation of these results is that polarization sensitivity in the early stages of the visual pathway may enhance contrast where intensity differences are absent (Leggett, 1976; Bernard and Wehner, 1977). Furthermore, since the polarization-related response is enhanced by a changing e-vector in at least some of these cells (also found in crabs; Leggett, 1976), it is possible that crayfish use polarization sensitivity as a mechanism of increasing temporal contrast sensitivity (e.g. responsiveness to a local time-varying signal intensity). Because temporal contrast is the foundation of all motion vision, this perspective places crayfish polarization within the context of movement detection.
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|
Fig.1B shows the response of the same cell to a continuously changing e-vector (produced by rotating a polarizer). Again, the maximum hyperpolarization occurs with the polarizer near the vertical orientation. When the polarizer rotates to the horizontal orientation, however, the membrane potential returns to the resting level (dashed line). The polarization response ratio for orthogonal e-vectors is infinite.
In most circumstances, visual interneurons exhibit a nonlinear relationship between stimulus intensity and response magnitude. An important exception occurs in a number of systems, however, when the intensity or contrast is modulated about a constant mean level. Systems as diverse as cat retinal ganglion cells (Enroth-Cugell et al., 1983), catfish horizontal cells (Krausz and Naka, 1980) and Limulus eccentric cells (Knight et al., 1970) exhibit linear behavior under these circumstances. The mean intensity, which is also an adapting light, has the effect of linearizing the response about the mean intensity. The LMC response to a drifting sine wave grating is linear with contrast [(Imax-Imin)/(Imax+Imin), where Imax and Imin are the maximum and minimum intensities respectively] for contrasts of 0 to 0.7 (Glantz and Bartels, 1994). When a rotating polarizer is viewed through an analyzer (e.g. a photoreceptor), the output signal is similar to the response to a modulated intensity at the same mean intensity. In the case of an LMC operating within its linear response range, the mean polarization sensitivity ratio (4.5) is equivalent to an intensity modulation of 0.63 [(4.5-1)/(4.5+1)], which is within the linear range of the LMC. In keeping with the notion that the LMC response to a rotating polarizer resembles the contrast sensitivity to a time-varying intensity, it was observed that the potential oscillation elicited by a rotating polarizer was independent of the mean intensity from 10x threshold to 300x threshold.
A distinctive feature of the LMC (and photoreceptor) polarization response functions is that they are approximately described by cos2 (max-
), where
is the e-vector angle and
max is the optimum e-vector angle. The cos2
function describes the transmittance of a perfect dichroic analyzer to a continuously rotating e-vector. It varies from 0 to 1.0, and has a period of 180°. The modulation of membrane potential in Fig.1B is approximately described by cos2
. The principle deviation is the saturation of the LMC signal as it approaches the resting potential. The adherence to cos2
implies that the responses are linearly related to the quantum catch of the photopigment.
The steady-state responsiveness to e-vector variations was assessed with a rotating polarizer that was stopped (for up to 5.0s) at different e-vector angles. Fig.1C shows a typical result. The shutter was opened with the polarizer at the horizontal orientation and the cell was permitted to reach a steady-state potential. After several stimulus cycles, polarizer rotation was stopped at a different e-vector angle. During the rotation, it is clear that the horizontal e-vector is associated with a more depolarized membrane potential than that obtained at the same e-vector during the initial exposure. While the steady-state responses at the start and end of the stimulus presentation differed by only 0.4mV, the difference in response to the same e-vectors during rotation was 1.80±0.17mV. Similar results were obtained in 4 of 5 cells so tested.
Tangential cells
The polarization responses of higher-order neurons differ in two respects from those of LMCs. Some of the cells reveal evidence of e-vector opponency, and most of the polarization response profiles differ markedly from cos2 functions.
Fig.2 shows examples of polarization responses from a tangential cell (Tan1). Tan1 neurons have dendrites in the medulla externa (second optic neuropile) and project their axons back to the lamina. Tan1 has a graded visual response; hyperpolarized by an increment of illumination, and may exhibit a depolarizing OFF response accompanied by membrane potential oscillations. In Fig.2A, a stationary flash at a horizontal e-vector elicits a large (-9mV) hyperpolarizing ON response and small (+1.5mV) OFF response. Conversely, a flash of vertically polarized light elicits a small (-2.5mV) ON response and a much larger (+7mV) OFF response. The depolarizing OFF response is transient. It is only observed with illumination decrements or, as shown below, as the e-vector rotates towards the vertical. The data suggest that the ON and OFF response mechanisms are driven by orthogonal e-vector orientations. This follows from the fact that flashes of horizontally polarized light elicit maximal ON responses and minimal OFF responses, and vice versa for the flashes of vertically polarized light. The opponency implied by responses such as those shown in Fig.2A is expressed over at least a 1000-fold range of stimulus intensities (Glantz, 1996b). When the same cell is subjected to a continuously changing e-vector, as in Fig.2B, the membrane potential appears to jump back and forth between two discrete potentials, hyperpolarized for a fraction of the cycle near =90° and depolarized for a comparable fraction near
=0°. The transitions are very rapid and quite distinct from the continuously graded cos2
functions shown by LMCs and receptors. The rapid transitions suggest that, over a limited range of e-vectors, the cell exhibits a relatively high e-vector resolution (
V/
, where
V is the change in membrane potential) when compared with a cos2
function.
|
Sustaining fibers
The sustaining fibers are the principal output neurons of the medulla externa. Each has an excitatory receptive field and an inhibitory surround. Their receptive field dimensions vary from approximately 15° (at half-maximum sensitivity) to 90° and there is extensive overlap of their excitatory regions. The 14 sustaining fibers initially distinguished by their receptive field locations (Wiersma and Yamaguchi, 1966) also have distinct dendritic arborizations in the medulla externa. The dendrites intersect the columnar projection of transmedullary neurons in areas that correspond to the receptive field in neuronal space (Kirk et al., 1982). Sustaining fibers exhibit a depolarization and an impulse discharge in response to increments of illumination. Although the name implies a response to maintained illumination, the sustained response (approx. 510impulsess-1) is actually modest compared with the transient response (200300impulsess-1). The transient ON response is very sensitive and can easily mask the stationary polarization sensitivity profile. The earliest attempts to measure this profile in sustaining fibers were unsuccessful (Waterman, 1984). The polarization sensitivity to flashes at stationary e-vector angles can be demonstrated however with near-threshold stimuli or with stimuli that elicit an excitatory postsynaptic potential but are subthreshold for the impulse discharge (Glantz and McIsaac, 1998). Alternatively, polarization sensitivity in the steady-state response can be observed in response to stepwise changes in e-vector angle while holding the illumination constant, as in Fig.3A.
|
|
Optomotor neurons
Because the sustaining fibers provide synaptic input to the crayfish oculomotor system (Glantz et al., 1984; Glantz and Nudelman, 1988; Okada and Yamaguchi, 1988; Okada et al., 1994) it is possible that optomotor responses may exhibit polarization sensitivity. Crayfish exhibit compensatory optomotor reflexes that stabilize the visual image during animal movements (Schöne, 1961; Neil, 1982). Body rotation elicits a rotation of the eyestalk in the opposite direction. Thus, if the head is pitched downward, the eyestalk rotates upwards (about its long axis). The reflexes that compensate for perturbations in the vertical planes (pitch and roll) are driven by a combination of afferents from the visual system, the statocysts (equilibrium organs) and the proprioceptors of the walking legs. The motoneurons that participate in these reflexes were initially identified on functional grounds by Wiersma and Oberjat (Wiersma and Oberjat, 1968), who named the cells after the optimal excitatory stimulus (e.g. head-down motoneuron). Subsequently, the cells were localized anatomically, and their structures were described by Mellon (Mellon, 1977). Wiersma and Oberjat (Wiersma and Oberjat, 1968) observed that steps and flashes of illumination in specific areas of visual space elicited motoneuron responses that generally resembled those of sustaining fibers, as shown in Fig.5A. These motoneuron responses are probably related to the steady-state eyestalk displacements associated with changes in the apparent direction of skylight, i.e. the dorsal light reflex (Schöne, 1961).
|
Fig.6 shows the response of an extracellularly recorded head-down motoneuron to polarizer rotation at 10°s-1. For the motoneuron studies, the optical axis of the visual stimulus was perpendicular to the dorsal surface of the eye so as to simulate light propagating downwards from the sky. Although the discharge pattern is not as tightly organized as that of sustaining fibers, it is clear that the maximum impulse rate occurs as the polarizer approaches the vertical (here aligned with the long axis of the eyestalk). When probed with flashes of 1.0s duration, the motoneurons typically exhibit a transient burst of activity at light onset and a low-frequency discharge thereafter, as shown in Fig.5A. Fig.7A shows post-stimulus time histograms (each based upon 40 responses) of responses to 1.0s flashes of polarized light at 12 e-vector orientations. The impulse frequency of the peak transient response is plotted as a function of e-vector angle in Fig.7B. The relative sensitivity of these responses is measured by determining the intensities associated with the same impulse frequencies elicited by unpolarized light (as shown in Fig.7C). Thus, the response at an e-vector angle of -30° requires approximately five times as much light (in Fig.7C) as that at +60°, which indicates a polarization sensitivity of 5.0. Similar measurements in 15 head-down motoneurons yielded an average polarization sensitivity ratio of 5.2±2.9 (mean ± S.D.) and with max between -30° and +30° for all cells. These results are consistent with previous studies of eyestalk movements elicited by polarized light in ghost crabs (Schöne and Schöne, 1961).
|
|
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
A second argument has implications both for and against the physiological relevance of polarization vision in crayfish. The magnitude of polarization sensitivity (approximately 4.5) observed in the retinular cells and in all the neurons with the exception of a subset of Tan1 cells, implies that with a high degree of polarization and in the presence of orthogonal e-vectors in the spatial or temporal visual scene, the system can generate polarization-related signals comparable with those associated with normal contrast vision. If the light is only partially polarized, however, or if the differences in e-vector angles among stimuli are small, it is uncertain whether the polarization sensitivity of most of the crayfish neurons will provide a substantial enhancement to vision. Thus, Labhart (Labhart, 1996) has shown that, in cricket polarization-opponent interneurons, the e-vector-dependence of the response declines substantially as the degree of polarization is reduced. This context raises two important questions. How significant is the polarization-related signal (relative to contrast vision) in natural conditions? How much of a signal enhancement is necessary to provide a selective advantage for the type of polarization detection system the crayfish appears to have? My hunch is that even a modest gain in visual performance will suffice. It should also be noted that the present description of the crayfish polarization sensitivity system deals with a small number of the most peripheral visual interneurons. The same columnar projection that synapses on the sustaining fibers, extends to the medulla interna where it innervates higher order visual interneurons. The sustaining fibers and dimming fibers project to both the brain and the medulla terminalis, which are major integrative centers in the nervous system. In the brain the sustaining fibers have additional targets including neurons, which descend from the brain to lower motor centers (Wood and Glantz, 1980), and are most likely involved in visually guided behaviors other than optomotor reflexes. The postsynaptic targets of the Tan1 neurons, which exhibit strong polarization opponency, are unknown. In previous studies (Wang-Bennett and Glantz, 1987) we found that hyperpolarization of Tan1 with extrinsic current indirectly excites sustaining fibers. The high polarization sensitivity of these neurons could better support polarization detection in partially polarized light and for small e-vector angle differences.
To consider how the crayfish might use polarization sensitivity I will assume that the degree of polarization is sufficient to elicit a polarization-related response in the relevant neurons. It is helpful to consider two environments, one in which the illumination is partially polarized and a second in which intensity contrasts might be minimal. The first circumstance is that the crayfish rotates in the horizontal plane in a field of downwelling polarized light. Here, we assume that the light principally strikes the dorsal part of the cornea where the vertical e-vector channels are aligned with the longitudinal axis of the eyestalk and the horizontal channels are aligned with the transverse axis of the eyestalk. As the animal rotates, its self-motion will induce a time-varying e-vector signal that will transiently excite sustaining fibers or dimming fibers, depending upon the alignment of the eyestalk and the stationary e-vector distribution of the illumination. In this scenario, the timing of excitation in the sustaining fibers or dimming fibers could provide the animal with a measure of the prevailing e-vector orientation of skylight. Alternatively, activity in the sustaining fibers and dimming fibers may provide a visual signal indicative of a change in body orientation relative to the incoming light path. This change would activate a compensatory optomotor response. Because the sustaining fibers directly innervate optomotor neurons (Glantz and Nudelman, 1988), they probably contribute to the polarization sensitivity of compensatory oculomotor reflexes. Previous studies in crabs support a modest polarization sensitivity in these systems (Schöne and Schöne, 1961).
In the second scenario, consider a crayfish in a somewhat murky aquatic environment dominated by scattered light that is partially polarized. The scattered light in water is principally horizontally polarized (Waterman, 1981), which implies that a system that extracts the vertical e-vector (e.g. crayfish sustaining fiber) should have superior underwater vision. Furthermore, objects that may be transparent on the basis of a pure intensity profile may still depolarize the transmitted light (Cronin et al., 1995). If such an object (e.g. an animal) were to move in this environment (and assuming that the object does not reflect the same e-vector distribution as the background), then two patches of the crayfish visual field will transiently experience new e-vector distributions. The patch initially exposed to the object will now see the background distribution of e-vectors, while the newly occupied visual field patch will be shaded from the background illumination. In both visual field patches, there is a temporal contrast of e-vector signals that would maximally activate the crayfish visual system. Because each retinal patch is subserved by sustaining fibers and dimming fibers acting in parallel, the system only requires that the e-vector orientations change over time. The capacity to detect the change is independent of absolute e-vector angle. Thus, if the net effect of object motion is to increase the preponderance of horizontally oriented e-vectors, it will activate the appropriate dimming fibers and silence the corresponding sustaining fibers. A shift toward a preponderance of vertical e-vectors will have the reverse effect. In either case, the location of a moving object is detected in the absence of an intensity contrast. The response is analogous to movement perception in a visual environment defined by spectral differences in the absence of intensity contrast (Bernard and Wehner, 1977).
![]() |
Conclusion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
![]() |
Acknowledgments |
---|
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Bernard, G. D. and Wehner, R. (1977). Functional similarities between polarization vision and color vision. Vision Res. 17, 10191028.[Medline]
Cronin, T. W., Shashar, N. and Wolff, L. (1995). Imaging technology reveals the polarized light fields that exist in nature. Biophotonics 2, 3841.
Enroth-Cuggell, C., Robson, J. G., Schweitzer-Tong, D. E. and Watson, A. B. (1983). Spatio-temporal interactions in cat retinal ganglion cells showing linear spatial summation. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 341, 279307.[Abstract]
Glantz, R. M. (1974). Defense reflex and motion detector responsiveness to approaching targets: The motion detector trigger to the defense reflex pathway. J. Comp. Physiol. A 95, 297314.
Glantz, R. M. and Bartels, A. (1994). The spatio-temporal transfer function of crayfish lamina monopolar neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 71, 21682182.
Glantz, R. M. (1996a). Polarization sensitivity in crayfish lamina monopolar neurons. J. Comp. Physiol. A 178, 413425.
Glantz, R. M. (1996b). Polarization sensitivity in the crayfish optic lobe, Peripheral contributions to opponency and directionally selective motion detection. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 34043414.
Glantz, R. M. and McIsaac, A. (1998). Two-channel polarization analyzer in the sustaining fiberdimming fiber ensemble of crayfish visual system. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 25712583.
Glantz, R. M. and Nudelman, H. B. (1988). Interval coding and band-pass filtering at oculomotor synapses in crayfish. J. Neurophysiol. 59, 5676.
Glantz, R. M., Nudelman, H. B. and Waldrop, B. (1984). Linear integration of convergent visual inputs in an oculomotor reflex pathway. J. Neurophysiol. 52, 12131225.
Herrnkind, W. F. (1972). Orientation in shore-living arthropods especially the sand fiddler crab. In Behavior of Marine Animals (ed. H. E. Winn and S. L. Olla), pp. 159. New York: Plenum.
Kirk, M. D., Waldrop, B. and Glantz, R. M. (1982). The crayfish sustaining fibers. I. Morphological representation of visual receptive fields in the second optic neuropile. J. Comp. Physiol. A 146, 175179.
Knight, B. W., Toyoda, J.-I. and Dodge, F. A. (1970). A quantitative description of the dynamics of excitation and inhibition in the eye of Limulus. J. Gen Physiol. 56, 421437.
Krausz, H. I. and Naka, K.-I. (1980). Spatiotemporal testing and modeling of catfish retinal neurons. Biophys. J. 29, 1336.[Abstract]
Labhart, T. (1988). Polarization-opponent interneurons in the insect visual system. Nature 331,435437.
Labhart, T. (1996). How polarization-sensitive interneurones of crickets perform at low degrees of polarization. J. Exp. Biol. 199, 14671475.
Leggett, L. M. W. (1976). Polarized light-sensitive interneurons in a swimming crab. Nature 262, 709711.[Medline]
Mellon, DeF. (1977). The anatomy and motor nerve distribution of the eye muscles in the crayfish. J. Comp. Physiol. A 121, 349366.
Nässel, D. and Waterman, T. (1977). Golgi EM evidence for visual information channeling in crayfish lamina ganglionaris. Brain Res. 130, 556563.[Medline]
Neil, D. M. (1982). Compensatory eye movements. In The Biology of Crustacea, Neural Integration and Behavior, Vol. 4. (ed. D. C. Sandeman and H. L. Atwood), pp. 133163. New York: Academic Press.
Okada, Y. and Yamaguchi, T. (1988). Nonspiking giant interneurons in the crayfish brain: Morphological and physiological characteristics of the neurons postsynaptic to visual interneurons. J. Comp. Physiol. A 162, 705714.
Okada, Y., Furudate, H. and Yamaguchi, T. (1994). Multimodal responses of the nonspiking giant interneurons of the brain of the crayfish Procambarus clarkii. J. Comp. Physiol. A 174, 411419.[Medline]
Pfeiffer, C. and Glantz, R. M. (1989). Cholinergic synapses and the organization of contrast detection in crayfish optic lobe. J. Neurosci. 9, 18721882.[Abstract]
Pfeiffer-Linn, C. and Glantz, R. M. (1991). An arthropod NMDA receptor. Synapse 9, 3542.[Medline]
Sandeman, D. C. (1977). Compensatory eye movements in crabs. In Identified Neurons and Behavior in Arthropods (ed. G. Hoyle), pp. 131147. New York: Plenum.
Schöne, H. and Schöne, H. (1961). Eyestalk movement induced by polarized light in the ghost crab, Ocypode quadrata. Science 134, 675676.[Medline]
Schöne, H. (1961). Complex behavior. In The Physiology of Crustacea, vol. 2. (ed. T. H. Waterman), pp. 465520. New York: Academic Press.
Wang-Bennett, L. and Glantz, R. M. (1987). The functional organization of the crayfish Lamina ganglionaris. II. Large field spiking and nonspiking cells. J. Comp. Physiol. A 161, 147160.[Medline]
Waterman, T. (1981). Polarization sensitivity. In Handbook of Sensory Physiology, vol. 7/6B (ed. H. Autrum), pp. 281469, Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Waterman, T. (1984). Natural polarized light and vision. In Photoreception and Vision in Invertebrates (ed. M. A. Ali), pp. 63114. New York: Plenum.
Wehner, R. (1989). Neurobiology of polarization vision. Trends Neurosci. 12, 353359.[Medline]
Wehner, R. (2001). Polarisation vision a uniform sensory capacity? J. Exp. Biol. 204, 25892596.
Wiersma, C. A. G. and Yamaguchi, T. (1966). The neural components of the optic nerve as studied by single unit analysis. J. Comp. Neurol. 128, 333358.[Medline]
Wiersma, C. A. G. and Oberjat, T. (1968). The selective responsiveness of various crayfish oculomotor fibers to sensory stimuli. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 26A, 116.[Medline]
Wiersma, C. A. G. and York, B. (1972). Properties of the seeing fibers in the rock lobster: Field structure, habituation, attention and distraction. Vision Res. 12, 627640.[Medline]
Wiersma, C. A. G., Roach, J. and Glantz, R. M. (1982). Neural integration in the optic system. In The Biology of Crustacea, vol. 4 (ed. D. Bliss, D. C. Sandeman and H. L. Atwood), pp. 131. New York: Academic Press.
Wood, H. and Glantz, R. M. (1980). Distributed processing by visual interneurons of the crayfish brain. Response characteristics and synaptic interactions. J. Neurophysiol. 43, 741753.