Olfactory sensitivity for aliphatic alcohols in squirrel monkeys and pigtail macaques
Department of Medical Psychology, University of Munich Medical School, Goethestraße 31, D-80336 Munich, Germany
* e-mail: Laska{at}imp.med.uni-muenchen.de
Accepted 12 March 2002
![]() |
Summary |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Key words: olfactory sensitivity, detection threshold, non-human primate, aliphatic alcohol, squirrel monkey, Saimiri sciureus, pigtail macaque, Macaca nemestrina
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
In recent years, an increasing number of behavioural observations call into
question the still widely held belief that olfaction is of only little, if
any, behavioural relevance to primates and concomitantly that members of this
order of mammals have generally only poor olfactory capabilities. There is now
evidence from a number of primate species for olfactory involvement in the
identification and selection of food (Bolen
and Green, 1997; Ueno,
1994
) and in social behaviours such as the establishment and
maintenance of rank (Kappeler,
1998
), territorial defence
(Mertl-Millhollen, 1986
),
identification of sexual partners
(Heymann, 1998
), recognition
of group members (Epple et al.,
1993
) and communication of reproductive status
(Smith and Abbott, 1998
).
Despite such observations, experimental investigations of olfactory
performance in non-human primates have been sparse.
Laska and Hudson (1993a)
introduced a new testing paradigm which, for the first time, allowed the
olfactory performance of a non-human primate species to be assessed using
psychophysical methods. Subsequent studies demonstrated that squirrel monkeys
possess highly developed olfactory discrimination abilities for structurally
related monomolecular substances (Laska
and Freyer, 1997
; Laska and
Teubner, 1998
; Laska et al.,
1999a
,b
),
for artificial odour mixtures (Laska and
Hudson, 1993b
) and for conspecific urine odours
(Laska and Hudson, 1995
).
Further, these studies showed that Saimiri sciureus has an excellent
long-term memory for odours (Laska et al.,
1996
), a well-developed olfactory sensitivity for aliphatic
carboxylic acids (Laska et al.,
2000
) and acetic esters (Laska
and Seibt, 2002
) and is capable of rapid odour learning
(Laska and Hudson, 1993a
).
Hübener and Laska
(1998,
2001
) adapted this method to
the species-specific needs of another primate species, the pigtail macaque,
and demonstrated that squirrel monkeys are not the only primate species with
surprisingly well-developed olfactory capabilities. Further, their behavioural
paradigm allows us to compare olfactory performance reliably between two
primate species.
The aims of the present study are twofold: (i) to gain further insight into the basic perceptual capacities of non-human primates by determining olfactory detection thresholds in squirrel monkeys and pigtail macaques for an array of monomolecular odorants; and (ii) to assess whether neuroanatomical features are reliable predictors of olfactory performance by comparing the detection thresholds of the two primate species tested here with those of other mammals.
We have chosen aliphatic alcohols as odour stimuli because this class of substance is presumed to indicate a fruit's degree of ripeness and is thus likely to be behaviourally relevant for frugivorous primates and because comparative data from humans and, at least for the majority of odorants, from other mammalian species are available. Further, the use of a homologous series of alcohols and some isomeric forms allowed us also to address the question of whether structural features of stimulus molecules such as carbon chain length or the position of a functional group affect detectability in a predictable manner.
![]() |
Materials and methods |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
The experiments reported here comply with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (National Institutes of Health Publication no. 86-23, revised 1985) and also with current German laws.
Behavioural tests
The squirrel monkeys were tested using a multiple-choice instrumental
conditioning paradigm (Hudson et al.,
1992). Opaque 1.5 ml Eppendorf flip-top reagent cups were fitted
with absorbent paper strips (35 mmx7 mm; Sugi, Kettenbach, Germany)
impregnated with 10 µl of an odorant signalling either that they contained
a peanut food reward (S+) or that they did not (S-). The odour strips were
attached to the vials by cutting a slit in each strip and slipping it over the
flip-up lid, which was connected to the vial by a narrow band. Eighteen such
cups, nine positive and nine negative, were inserted in pseudorandom order in
holes along the horizontal bars of a climbing frame in such a way that some
effort was required for the animals to remove them. The frame was mounted on
one of the enclosure walls at a distance of 10 cm and consisted of a 2.5 m
vertical pole (40 mm diameter) fitted with seven cross-bars (20 mm diameter)
30 cm apart, the middle three of which extended 50 cm to either side and were
equipped with conically bored holes to hold the cups.
In each test trial, each monkey was allowed 1 min to harvest as many baited cups from the frame as possible. Five such trials were conducted per animal per session, and usually two sessions were conducted per day. Cups were used only once, and the odourized strips were prepared fresh at the start of each session.
The pigtail macaques were tested using a two-choice instrumental
conditioning paradigm (Hübener and
Laska, 2001). Two cube-shaped open polyvinyl chloride containers
(side length 5.5 cm) were attached to a metal bar (50 cm long and 6 cm wide)
at a distance of 22 cm. Each container was equipped with a hinged metallic lid
that could be opened only by drawing a metallic pin from a hole extending
horizontally through the overlapping lid and the front side of a container. A
clip on top of each lid held an absorbent paper strip (70 mmx10 mm)
impregnated with 10µl of an odorant signalling either that the container
held a Kellogg's Honey Loop food reward (S+) or that it did not (S-). The
odourized paper strips extended 5 cm into the test cage when the apparatus was
attached to the front of the cage.
In each test trial, each monkey sniffed at both options for as often as it liked and then decided to open one of the two boxes. After each decision, the apparatus was removed from the mesh and (out of sight of the test animal) was prepared for the next trial by baiting the container bearing the S+ again and adopting a pseudorandomized sequence of presentations of the S+ on the left or on the right side. Ten such trials were conducted per animal and session, and three sessions were usually conducted per day.
It is important to note that the mode of stimulus presentation (10 µl of odorant on an absorbent paper strip) was identical with squirrel monkeys and pigtail macaques.
For both species, olfactory detection thresholds were determined by testing the animals' ability to discriminate between manipulation objects scented with increasing dilutions of an odorant used as S+ and those scented with the odourless solvent alone used as S-. Starting with a dilution of 1:100, each odorant was successively presented in 10-fold dilution steps, for two sessions with the squirrel monkeys and for three sessions with the pigtail macaques, until an animal failed to discriminate significantly the odorant from the solvent. Subsequently, this descending staircase procedure was repeated for two (for squirrel monkeys) or three (for pigtail macaques) more sessions per dilution step. Finally, intermediate dilutions were tested to determine the threshold value more exactly. If, for example, an animal significantly discriminated a 1:10 000 dilution from the solvent, but failed to do so with a 1:100 000 dilution, then the animal was presented with a 1:30 000 dilution. To prevent the more challenging conditions leading to extinction or to a decline in the animals' motivation, these were always followed by a return to, or in the case of the intermediate dilutions, interspersed with, an easy control task. This consisted of the discrimination between a 100-fold dilution of the S+ and the odourless solvent as S.
Odorants
A set of 11 odorants was used: ethanol, 1-propanol, 1-butanol, 1-pentanol,
1-hexanol, 1-heptanol, 1-octanol, 2-propanol, 2-butanol, 2-pentanol and
3-pentanol. The rationale for choosing these substances was to assess the
monkeys' sensitivity for odorants representing members of a homologous series
of aliphatic compounds, i.e. substances sharing the same functional group but
differing in carbon chain length, and for isomeric forms of some of these
compounds, i.e. substances sharing the same carbon chain length and type of
functional group but differing in the position of their oxygen moiety,
allowing us to assess the impact of both structural features on detectability.
All substances were obtained from Merck (Darmstadt) and had a nominal purity
of at least 99%. They were diluted using odourless diethyl phthalate (Merck)
as the solvent.
Data analysis
For each squirrel monkey, the percentage of correct choices from the best
two sessions per dilution step, i.e. from 10 1-min trials comprising a total
of at least 60 decisions, was calculated. Similarly, for each pigtail macaque,
the percentage of correct choices from the best three sessions per dilution
step, comprising a total of 30 decisions, was calculated.
Correct choices consisted both of animals correctly rejecting negative manipulation objects by failing to open them and identifying positive manipulation objects by opening them to obtain the food reward. Conversely, errors consisted of animals opening negative manipulation objects or failing to open positive manipulation objects.
Significance levels were determined by calculating binomial
z-scores corrected for continuity
(Siegel and Castellan, 1988)
from the number of correct and false responses for each individual and
condition. All tests were two-tailed, and the alpha level was set at 0.05.
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|
Fig. 2 shows the performance of the squirrel monkeys in discriminating between various dilutions of a given aliphatic alcohol with a non-terminal functional group and the odourless solvent. All three animals significantly distinguished dilutions as low as 1:300 2-propanol, 1:10 000 2-butanol, 1:30 000 2-pentanol and 1:30 000 3-pentanol from the solvent (binomial test, P<0.05), with some individuals scoring even better.
|
The individual squirrel monkeys demonstrated very similar threshold values and usually differed only by a dilution factor of three or ten between the highest- and the lowest-scoring animal. In the case of 2-pentanol and 3-pentanol, they even showed identical threshold values. The largest difference in sensitivity for a given odorant between individuals comprised a dilution factor of 33 and was found with 1-heptanol.
A significant negative correlation between perceptibility in terms of olfactory detection thresholds and carbon chain length of the 1-alcohols was found (Spearman, rs=-0.81, P<0.01; Fig. 3). This correlation was even highly significant when the threshold values for the two substances with the longest carbon chain tested, i.e. 1-heptanol and 1-octanol, were removed from the calculations (Spearman, rs=-0.95, P<0.001). A corresponding significant correlation was also found with the three 2-alcohols tested (Spearman, rs=-0.97, P<0.01; Fig. 3).
|
Table 1 summarizes the
threshold dilutions for both the best- and the poorest-performing squirrel
monkeys and shows various measures of corresponding vapour phase
concentrations (Weast, 1987).
In the majority of cases, threshold dilutions correspond to vapour phase
concentrations below 1 part per million, and with 1-hexanol the best-scoring
animal was even able to detect a concentration of 2 parts per billion.
|
Pigtail macaques
Fig. 4 shows the performance
of the pigtail macaques in discriminating between various dilutions of a given
aliphatic alcohol with a terminal functional group and the odourless solvent.
All four animals significantly distinguished dilutions as low as 1:300
ethanol, 1:1000 1-propanol, 1:3000 1-butanol, 1:1000 1-pentanol, 1:30 000
1-hexanol, 1:30 000 1-heptanol and 1:30 000 1-octanol from the solvent
(binomial test, P<0.05), with some individuals scoring even
better.
|
Fig. 5 shows the performance of the pigtail macaques in discriminating between various dilutions of a given aliphatic alcohol with a non-terminal functional group and the odourless solvent. All four animals significantly distinguished dilutions as low as 1:1000 2-propanol, 1:1000 2-butanol, 1:3000 2-pentanol and 1:3000 3-pentanol from the solvent (binomial test, P<0.05), with some individuals scoring even better.
|
The individual pigtail macaques demonstrated very similar threshold values and usually differed only by a dilution factor of three or ten between the highest- and the lowest-scoring animal. In the case of 2-propanol, they even showed identical threshold values. The largest difference in sensitivity for a given odorant between individuals comprised a dilution factor of 100 and was found with 1-pentanol.
Similar to the findings with the squirrel monkeys, a significant negative correlation between perceptibility in terms of olfactory detection thresholds and carbon chain length of the 1-alcohols was found (Spearman, rs=-0.90, P<0.01; Fig. 6). Also in line with the squirrel monkeys, a corresponding significant correlation was found for the three 2-alcohols tested (Spearman, rs=-0.97, P=0.01; Fig. 6).
|
Table 2 summarizes the
threshold dilutions for both the best- and the poorest-performing pigtail
macaques and shows various measures of corresponding vapour phase
concentrations (Weast, 1987).
In the majority of cases, threshold dilutions correspond to vapour phase
concentrations below 1 part per million, and with 1-heptanol the best-scoring
animal was even able to detect a concentration of 4 parts per billion.
|
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Although only three or four animals were tested per species, the results
appear robust because interindividual variability was remarkably low and
generally smaller than the range reported in studies on human olfactory
sensitivity, i.e. within three orders of magnitude
(Stevens et al., 1988). In
fact, for the majority of substances tested, there was only a factor of three
or ten between the threshold values of the highest- and the lowest-scoring
animal of a species. Further, for all substances tested, the animals'
performance at the lowest concentration presented dropped to chance level,
suggesting that the statistically significant discrimination between higher
concentrations of an odorant and the pure diluent was indeed based on odour
perception and not on other cues.
Fig. 7 compares the
olfactory detection threshold values obtained with squirrel monkeys and
pigtail macaques for the substances tested with those from other mammalian
species. Although such across-species comparisons should be considered with
caution because different methods may lead to widely differing results
as can be seen with the threshold values depicted for 1-hexanol in the rat
it seems admissible to state that Saimiri sciureus and
Macaca nemestrina are far from being `microsmats', i.e. species with
a poorly developed sense of smell. With the majority of the aliphatic
1-alcohols tested, for example, both species demonstrated olfactory threshold
values lower than those of the rat, which is traditionally regarded as a
`macrosmatic' animal, i.e. a species with a highly developed sense of smell.
Interestingly, human subjects showed an olfactory sensitivity for the alcohols
employed here quite similar to that of the two non-human primate species and
thus better than the traditional view suggests (see
Fig. 7). It should be mentioned
that the threshold values of the human subjects for the 1-alcohols as depicted
in Fig. 7 are taken from the
study by Commetto-Muniz and Cain
(1990). Although some other
studies reported slightly lower values for some of the substances (e.g. for
1-propanol, Corbit and Engen,
1971
; for 1-butanol, Laing,
1982
; for 1-hexanol, Hellman
and Small, 1974
), all these other studies had tested only one or a
few members of the homologous series of alcohols and none of them had used
signal detection methods and a comparably sophisticated mode of stimulus
presentation to that of Cometto-Muniz and Cain
(1990
).
|
Across-species comparisons of olfactory performance raise the question as to possible reasons for the observed similarities and, sometimes marked, differences in olfactory sensitivity for a given substance. Similarly, within-species comparisons of olfactory performance should be discussed with regard to possible explanations for differences in sensitivity among substances.
It seems appropriate to assume that the efficiency of a sensory system
reflects an evolutionary adaptation of a species to its ecological niche.
Although this idea is widely recognised and well supported by numerous
examples in the visual and auditory modalities
(Dusenbery, 1992),
surprisingly few authors have considered olfactory performance from this point
of view. Rather, there is a long-standing tradition of assigning species with
general labels such as `microsmat' or `macrosmat'. This classification,
however, is usually based on neuroanatomical features that are interpreted as
indicating either a pivotal or a negligible role of the sense of smell in a
given species and only rarely on experimental assessments of olfactory
performance. Our finding of a well-developed olfactory sensitivity for
aliphatic alcohols in squirrel monkeys and pigtail macaques is yet another
example showing that allometric comparisons of olfactory brain structure
volumes or of the absolute size of olfactory epithelia are poor predictors of
chemosensory performance. There is no doubt that the relative size of the
rat's brain structures devoted to processing olfactory information and the
absolute size of the rat's olfactory epithelium are both considerably larger
than those of the squirrel monkey or of the pigtail macaque
(Stephan et al., 1988
). Our
data, however, clearly show that such comparisons of neuroanatomical
structures do not allow us to draw generalizable conclusions about the
olfactory sensitivity of any two species.
Considering that, even for the most intensively studied species of
non-human mammals, measurements of olfactory sensitivity or discrimination
abilities have usually been restricted to little more than a handful of
substances (Walker and Jennings,
1991), it is obvious that the assignment of general labels such as
`microsmat' or `macrosmat' to any species is at least premature and does not
take into account the vast complexity of our natural odour world and the
diversity of contexts in which the sense of smell may be crucial for an
animal. Therefore, we argue that these terms should no longer be used.
To explain similarities or differences in olfactory performance among or within species, it might be more appropriate to consider whether given odorants or classes of odorant differ in their degree of behavioural relevance for a species.
Squirrel monkeys and pigtail macaques have been reported to include a
considerable proportion of fruit into their diets
(Clutton-Brock and Harvey,
1977; Ross, 1992
).
Our finding that both species are generally at least as sensitive as rats to
aliphatic alcohols and clearly outperform common bats (see
Fig. 7) appears to make sense
in terms of an evolutionary adaptation to optimal foraging because these
substances are known to be products of microbial fermentation processes in
fruits and are thus indicative of their degree of ripeness. Therefore, it
seems plausible to assume that aliphatic alcohols may be more relevant for
species feeding on fruit than for a granivorous species such as the rat or an
insectivorous species such as the common bat. In line with this idea,
short-tailed fruit bats have also been shown to be more sensitive than rats to
aliphatic alcohols (Laska,
1990
; see Fig.
7).
Carnivorous, insectivorous or sanguivorous species such as the dog, the
hedgehog and the vampire bat, respectively, have been found to be more
sensitive than the squirrel monkey or the pigtail macaque to short-chained
carboxylic acids (Hübener and Laska,
2001; Laska et al.,
2000
). This class of odorants makes up the main component of
body-borne prey odour (Flood,
1985
) and is thus believed to be highly relevant for species
feeding on animal prey, but presumably less important for mainly frugivorous
primates.
A comparison of the olfactory performance of squirrel monkeys and pigtail
macaques in detecting aliphatic esters (Laska and Seibt, 2001) and aliphatic
alcohols reveals that both species are considerably more sensitive to the
former group of substances than to the latter group. This finding concurs with
the idea that not only the behavioural relevance but also the frequency of
occurrence of a substance or substance class in a species' chemical world may
determine its chemosensory capabilities because aliphatic alcohols, probably
because of their high degree of chemical reactivity, are found in a much lower
number of naturally occurring complex odours and usually at lower
concentrations than aliphatic esters, which are known to comprise the
qualitatively and quantitatively predominant aliphatic components in a wide
variety of plant odours (Maarse,
1991; Knudsen et al.,
1993
).
Despite the obvious role played by the sense of smell in finding and selecting food in many species, it should be emphasized that dietary specialization is only one of (presumably) numerous factors that make up the ecological niche of a species and that are likely also to affect its pattern of olfactory sensitivity and discrimination ability. To identify such factors and their impact on measures of olfactory performance warrants further study.
A final aspect of the present study is our finding of a significant
negative correlation between detection thresholds obtained in both squirrel
monkeys and pigtail macaques and carbon chain length of the aliphatic 1- and
2-alcohols tested (see Figs 3,
6). The same regular
association between olfactory sensitivity and this molecular property of the
odorants has been found in human subjects
(Cometto-Muniz and Cain, 1990;
see Fig. 7) and in rats
(Moulton, 1960
; see
Fig. 7). Corresponding
correlations have also been found for homologous series of aliphatic
carboxylic acids (Laska et al.,
2000
) and acetic esters (Laska and Seibt, 2001) in squirrel
monkeys and pigtail macaques as well as in humans
(Cometto-Muniz and Cain, 1991
;
Cometto-Muniz et al., 1998
),
suggesting that this type of correlation might not be restricted to the class
of odorants tested here but may represent a more general phenomenon.
In contrast, we found no correlation between olfactory detection thresholds and the second molecular feature studied here, i.e. the position of the functional alcohol group. Both squirrel monkeys and pigtail macaques showed very similar threshold values for 1-, 2- and 3-pentanol (see Fig. 7) and for 1- and 2-propanol and for 1- and 2-butanol, respectively, suggesting that, at least for the class of aliphatic alcohols, the position of the oxygen moiety has little effect on detectability. This finding, too, is in agreement with reports in human subjects (see Fig. 7).
In conclusion, the results of the present study provide further evidence of a well-developed olfactory sensitivity in two non-human primate species, the squirrel monkey and the pigtail macaque. These findings support the idea that olfaction may play an important role in the regulation of behaviour in these species. Further, they suggest that across-species comparisons of neuroanatomical features are a poor predictor of olfactory performance and that general labels such as `microsmat' and `macrosmat' are inadequate to describe a species' olfactory capabilities. An ecological view of such capabilities that attempts to correlate sensory performance with the behavioural relevance of odour stimuli might offer a promising approach in appraising the significance of the sense of smell for a particular species.
![]() |
Acknowledgments |
---|
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Bolen, R. H. and Green, S. M. (1997). Use of olfactory cues in foraging by owl monkeys (Aotus nancymai) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J. Comp. Psychol. 111,152 -158.[Medline]
Brown, W. M. (2001). Natural selection of mammalian brain components. Trends Ecol. Evol. 16,471 -473.
Clutton-Brock, T. H. and Harvey, P. H. (1977). Species differences in feeding and ranging behaviour in primates. In Primate Ecology: Studies of Feeding and Ranging Behaviour in Lemurs, Monkeys and Apes (ed. T. H. Clutton-Brock), pp.557 -584. New York: Academic Press.
Cometto-Muniz, J. E. and Cain, W. S. (1990). Thresholds for odor and nasal pungency. Physiol. Behav. 48,719 -725.[Medline]
Cometto-Muniz, J. E. and Cain, W. S. (1991). Nasal pungency, odor and eye irritation thresholds for homologous acetates. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 39,983 -989.[Medline]
Cometto-Muniz, J. E., Cain, W. S. and Abraham, M. H. (1998). Nasal pungency and odor of homologous aldehydes and carboxylic acids. Exp. Brain Res. 118,180 -188.[Medline]
Corbit, T. E. and Engen, T. (1971). Facilitation of olfactory detection. Percept. Psychophys. 10,433 -436.
Devos, M., Patte, F., Rouault, J., Laffort, P. and van Gemert, L. J. (1990). Standardized Human Olfactory Thresholds. Oxford: IRL Press.
De Winter, W. and Oxnard, C. E. (2001). Evolutionary radiations and convergences in the structural organization of mammalian brains. Nature 409,710 -714.[Medline]
Dusenbery, D. B. (1992). Sensory Ecology. How Organisms Acquire and Respond to Information. New York: Freeman.
Epple, G., Belcher, A. M., Küderling, I., Zeller, U., Scolnick, L., Greenfield, K. L. and Smith, A. B. (1993). Making sense out of scents: species differences in scent glands, scent-marking behavior and scent-mark composition in the Callitrichidae. In Marmosets and Tamarins: Systemactics, Behavior and Ecology (ed. A. B. Rylands), pp.123 -151. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Farbman, A. I. (1992). Cell Biology of Olfaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Flood, P. (1985). Sources of significant smells: the skin and other organs. In Social Odours in Mammals (ed. R. E. Brown and D. W. MacDonald), pp.19 -36. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Hellman, T. M. and Small, F. H. (1974). Characterisation of the odor properties of 101 petrochemicals using sensory methods. J. Air Pollut. Contr. Assoc. 24,979 -982.[Medline]
Heymann, E. W. (1998). Sex differences in olfactory communication in a primate, the moustached tamarin, Saginus mystax (Callitrichinae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 43, 37-45.
Hübener, F. and Laska, M. (1998). Assessing olfactory performance in an Old World primate, Macaca nemestrina. Physiol. Behav. 64,521 -527.[Medline]
Hübener, F. and Laska, M. (2001). A two-choice discrimination method to assess olfactory performance in pigtailed macaques, Macaca nemestrina. Physiol. Behav. 72,511 -519.[Medline]
Hudson, R., Laska, M. and Ploog, D. (1992). A new method for testing perceptual and learning capacities in unrestrained small primates. Folia Primatol. 59, 56-60.[Medline]
Kappeler, P. (1998). To whom it may concern: the transmission and function of chemical signals in Lemur catta.Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42,411 -421.
King, J. E. and Fobes, J. L. (1974). Evolutionary changes in primate sensory capacities. J. Human Evol. 3,435 -443.
Knudsen, J. T., Tollsten, L. and Bergström, L. G. (1993). Floral scents A checklist of volatile compounds isolated by head-space techniques. Phytochemistry 33,253 -280.
Laing, D. G. (1982). Characterisation of human behaviour during odour perception. Perception 11,221 -230.[Medline]
Laska, M. (1990). Olfactory sensitivity to food odor components in the short-tailed fruit bat, Carollia perspicillata.J. Comp. Physiol. A 166,395 -399.
Laska, M., Alicke, T. and Hudson, R. (1996). A study of long-term odor memory in squirrel monkeys, Saimiri sciureus.J. Comp. Psychol. 110,125 -130.[Medline]
Laska, M. and Freyer, D. (1997). Olfactory discrimination ability for aliphatic esters in squirrel monkeys and humans. Chem. Senses 22,457 -465.[Abstract]
Laska, M. and Hudson, R. (1993a). Assessing olfactory performance in a New World primate, Saimiri sciureus.Physiol. Behav. 53,89 -95.[Medline]
Laska, M. and Hudson, R. (1993b). Discriminating parts from the whole: determinants of odor mixture perception in squirrel monkeys, Saimiri sciureus. J. Comp. Physiol. A 173,249 -256.[Medline]
Laska, M. and Hudson, R. (1995). Ability of female squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) to discriminate between conspecific urine odours. Ethology 99, 39-52.
Laska, M., Liesen, A. and Teubner, P. (1999a).
Enantioselectivity of odor perception in squirrel monkeys and humans.
Am. J. Physiol. 277,R1098
-R1103.
Laska, M. and Seibt, A. (2002). Olfactory sensitivity for aliphatic esters in squirrel monkeys and pigtail macaques. Behav. Brain Res. (in press).
Laska, M., Seibt, A. and Weber, A. (2000).
`Microsmatic' primates revisited Olfactory sensitivity in the squirrel
monkey. Chem. Senses 25,47
-53.
Laska, M. and Teubner, P. (1998). Odor
structureactivity relationships of carboxylic acids correspond between
squirrel monkeys and humans. Am. J. Physiol.
274,R1639
-R1645.
Laska, M., Trolp, S. and Teubner, P. (1999b). Odor structureactivity relationships correspond between human and non-human primates. Behav. Neurosci. 113,998 -1007.[Medline]
Maarse, H. (1991). Volatile Compounds in Foods and Beverages. New York: Marcel Dekker.
Mertl-Millhollen, A. S. (1986). Olfactory demarcation of territorial but not home range boundaries by Lemur catta.Folia Primatol . 50,175 -187.
Moulton, D. G. (1960). Studies in olfactory acuity. III. Relative detectability of n-aliphatic acetates by the rat. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 12,203 -213.
Passe, D. H. and Walker, J. C. (1985). Odor psychophysics in vertebrates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 9, 431-467.[Medline]
Ross, C. (1992). Basal metabolic rate, body weight and diet in primates: an evaluation of the evidence. Folia Primatol. 58,7 -23.[Medline]
Rouquier, S., Blancher, A. and Giorgi, D.
(2000). The olfactory receptor gene repertoire in primates and
mouse: evidence for reduction of the functional fraction in primates.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
97,2870
-2874.
Schoenemann, P. T. (2001). Brain scaling, behavioral ability and human evolution. Behav. Brain Sci. 24,293 -295.
Siegel, S. and Castellan, N. J. (1988). Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: McGraw Hill.
Smith, T. E. and Abbott, D. H. (1998). Behavioral discrimination between circumgenital odor from peri-ovulatory dominant and anovolatory female common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Am. J. Primatol. 46,265 -284.[Medline]
Stephan, H., Baron, G. and Frahm, H. D. (1988). Comparative size of brains and brain structures. In Comparative Primate Biology, vol. 4 (ed. H. Steklis and J. Erwin), pp. 1-38. New York: Alan R. Liss.
Stevens, J. C., Cain, W. S. and Burke, R. J. (1988). Variability of olfactory thresholds. Chem. Senses 13,643 -653.
Ueno, Y. (1994). Olfactory discrimination of eight food flavors in the capuchin monkey, Cebus apella: comparison between fruity and fishy odors. Primates 35,301 -310.
Walker, J. C. and Jennings, R. A. (1991). Comparison of odor perception in humans and animals. In The Human Sense of Smell (ed. D. G. Laing, R. L. Doty and W. Breipohl), pp.261 -280. Berlin: Springer.
Weast, R. C. (1987). Handbook of Chemistry and Physics. 68th edition. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Related articles in JEB: