High-speed video analysis of wing-snapping in two manakin clades (Pipridae: Aves)
1 Museum of Vertebrates, Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology,
Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA
2 Natural History Museum and Biodiversity Research Center, and Department of
Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS 66045,
USA
* Author for correspondence (e-mail: ksb6{at}cornell.edu)
Accepted 14 July 2003
![]() |
Summary |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Key words: Manacus, Pipra, sonation, kinematics, acoustic communication, courtship display
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Several characteristics of avian sonations have prevented research on sound production mechanisms. Sounds are usually produced in behavioral contexts that are difficult to induce in captivity, often during flight, using motions too rapid to be seen by the unaided eye or conventional-speed video recording, and by species in remote and/or restricted localities around the world. These factors, combined with the fact that many birds are prohibitively difficult to obtain, maintain and manipulate in laboratory settings, have prevented mechanism-oriented studies of this class of communicative signals in birds.
Nonetheless, sonations play important roles in many species' behavioral
repertoires: they are frequently used in concert with vocalizations for
territorial maintenance (Aubin,
1972; Winkler and Short,
1978
; Miller and Inouye,
1983
; Craig, 1984
;
McBurney, 1989
), or in
polygynous or lek-breeding species as elements of complex courtship displays
(Prum, 1998
). Morphologically,
birds use diverse structures for sound production, and feather modifications
have been described for many species known to sonate (overviews by
Darwin, 1871
;
Manson-Barr and Pye, 1985
;
Prum,
1998
;Bostwick, in
press
).
The most extreme development of the use of sonation is found in the
Neotropical manakins, the Pipridae (Prum,
1998). The Pipridae include approximately 40 species of
polygynous, lek-breeding, sexually dimorphic birds found in the tropical
forests of Central and South America. Male manakins perform elaborate
courtship displays, and over half of the species sonate during these displays
(Prum, 1998
). Phylogenetic
analyses of the Pipridae indicate numerous independent evolutionary origins of
sonation within the family, and extensive evolution within piprid genera and
species (Prum, 1998
;
Bostwick, 2000
). Included among
the diversity of piprid sonations are several acoustic classes of sounds, the
most common of which are brief, broad-frequency pulses
(Prum, 1998
).
Because many bird species sonate using their wings
(Manson-Barr and Pye, 1985;
Prum, 1998
), hypotheses of
sound-producing mechanisms have been proposed previously, and an examination
of the literature yields two basic acoustic classes with accompanying
mechanistic hypotheses. The first class includes relatively loud, brief,
broad-frequency (or toneless) sounds, variously referred to as snaps,
clicks, claps or cracks. Many species are known to snap
with their wings (Chapman,
1935
; Skutch,
1949
,
1969
;
Sick, 1959
; Snow,
1962
,
1963
,
1977
;
Payne, 1973
;
Bertram, 1977
;
Schwartz and Snow, 1978
;
Robbins, 1983
;
Bomford, 1986
;
Sankaran, 1996
;
Prum, 1998
;
Tello, 2001
), and competing,
speculative hypotheses of the kinematic mechanisms behind wing snaps
include: (1) wing-to-wing claps above the body, (2) wing-to-wing claps below
the body, (3) wing-to-body claps, (4) striking together of adjacent secondary
feathers or (5) sudden interruption of airflow between wing-flaps (for a
review, see Bostwick, in
press
). None of these hypotheses have been critically examined or
tested. The second class of wing-generated sonations includes a variety of
airy, pulsed and tonal sounds produced in flight, variously referred to as
whistles, humms or whirrs. These are universally attributed
to vibrations induced in flight feathers when air is forced through them at
relatively rapid speeds (Miller,
1925
; Pettingill,
1936
; Carr-Lewty,
1943
; Tuck, 1972
;
Craig, 1984
;
Miskelly, 1990
). This
hypothesis, while reasonable, is supported by little experimental data, and an
alternative hypothesis proposed here, of friction among feathers, has not been
considered (for a review, see Bostwick, in
press
).
Extreme modifications of wing morphology
(Lowe, 1942;
Schultz et al., 2001
;
Bostwick, 2002
) and physiology
(Schultz and Schlinger, 1999
;
Saldanha et al., 2000
) have
been documented in several sonating piprids, especially the genus
Manacus, but the function of the morphological modifications has
remained unexplained due to lack of information on the mechanisms of sound
production. The goal of the research presented here was to test the
mechanistic hypotheses delineated above in order to understand the functional
role of the modified piprid wing morphology. To this end, we distinguish
between two levels of mechanistic description: (1) kinematic mechanisms, or
the gross-level motions of the body and limbs used to sonate, vs. (2)
physical mechanisms, or the finer-level interactions of structures with air
that create sound. We here test several sonations for congruence with one of
four kinematic hypotheses (modified from above): (1) wing-to-wing claps above
the body, (2) wing-to-wing claps below the body, (3) wing-to-body claps and
(4) wing flicks into air. These hypotheses make obvious, non-overlapping
predictions about observed motions of the wings relative to each other and the
body. Identifying the fundamental kinematic mechanisms underlying sound
production will better enable functional interpretation of morphological
modifications found in piprids. Three competing hypotheses of the physical
mechanisms of sound production are suggested for wing snap sonations,
and are examined here preliminarily: (1) percussion (created by forceful
contact between solid objects), (2) whip-like sonic booms (created when an
object moves faster then the speed of sound), and (3) vacuum-created pressure
claps (created when a low pressure center is suddenly collapsed). Contact
between two solid structures is a minimum critical prediction for a percussive
mechanism. Extremely rapid motion of a structure through air is necessary for
either the whip or vacuum mechanisms, with the whip mechanism requiring
motions faster than the speed of sound. The absence of these respective
actions during sound production amounts to rejection of these respective
hypotheses.
Representatives from two lineages of piprid hypothesized to have evolved
sonation independently, Manacus and Pipra
(Prum, 1998;
Bostwick, 2000
), are examined
in detail. Many behaviorally and acoustically distinct sonations have been
recognized in, and are shared by, the four species in the genus
Manacus (Chapman,
1935
; Snow, 1962
;
Skutch, 1969
). The sonations
of Manacus will be referred to as follows: (1) snaps, (2)
rolls or roll-snaps, (3) snorts, (4)
whirrs, (5) rattles and (6) fanning (terms used
previously in the literature; see Chapman,
1935
; Snow, 1962
;
Skutch, 1969
). The mechanical
sounds of Pipra mentalis have been described in less detail; however,
both of the two classes of sounds described above have been recognized: a
low-frequency humm, and brief, broad frequency snaps used in
multiple behavioral contexts (poops and kloks, respectively;
Skutch, 1949
,
1969
).
Recent advances in high-speed video technology make it possible to record
sonation kinematics in field conditions
(Dalton, 2002). Here we report
the results of the analysis of high-speed video-recordings of Manacus
and Pipra wing-sound production in wild and captive conditions. We
verify the non-vocal nature of the sounds, analyze the acoustic
characteristics of each sonation observed, distinguish the behavioral contexts
in which different sounds are produced, and delineate the gross-level
kinematics behind sound production where possible. We test each of four
kinematic hypotheses of sound production, and make preliminary examinations
for each of three physical hypotheses, for each of several snap
sonations. In doing so, we uncover a previously unknown radiation of
mechanistic diversity for sound-production in birds.
![]() |
Materials and methods |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Two video cameras recorded simultaneously: a digital camcorder (with both NTSC standard and progressive scan recording modes) to record sound and behavior simultaneously, and a high-speed digital video camera to capture the details of sound-producing motions. The conventional Sony DCR VX2000 miniDV camcorder (Tokyo, Japan) was used with a 1.7x teleconverter lens, and shutter speeds of 1/60-1/2500 s (as permitted by light conditions). Recordings were made using Sony's progressive scan recording mode, which captures 15 full (all pixels) images per second. A high-speed Redlake MotionMeter digital video camera (San Diego, CA, USA) was used with a 28-70 mm 1-2.8f zoom lens, recording speeds of 60, 125, 250, 500 and 1000 frames per second, and shutter speeds of 1/60-1/4000 s. Individual video frames, taken from simultaneous recordings with the Sony and high-speed video cameras, were visually aligned to provide maximum synchronization with the Sony audio recordings to within 1, 2 or 4 ms (for high-speed recordings at 1000, 500 and 250 frames per second, respectively). This resolution was sufficient for description of the overall kinematics of sound production and distinguishing between alternative kinematic hypotheses, but it was not sufficient for determining the exact moment of sound production, and thus distinguishing between competing hypotheses of physical mechanisms of sound production. Acoustic samples were augmented with additional tape recordings made with a Sony TCM5000 cassette recorder and a Sennheiser ME80 (Wedemark, Germany) microphone.
Two captive individual males of M. manacus were recorded at the San Diego Zoo (1 March, 2001) and San Diego Wild Animal Park (28 February - 5 March, 2001). Two wild individuals of M. candei were recorded in La Selva Biological Reserve, Prov. Heredia, Costa Rica, on 13-15 March, 2001. Wild M. aurantiacus were recorded in Carara National Park, Prov. Puntarenas, Costa Rica on 23 March, 2001. Displays and sonations in M. aurantiacus appeared identical to those of other Manacus species and therefore were only recorded by conventional video. Recordings of at least four individual male P. mentalis were made in La Selva Biological Reserve, Prov. Heredia, Costa Rica, at 800-850 m on 9-15 March, 2001.
Video and audio analysis
Male manakins display and sonate incessantly during the breeding season
(Snow, 1962;
Skutch, 1969
;
Robbins, 1983
;
Bostwick, 2000
). Accordingly,
the observed individuals sonated dozens of times an hour, from 07:00 h-13:00
h, daily, during the 14 days of video-recording. As is common with
vocalizations in birds, each sonation type was audibly distinct and easily
identifiable, and the behavioral contexts in which individual sonation types
were employed were non-overlapping. Attempts were made to record each sonation
from numerous perspectives to best characterize the motions involved in sound
production. Video footage was edited in iMovie 2 (Apple Computer, Inc.,
Cupertino, CA, USA), and organized by species and sonation type. Acoustic and
kinematic analysis (below) showed that the acoustic properties and kinematics
for each sonation are distinct from each other, congruent with the field-based
distinctions. Sample sizes of the high-speed video recordings examined to
characterize the kinematics of each sonation type vary and are given with each
sonation description (below). Durations of sound-producing behaviors were
calculated from the frame numbers and recording speeds.
Acoustic samples of each distinct sonation were taken from video and audio
recordings and analyzed in Canary 1.2.1
(Charif et al., 1995). Sample
sizes varied with the frequency at which the sounds were produced and
recorded. Variables such as number of sound pulses or notes, intervals between
pulses, and peak (or most powerful) frequency were measured for each sound as
appropriate. The majority of the sonations described are characterized by
being very brief, broad-frequency pulses. Pulse duration and frequency range
are inherently difficult to characterize in such sounds
(Bradbury and Vehrencamp,
1998
), a problem exacerbated by the variety of recording
conditions experienced in the field. Therefore, we only report measurements of
pulse duration, or peak frequency, of these brief, broad-frequency pulses, for
sounds in which these measures are reproducible. Measurements of homologous
sounds from each of the three species of Manacus were initially
analyzed separately. Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were run on species means
of pulse number and inter-pulse interval for the four potentially variable
sonations produced by Manacus, only one of which varied
significantly. Therefore, with the exception of this one variable
(rattle interpulse interval), Manacus sound measurements are
pooled among the three species. Values are given as means ±
S.D. and range.
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Pipra mentalis also produces five distinct sonations, here named
the click, rub-snap, clap, humm and swoop. Male P.
mentalis also display within lek territories, typically on one or two
main display perches in the sub-canopy 7-20 m from the ground
(Skutch, 1969;
Prum, 1990
). Three sounds, the
click, rub-snap and clap, are performed while perched. The
other two sounds, the swoop and humm, are performed in
flight. Three of the sonations, clicks, humms and
rub-snaps, are produced together in stereotyped sequence in
to-and-fro display flights (sensu
Prum, 1990
). The to-and-fro
display entails two short (1-2 m) flights away from and then back to the main
display perch of the male. Each of the two flights is preceded by a
click, accompanied by a humm, and the display ends with one
rub-snap after the return flight (N>50). The timing of
these elements are: click-to-humms 25.35±0.77 ms
(range 23.90-26.30 ms; N=20); 1st click to 2nd
click 1095.2±152.0 ms (854.0-1328.0 ms; N=17); 2nd
click to rub-snap 711.2±87.8 ms (564.0-854.0 ms;
N=19).
Each sonation is described separately, with the acoustic characteristics described first, followed by kinematic descriptions, and finally the proposed kinematic and physical mechanisms where possible. Five sonations, the snap, roll-snap, click, rub-snap and clap, are acoustically similar in being loud, brief, broad frequency snaps, and these are described first. The kinematics involved in the production of the six non-snap sonations, the snort, rattle, whirr, fanning, humm and swoop, are, in general, more difficult to characterize and therefore are only discussed briefly.
Snap sonations
Snap: Manacus
The sound is a sharp, powerful snap or whip-like crack.
Acoustically, it is a single powerful, brief, broad-frequency pulse of sound
(Fig. 1). The peak, or most
powerful, frequency of a pulse is 4.61±0.90 kHz (2.80-6.12 kHz;
N=57).
|
The snap is produced while the male is airborne by clapping the dorsal surfaces of the wings together above the back of the bird (N=28 high-speed recordings). The male perches horizontally near the base of a vertical sapling bordering his display court in preparation for a spring/snap across the court to another vertical sapling. First he crouches against the sapling-perch, his back facing the direction in which he is about to spring (Fig. 2.1). While crouching, he first supinates his humerus, rotating it outward along its long axis; this action has the effect of elevating the wrists slightly above his back (Fig. 2.2). Next he extends his legs to launch himself forcefully from the perch (Fig. 2.3). As he springs, back first, he extends his wrist and elbow joints so that his wings form a flat plane across his back (as if in a `soaring' position, Fig. 2.3,4). After leaving the perch, he rapidly adducts his open wings over his back by rapidly retracting and further supinating the humerus. This action forcefully claps together the dorsal surfaces of the wrist joints, approximately where the bases of the remiges insert (Fig. 2.5,6). The male then rotates the leading edge of the wing outward in a pronating motion (Fig. 2.7-2.8). Finally, the wings are either closed to a resting position at the sides, or begin a downstroke that determines the subsequent maneuvering of the male's body (Fig. 2.9,10), before he alights on a neighboring vertical perch. All wing-to-wing contact occurs as the male sails through the air from the momentum of his jump between perches.
|
Kinematically, the snap is produced using a wing-against-wing clap of the dorsal surfaces of the wings above the male's body, consistent with hypothesis 1 (see Introduction). The kinematics observed eliminate the alternative hypotheses of (2) wing-to-wing percussion below the body, (3) wing-to-body claps and (4) wing flicks into air, as none of the motions predicted for these alternative hypotheses are observed. None of the three hypotheses of the physical mechanism of sound production can be eliminated: the sound may be produced either by percussion (of the wrists, flight feather bases, or more distal parts of the secondary feathers), or by a whip-like or collapsing vacuum mechanism possibly produced when the tips of the flight feathers whip through the air as a result of wing collision. The wrist contact is distinctly observable on the video, and is powerful, as indicated by the way with which the wing reverberates from this contact. Contact appears to occur near the swollen bases of modified secondary feathers, thus supporting a percussive mechanism of sound production. The nature of wing contact and feather motions subsequent to the collision of the wrists and swollen feather bases is difficult to discern, and may be a by-product of the pronating motion involved in wing recovery.
Roll-snap: Manacus
The roll-snap sounds like a loud, drawn-out, explosive,
crrrackkkk! Acoustically, it is a rapidly delivered series of pulses
of sound each of which is similar in structure to the snap
(Fig. 1). Roll-snaps
include 7.10±3.0 pulses (3-14 pulses; N=31). Intervals between
the snap pulses in a roll-snap are 19.91±1.1 ms
(17.85-22.48 ms; N=29). Peak pulse frequency is 3.95±0.67 kHz
(2.24-5.24 kHz; N=29).
The fundamental motion used to produce roll-snaps is the same one used to produce snaps; the dorsal surfaces of the wings are struck together above the back. However, the male remains perched on a horizontal branch for the duration of the sonation, and since sound production involves a rapidly repeated train of pulses, the set-up and recovery of the wing form a repeatable cycle (N=11 high-speed recordings).
A perched male leans forward and elevates his wrists through humeral supination (Fig. 3.2). He momentarily opens his wings laterally into a `soaring' position by extending the elbow and wrist joints (Fig. 3.3-5), before he then raises and retracts his open wings rapidly above his back, leading with the wrist, adducting them until the wrists forcefully collide (Fig. 3.6-9), as in snap production. The wing is then pronated, pulling the leading edges of the wings apart, while the tips of the flight feathers continue to move medially towards each other. As the distal tips of the flight feathers nearly meet, the wrists have reached their full lateral extent (Fig. 3.9-16). The wing is then supinated and retracted, bringing the wrists together again to collide, making the second pulse of sound, and swinging the tips of the flight feathers outwards (Fig. 3.16-19). This motion is repeated once for each snap pulse in the roll-snap. During this activity, the head, body and tail remain motionless, and the extension of the elbow and wrist joints remains constant. All motion is limited to the orientation of the wings, which swivel inward and outward around the shoulder joint, above the body, in a graceful, cyclic motion.
|
Kinematically, the sound pulses in a roll-snap are produced by a series of the same, above-the-body, percussive, wing-against-wing movements that Manacus uses to produce single snaps. As with single snaps, sound-generation appears to occur physically by the percussive wing contact, but the alternative hypotheses cannot be eliminated.
Click: Pipra mentalis
This sound is a simple woody click, somewhat like two chopsticks
being hit together. Acoustically, the click is lower in amplitude
than the snap of Manacus, but is a similarly brief, broad
frequency pulse (Fig. 1).
Unlike the snaps of Manacus, these pulses exhibit variation
in the amplitude of different frequencies, although variation in quality of
recordings made it difficult to characterize these patterns. Peak pulse
frequency is 4.81±1.26 kHz (2.33-7.45 kHz; N=26), and the
duration of the click pulse was 3.57±0.50 ms (2.18-4.50 ms;
N=26).
Clicks are produced during a lateral wing flick and recovery prior to the male's departure from a perch (N=22 high-speed recordings). Preliminary wing movement for the click involves an extended set of motions (Fig. 4). The bird first rotates the humerus, abducting and raising the wrist joints, and then extends the wrist and elbow joints to open the wings above the back (Fig. 4.1-7). The raised wings are then protracted and depressed as if beginning a downstroke for flight (Fig. 4.7-10). However, unlike a downstroke for propulsive flight, this downward motion continues only until the wing tips are fully stretched laterally to the side of the body in a flat plane (Fig. 4.11) before the wings are drawn in (the wing tips are never depressed below the level of the bird's body, Fig. 4.12-16). Next, the wings are drawn in while the bird begins leaning forward and starts to extend his legs in preparation for flight (Fig. 4.17). Once the wings are fully retracted, the bird then elevates them again to begin the first propulsive downstroke, at which point the male finally leaves the perch (Fig. 4.17-24).
|
Since there is no wing-to-wing percussion, nor any wing-to-body percussion, these data support only one of the four competing kinematic hypotheses, the lateral wing-flick into the air. None of the three physical hypotheses can be eliminated; the sound may be made by collisions between adjacent feather rachii as the laterally extended wing is suddenly adducted toward the body, or alternatively, as the wing feather tips move rapidly through the air, an air compression/expansion mechanism may generate the sound, or a sonic whip-like mechanism.
Rub-snap: Pipra mentalis
The rub-snap sounds like a harsh, static-electric churt.
Acoustically the rub-snap is composed of three brief, powerful,
broad-frequency pulses produced in very rapid series
(Fig. 1). No variation in the
number of pulses was observed (N=64). Intervals between the pulses
are approximately 13.40±1.02 ms (12.40-17.51 ms; N=32). Peak
pulse frequencies were 4.45±1.22 kHz (2.84-7.60 kHz;
N=20).
This sound is produced while the bird is perched when the wing feather tips are rubbed down the sides of the tail in three rapidly repeated cycles (N=19 high-speed recordings). The wrist and primaries of each wing are first depressed along the sides of the body (Fig. 5A,B,2,3), which causes the tips of the primaries to slide across the base of the fanned tail (Fig. 5A,B,3-5), and then the still-flexed wing is abducted slightly outward and elevated in a recovery stroke (Fig. 5A,B,5-7). This motion is repeated in full three times, corresponding to the three pulses of sound seen in the spectrogram. Overall, during each cycle the wrist moves in a circular motion, moving caudoventrally, then laterally, then craniodorsally, and finally medially back to the starting position. The tail is fanned and elevated as the primary tips slide past in the opposite direction (Fig. 5A,B,2-4) and it relaxes back to its relatively ventral position while the wings are recovered to their dorsal position (Fig. 5A,B,6-8). The head, neck, and the breast are raised and stretched upwards and then back to a resting position in one smooth continuous motion made during the three wing rubs.
|
This kinematic mechanism does not fit the predictions of any of the four previously hypothesized mechanisms, and thus represents a new class of kinematic mechanisms: wing and tail feather interaction/rub. Physically, the rub-snap may be created by any of the three proposed mechanisms as follows: (1) by primary-against-tail feather collisions (percussion), (2) by collisions among adjacent flight feathers elastically loaded by tail resistance (percussion), or (3) through a rapid flick of the elastically loaded tips of the primaries into the air (sonic-boom or collapsing vacuum mechanisms). The first hypothesis is weakest because the tail and wings are not clapped or beat together in any forceful way, as would be expected for a sound made by percussion; rather, they appear to slide past one another. The video footage also shows that individual wing feathers bend, separate from, and then rejoin each other as the wings slides past the tail in a way that is more consistent with the second and third hypotheses. Specifically, as the feathers slip past the tail, successive feather vanes or rachii may clap against each other to produce sound, or alternatively whip rapidly into the air.
Claps: Pipra mentalis
Together a series of claps create an explosive, tack! tack!
tack! sound, like a toy machine-gun. Claps were given in series
of 3-10 in a row (6.38±2.45; N=8). Acoustically, each `tack'
or `clap' is actually two distinct and brief broad-frequency pulses in quick
succession. The two pulses are not identical; the first is relatively
low-amplitude, and its range of frequencies higher; the second is louder, more
abrupt, and of a lower range of frequencies than the first pulse
(Fig. 1). The dominant
frequency of the first and second pulses are 4.48±1.28 kHz (2.89-7.12
kHz; N=8) and 3.90±0.63 kHz (2.76-4.80 kHz; N=8),
respectively. The two pulses within a clap are separated by
22.17±0.76 ms (20.97-23.36 ms; N=8), and the interval between
consecutive claps is 185.01±10.37 ms (170.84-201.48 ms;
N=8).
While perched in a stereotypic head-down, tail-raised posture, each
double-pulsed clap is made by two movements: (1) a wing flick into
the air (Fig. 6.7) followed by
(2) a forceful clap of the wings against the sides of the body
(Fig. 6.13-14, N=24
high-speed recordings). First, the perched male raises his wings above his
back as if he was going to make a normal propulsive wing-beat
(Fig. 6.1-6). The first of the
two pulses of sound is made by a rustle or flick of the partially open wings
before the downstroke begins. This flick is executed by first raising the
posterior edge of the wing to about a 45° angle above the body while
opening the wrist and elbow joints slightly. The wing is then elevated a few
degrees more before a subtle pronation motion initiates the downstroke
(Fig. 6.7). The timing of the
elevation-pronation transition corresponds to the production of the first
sound pulse, and a wave or rustling of the secondary feathers is visible
(Fig. 6.7). Subsequent to the
wing flick, the second sound pulse is made by the collision of the wings
against the body. The opened elevated wings are pulled downward through the
air, with the primary feathers bending backward from air resistance
(Fig. 6.8-10). When the wings
are fully extended laterally, the male flexes the elbow and wrist joints,
forcefully closing and adducting the wings against his body
(Fig. 6.10-12). The thigh in
particular reverberates with the impact of the wing, coincident with the
production of the second pulse of sound
(Fig. 6.14). This act
effectively returns the wing to its resting position. The bird stands
motionless, still leaning forward over the perch, often with the tail
vibrating slightly, before opening the wings again, 180 ms later, to make
the next clap.
|
Kinematically, the motions observed in the first pulse are consistent with the wing-flick into the air hypothesis, and motions observed to produce the second pulse are consistent with the wing-to-body hypothesis; thus, two previously proposed kinematic mechanisms are used to produce this one sonation. As with the click and rub-snap, we cannot reject any physical hypothesis on how the first pulse is made: either collisions between adjacent secondary feather rachii, or by creation of a low-pressure center behind the wing as it is flicked slightly upward before the downstroke. The second pulse, however, corresponds with a percussive event after which there is no wing motion, which precludes the non-percussive hypotheses, thus this sound appears to be made by the percussion of the ventral wing surface against the body.
Non-snap sonations
Snort: Manacus
The wing snort is a low flatulent sound. Acoustically, it is a
series of relatively soft, brief, rapidly repeated, low-frequency pulses
(Fig. 7). The number of pulses
comprising a snort is 10.22±1.20 pulses (8-13 pulses;
N=23). Intervals between pulses are 12.44±0.98 ms (10.02-13.51
ms; N=22). Peak pulse frequency is 2.25±0.44 kHz (1.53-2.98
kHz; N=23).
|
The wing snort is produced by a series of rapid lateral flicks of the primary feathers, made by a modified wing-beat as the male springs nearly vertically from the cleared ground of his court to the top of one of the vertical saplings bordering the court (Fig. 8, N=13 high-speed recordings). As the male rises from the ground, he flicks the manus and primaries laterally in a rapid extention/flexion motion of the wrist and elbow joints (Fig. 8.4,7,10). Analysis of numerous recordings indicate these sound-producing wing-beats are shallow, with an arc or excursion of only about 60°. The intervals between successive wing flicks are extremely short (12.44±0.98 ms), such that the wings cycle twice as fast as in normal propulsive flight (27.14±2.4 ms; N=21). Each wing stroke corresponds to one pulse of sound, thus we hypothesize that the sounds are physically produced by the rapid motion of the primary feathers through the air, an action which may briefly induce vibrations in the feathers. Males were occasionally observed to produce this sound while perched.
|
Rattle: Manacus
Wing rattles sound like low nasal snickers. M. manacus
only rattled infrequently, therefore no acoustic recordings were
made, and measurements are from M. candei and M. aurantiacus
only. Acoustically, rattles are 1-3 (2.09±0.73; N=23) brief,
soft, low-frequency pulses (Fig.
7). Pulse intervals differed significantly between M.
candei and M. aurantiacus: 15.57±0.86 ms
(14.1-16.5 ms; N=10) for M. candei, and 20.29±1.13 ms
(18.3-22.1 ms; N=7) for M. aurantiacus. The peak pulse
frequency of 2.38±0.27 kHz (1.46-2.84 kHz; N=23) did not
differ between species.
Production of rattles was not captured on high-speed video; these sounds are produced sporadically during short flights around the male's territories, often moments before descending to the display court (K.S.B., personal observation). Acoustically, the peak frequencies of rattles overlap with those of snorts. We hypothesize that the same basic kinematic and physical mechanisms are used to generate the sound; modified wing-beats in the form of rapid lateral flicks may be used to induce resonant vibrations in modified primary feathers. Differences in the intervals between pulses in snorts and rattles may be due to the different behavioral contexts in which they are produced, the former made during courtship in a short (>1 m) vertical ascent from the forest floor, and the latter during level flight around the territory.
Whirr: Manacus
Wing whirrs are light reedy whirring sounds that are
frequently produced during normal propulsive flight, and may not be voluntary.
Acoustically, they are a rapid series of very low-amplitude, brief, relatively
broad frequency pulses of sound (Fig.
7). Peak frequency could not be characterized. Pulse numbers are
highly variable, 9.04±4.1 (4-21; N=23). This is probably due
to the variable number of wing-beats needed for varying lengths of flight.
Intervals between pulses are 26.63±1.5 (23.61-29.93; N=21),
which is not significantly different from independently measured wing-beat
intervals measured from high-speed video (250-1000 frames s-1) of
flying birds, 27.14±2.4 ms (24-32 ms; N=21), (P=0.47,
t=-0.73, d.f.=33).
Kinematics used for production of this sound were not distinguishably different from normal flight motions captured on high-speed video. We hypothesize that these sounds are also made by the modified primaries when moved through the air in normal propulsive flight. However, the rate of airflow and/or the positioning of the feathers may not induce the same resonant vibrations that produce the more tonal snort and rattle sounds, and may be due to friction or non-resonant feather vibration. The degree to which this sound can be intentionally modulated is not known.
Fanning: Manacus
Fanning sounds like a persistent reedy rustling. Acoustically it
is a rapidly delivered series of very low-amplitude, brief, broad-frequency
pulses (Fig. 7). Peak frequency
could not be adequately characterized. Intervals between pulses are
52.44±2.13 ms (47.90-55.57 ms; N=9), two to four times slower
than other mechanical sounds produced by Manacus. Males assume
fanning behavior and can continue for several minutes at a time, such
that the number of pulses is highly variable.
This sound is produced by continuous lateral flicks of the primary feathers of a perched male made at a rate four times slower than snorts (N=2 high-speed recordings). The male assumes a crouching position, so that his legs are not visible beneath his body, which is low and horizontal (Fig. 9.1). The male rotates his humerus, apparently to orient the wing into the correct posture for lateral opening. He extends the elbow and wrist joints, such that the wing surface opens out laterally, and the primaries are flicked outward (Fig. 9.2). The wing is slightly depressed as the elbow and wrist joints are flexed, closing the wing in to a resting position (Fig. 9.2,3), from which the cycle begins again.
|
We hypothesize that fanning is made either by movement of air over modified primaries, or by the friction of flight feathers brushing across one another. The speed and position of the feathers as they move through the air do not appear to generate the resonant vibrations that produce the snorts and rattles. Nonetheless, this sound is clearly modulated and intentionally produced.
Fanning was reported in Manacus manacus by Snow
(1962), and was only observed
in Manacus manacus in this study.
Humm: Pipra mentalis
The sound is a low humm, reminiscent of that of a hummingbird in
flight, but deeper. Acoustically, it is a series of 2-5 (4.20±0.89;
N=20) low-frequency pulses (Fig.
7).Intervals between pulses were 21.11±2.02 ms (18.80-24.50
ms; N=20). Peak pulse frequency was 184.0±157.2 Hz (20.0-520.0
Hz; N=20), with high frequency measures of 982.0±52.7 Hz
(880.0-1090.0 Hz; N=20). The average duration of a single
low-frequency pulse was 6.50±0.91 ms (5.05-8.32 ms; N=20).
This sound is made in flight by the wing feathers as they move through the air during propulsive downstrokes shortly after the bird leaves its perch. How the wing kinematics used to produce the humm differ from that of normal propulsive flight was not determined. Humms are made in the first few (2-5) strokes of flight following a click and departure from a perch. Intervals between humm pulses are identical to intervals between successive flight strokes. It seems likely that humms are made when the feathers are moved through the air at a speed and posture that induces resonant vibrations.
Swoop: Pipra mentalis
The swoop is a subtle sound that is easily missed. It sounds like
a heavy object bounced once on a large rubber membrane, a soft gulping sound,
or a pluck on a contrabass string. Acoustically, it is relatively long in
duration 162.60±17.46 ms (119.60-178.20 ms; N=12), with a peak
frequency always measuring between 1 and 20 Hz (N=12) and a maximum
frequency of 664.2±68.9 Hz (510.0-750.0 Hz; N=12)
(Fig. 7).
The sound is made when the male, flying at extremely high speeds, dives precipitously in his flight trajectory, swooping toward the ground (N=22 conventional video recordings). Beyond this, the kinematics of sound production cannot be described in detail because production of this sound was not adequately captured on high-speed video. The swoop is acoustically similar to humms, and is probably produced by a similar physical mechanism. However, while humms are made when the wing is moved rapidly through the air during propulsive flight strokes, the swoop is most likely made as the whole wing is simply held opened at a critical moment when the rapid movement of the bird's body through the air can induce the vibrations necessary to produce the sound.
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Kinematic and physical mechanisms
The two basic acoustic classes of sonation known from other birds are found
in both clades of piprid studied. The first class of sounds,
wing-snaps, are made using three of four previously hypothesized
kinematic mechanisms: (1) clapping the dorsal surfaces of the wings together
above the back (snap and roll-snap of Manacus), (2)
clapping the ventral surface of the wings to the body (clap 2nd pulse
of P. mentalis), and (3) rapidly flicking wing feathers into the air
(click and clap 1st pulse of P. mentalis). A
fourth, previously unproposed mechanism of rubbing the primary feathers
against the tail feathers (rub-snap of P. mentalis) is
discovered. This multiplicity of kinematic mechanisms was not predicted and
highlights how poorly known these communicative signals are in birds.
The second class of sounds, the lower-amplitude and often lower-frequency, pulsed or sustained notes, are made by both Manacus (snorts, whirrs, rattles and fanning) and Pipra mentalis (humms and swoops). Most of these sounds are made using the basic kinematic motions of cyclical wing movements (the swoop excepted), mostly during flight (fanning excepted), but how these motions differ from normal, silent flight is not clear. At least snorts and fanning seem to emphasize a lateral extension/flexion motion of the elbow and wrist joints. That these sonations vary in the number, rate, and acoustic structure of pulses, may indicate the physical mechanisms involved in making these sounds may not be identical among them. That is, the wings may cycle regularly to produce sounds, but some cycling may induce resonant vibrations in primary feathers, while others may simply result in friction among adjacent flight feathers. More research is required to distinguish between these possibilities.
Our kinematic data describe the gross, body-level motions used to produce the class of snap sonations, thus allowing us to generate preliminary hypotheses at the finer level of the physical mechanisms involved in snap production. For most of the described sonations it is impossible to distinguish without further research which of three physical mechanisms: (1) percussion, (2) sonic-boom or (3) collapsed low-pressure center, are used to sonate in these two clades. Gross-level percussion (wings against wings or wing against body) is the kinematic and physical mechanism most likely to be used by Manacus in its snaps and rolls, and by P. mentalis in its claps (2nd pulse). Feather percussions among a suite of adjacent feathers may produce the rub-snaps, clicks and claps (1st pulse) of P. mentalis, or alternatively, any or all of these sounds may be produced by air-compression/expansion events or a miniature sonic boom caused by the rapid whip-like movement of the whole wing. As regards the non-snap sonations, induced vibrations of primary feathers may be involved in the production of snorts, whirrs, rattles and flanning of Manacus, and the swoops and humms of P. mentalis. Alternatively, we propose that friction created when feathers move over one another may also create these sounds, and is particularly likely to explain the broader-frequency, less tonal sonations, such as fanning and whirr in Manacus.
Morphological and physiological significance
Motor requirements for sound production in manakins have pushed them to
physiological extremes. If we can assume that the repeated, sequential
contractions of individual wing muscles are responsible for the repeated,
sequential or cyclical wing movements that we recorded, then given that these
wing cycles relate in a one-to-one manner with the sound pulses they produce,
the inter-pulse intervals of the sounds that we measured should accurately
represent the rates of muscle contraction used to produce these sounds. Thus,
wing cycle frequencies used to produce roll-snaps and snorts
in Manacus and rub-snaps in P. mentalis are
comparable to those used by hummingbirds for flight (Mason-Barr and Pye, 1985;
Lasiewski and Lasiewski, 1967;
Calder and Calder, 1992
).
These results thus provide independent support for the premise that `sonic
muscles', those used in sound production, are among the fastest vertebrate
muscles known. Specifically, the shaker muscles of a rattlesnake's rattle
contract at
90 Hz, several times faster than more normal vertebrate
locomotory muscles, which contract at 20-30 Hz
(Rome et al., 1996
). The
piprids described herein can employ similarly rapid wing cycles of 75 Hz
(P. mentalis, rub-snap) and 80 Hz (Manacus, snort), at least
doubling the contraction rates used in normal flight.
Previous morphological work has uncovered striking modifications of the
wing morphology of Manacus (Lowe,
1942; Schultz et al.,
2001
; Bostwick,
2002
; K.S.B., unpublished data) and other species in the Pipridae,
including P. mentalis (Bostwick,
2002
; K.S.B., unpublished data). The wing kinematics described
above provide a preliminary functional context in which to interpret the
morphological modifications found in Manacus, but they also
complicate morphological interpretation by showing that the snaps and
rolls of Manacus are produced by fundamentally different
wing kinematics than snorts and fanning (and presumably
whirrs and rattles). Thus, we now know that any given
morphological modification needs to be examined not only as a potential
modification for sonation in general, but as a modification for one of several
particular sonations. Fortunately, the four Manacus sonations
captured on video are probably the most functionally important ones
(Chapman, 1935
;
Snow, 1962
), and each employs
only one of two fundamentally different sound-generating motions; the
explosively loud snaps and rolls are produced by a medially
oriented clap of the dorsal wing surface, while snorts and
fanning are produced by a series of extremely rapid, lateral wing
flicks.
The snaps and roll-snaps are produced by forceful
adduction of the dorsal wing surface, as opposed to forceful depression of the
ventral wing surface, and thus involve generation of force in the direction
opposite that required for flight. The primary motion used to create the
medial clap appears to be supination and retraction of the humerus, while the
flexion of the wrist and elbow joints remains open and constant. We therefore
predict that those muscles whose primary action involves retraction and/or
supination of the humerus are largely responsible for the production of
snaps and roll-snaps. Two of the three largest flight
muscles, the scapulohumeralis caudalis and the supracoracoideus, retract the
humerus; the former also pronates, while the later supinates
(Raikow, 1985). Together with
the primary downstroke muscle, the pectoralis, these muscles have been
quantitatively compared in terms of mass and physiology between male and
female Manacus vitellinus and Taenopygia guttata (Zebra
finch). Male M. vitellinus exhibit muscular modifications in males
relative to females, and in M. vitellinus relative to the control
species (Schultz et al.,
2001
). The scapulohumeralis caudalis in particular exhibits the
greatest disparity in mass and fiber type composition
(Schultz et al., 2001
), and in
a manner consistent with those expected for increased force generation and
muscle contraction speeds. While consistent with our predictions, this
interspecific comparison (Schultz et al.,
2001
) was made between M. vitellinus and T.
guttata, of which the former were wild-caught, and the latter were
captive individuals of a distantly related, ecologically distinct species that
does not perform physical courtship displays. Thus, the great differences
between the comparative and control species allows for numerous alternative
explanations for the morphological differences found.
Additional support for our premise that observed modifications of male
Manacus shoulder myology, and the scapulohumeralis caudalis and the
supracoracoideus in particular, can be attributed to their use in
adducting/retracting the wing during snap production, is provided by
myological comparisons of M. manacus with other, wild-caught,
territorial manakin species that also perform physical displays but do not
wing-snap (Bostwick, 2002;
K.S.B., unpublished data). Such comparisons reveal that, in general, sonating
piprids have distinctive and localized morphological modifications relative to
non-sonating ones. Relative to non-sonating piprids, Manacus in
particular exhibits an exceptionally hypertrophied scapulohumeralis caudalis,
a moderately hypertrophied supracoracoideus, a modified humeral head (that
partially determines the mobility of the shoulder joint), and a scapula with a
uniquely widened blade among piprids (a modification that presumably
accommodates the hypertrophied scapulohumeralis caudalis;
Bostwick, 2002
).
There is anatomical and experimental evidence that the scapulohumeralis
caudalis retracts the humerus (Raikow,
1985; Dial, 1992
).
Wing-propelled divers such as penguins, which generate force during both the
downstroke and upstroke of the wing, have greatly enlarged scapulohumeralis
caudalis muscles, indicating that this muscle is indeed important for
generation of force in a dorsal/medial direction
(Dial, 1992
). Thus, the
understood role of the scapulohumeralis caudalis in humeral retraction, the
primary motion involved in creating the medial wing clap, makes its potential
role in sound production in general, and the snap and
roll-snap sonations specifically, extremely plausible. Further, the
moderately hypertrophied supracoracoideus probably aids in the humeral
retraction and adds the element of humeral supination observed, bringing the
leading/anterior edges of the wing into forceful contact.
In contrast to the snap-generating medial clap, in which the elbow
and wrist joints are held relatively fixed, the snort and
fanning sonations entail rapid lateral flicks executed by repeated
flexion/extention motions of the elbow and wrist joints. Numerous muscles in
the forewing of Manacus are hypertrophied, the most notable being the
elbow and wrist joint extensors, such as the humerotriceps and the extensor
metacarpi radialis, and the flight feather flexors, such as the flexor carpi
ulnaris cranialis and caudalis (Bostwick,
2002). Additional modifications of the forewing that are likely to
relate to the production of snorts include the extremely shortened
carpometacarpus of the manus (Bostwick,
2002
), which may promote rapid wing-flicking, and the deeply
incised, decurved and reduced primary feathers unique to Manacus
among manakins (Chapman,
1935
). Incised primary or tail feathers are common in species
known to sonate in flight display dives
(Miller, 1925
;
Pettingill, 1936
;
Carr-Lewty, 1943
;
Tuck, 1972
;
Craig, 1984
;
Miskelly, 1990
). Such feather
modifications are hypothesized to be an adaptation to enable the feathers to
vibrate more readily when air is forced through them at the high speeds
reached in dives.
Thus, the morphological modifications unique to Manacus are consistent with those expected to execute the sound-producing motions observed on video. The distinct kinematics used to produce different sonations appear to have resulted in several equally distinct morphological modifications. These preliminary analyses indicate that more detailed morphological work and kinematic data may very well yield a relatively sophisticated understanding of this unique functional-morphological system in Manacus.
Evolutionary and behavioral significance
Perhaps the most outstanding feature of this system is the diversity of
sonations represented by these two clades, and the multiple means by which
that diversity has been achieved. Thus, the two clades use largely
non-overlapping kinematics to produce sounds, both clades use more than one
kinematic mechanism, and at least Manacus also employs the same
mechanism in different behaviors to increase repertoire size. For instance,
Manacus uses the same kinematics, claps of the dorsal wing surface,
in a single airborne clap to produce the snap, and also while perched
in a repeated train of claps to create the roll-snap. Meanwhile,
P. mentalis increases its repertoire by employing a multiplicity of
kinematic mechanisms, creating its snaps in three distinct ways.
While this research uncovers unanticipated levels of diversity, these
observations are restricted to only two of as many as five phylogenetically
independent sonating clades, and only four of approximately 20 sonating
manakin species. Sonations made by members of other piprid genera
Corapipo, Ilicura, and Heterocercus may be independently
evolved, and are behaviorally and acoustically distinct from those reported
here. Existing information indicates that these clades are likely to use
kinematics and/or physical mechanisms of sound production distinct from those
described here (Prum,
1998).
The acoustic, behavioral, mechanistic and morphological diversity
surrounding sonation raises the question of what evolutionary forces may
underlie such rampant character evolution. The role of sonations in courtship
displays supports the hypothesis that piprid wing sounds have evolved by
female choice. Across aves, and in piprids in particular, the origin of
sonation is associated with female choice
(Prum, 1998). Thus, the
diverse acoustics, kinematics, morphology and behaviors involved in piprid
sonations document a sexually selected character radiation. Subsequent to the
multiple origins of sonation in the family, sexual selection has fostered an
explosive diversification in all aspects of biology relating to non-vocal wing
sound production.
![]() |
Acknowledgments |
---|
![]() |
Footnotes |
---|
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Aubin, A. E. (1972). Aural communication in ruffed grouse. Can. J. Zool. 50,1225 -1229.
Bennet-Clark, H. C. (1975). Sound production in insects. Sci. Prog. 62,263 -283.[Medline]
Bennet-Clark, H. C. and Young, D. (1992). A
model of the mechanism of sound production in cicadas. J. Exp.
Biol. 173,123
-153.
Bertram, B. C. (1977). Variation in the wing-song of the Flappet Lark. Anim. Behav. 25,165 -170.
Bomford, M. (1986). Breeding displays and calls of the Banded Dotterel (Charadrius bicinctus). Notornis 33,219 -232.
Bostwick, K. S. (2000). Display behaviors, mechanical sounds, and their implications for evolutionary relationships of the Club-winged Manakin (Machaeropterus deliciosus). Auk 117,465 -478.
Bostwick, K. S. (2002). Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of behavior, wing morphology, and the kinematics of mechanical sound production in the Neotropical manakins (Aves: Pipridae). In Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, pp.208 . Lawrence: University of Kansas.
Bostwick, K. S. (in press). Mechanisms of feather sonation in aves: unanticipated levels of diversity. Acta Zool. Sin.
Bradbury, J. W. and Vehrencamp, S. L. (1998). Principles of Animal Communication. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates.
Burrows, M. (2001). The kinematics and neural control of high-speed kicking movements in the locust. J. Exp. Biol. 204,3471 -3481.[Medline]
Burrows, M. and Morris, G. (2001). The kinematics and neural control of high-speed kicking movements in the locust. J. Exp. Biol. 204,3471 -3481.[Medline]
Calder, W. A. and Calder, L. L. (1992). Broad-tailed Hummingbird. In Birds of North America, vol. 16 (ed. P. Stettenheim and F. Gill), pp.1 -15. Philedelphia: Academy of Natural Sciences.
Carr-Lewty, R. A. (1943). The aerodynamics of the drumming of the common snipe. Br. Birds 36,230 -234.
Chapman, F. M. (1935). The courtship of Gould's Manakin (Manacus vitellinus vitellinus) on Barro Colorado Island, Canal Zone. Bull. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist. 68,471 -525.
Charif, R. A., Mitchell, S. and Clark, C. W. (1995). Canary 1.2 User's Manual. Ithaca: Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology.
Conner, W. E. (1999). `Un chant d'appel
amoureux': Acoustic communication in moths. J. Exp.
Biol. 202,1711
-1723.
Craig, J. L. (1984). Wing noises, wing slots and aggression in New Zealand honey eaters (Aves: Meliphagidae). New Zealand J. Zool. 11,195 -200.
Dalton, R. (2002). Caught on Camera. Nature 418,721 -722.[CrossRef][Medline]
Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray.
Desuttergrandcolas, L. (1995). Functional forewing morphology and stridulation in crickets (Orthoptera, Grylloidea). J. Zool. 236,243 -252.
Dial, K. P. (1992). Activity patterns of the wing muscles of the pigeon (Columba livia) during different modes of flight. J. Exp. Zool. 262,357 -373.
Dumortier, B. (1963). Insect sound production. In Acoustic Behavior of Animals (ed. Busnel), pp.277 -345. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Haskell, P. T. (1974). Sound production. In The Physiology of Insecta (ed. M. Rockstein), pp.354 -410. New York: Academic Press.
Lasiewski, R. C. and Lasiewski, R. J. (1967). Physiological responses to the Blue-throated and Rivoli's hummingbirds. Auk 84,34 -48.
Lowe, P. R. (1942). The anatomy of Gould's Manakin (Manacus vitellinus) in relation to its display, with special reference to an undescribed pterylar tract and the attachment of the flexor carpi ulnaris and flexor sublimis digitorem muscles to the secondary wing-feathers. Ibis 6,50 -82.
Manson-Barr, P. and Pye, J. D. (1985). Mechanical sounds. In A Dictionary of Birds (ed. B. Campbell and E. Lack), pp. 342-344. Staffordshire: Poyser.
McBurney, R. S. (1989). Drumming and roadside drumming counts. In Ruffed Grouse (ed. S. Atwater and J. Schnell), pp. 176-188, 193, 206-209. Harrisburg: Stackpole Books.
Miller, A. H. (1925). The boom-flight of the Pacific Nighthawk. Condor 27,141 -143.
Miller, S. J. and Inouye, D. W. (1983). Roles of the wing-whistle in the territorial behaviour of male Broad-tailed Hummingbirds (Selasphorus platycercus). Anim. Behav. 31,689 -700.
Miskelly, C. M. (1990). Aerial displaying and flying ability of Chatham Island Snipe Coenocorypha pussila and New Zealand Snipe C. aucklandica. Emu 90, 28-32.
Patek, S. N. (2001). Spiny lobsters stick and slip to make sound. Nature 411,153 -154.[CrossRef][Medline]
Payne, R. B. (1973). Wingflap dialects in the Flappet Lark Mirafra rufocinnamomea. Ibis 115,270 -274.
Pettingill, O. S., Jr (1936). The American Woodcock Philohela minor (Gmelin). Mem. Boston Soc. Nat. Hist. 9,1 -391.
Prum, R. O. (1990). Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of display behavior in the Neotropical manakins (Aves: Pipridae). Ethol. 84,202 -231.
Prum, R. O. (1998). Sexual selection and the evolution of mechanical sound production in manakins (Aves: Pipridae). Anim. Behav. 55,977 -994.[CrossRef][Medline]
Raikow, R. J. (1985). Locomotor system. In Form and Function in Birds, vol.3 , pp. 57-147. London: Academic Press.
Robbins, M. B. (1983). The display repertoire of the Band-tailed Manakin (Pipra fasciicauda). Wils. Bull. 95,321 -504.
Rome, L. C., Syme, D. A., Hollingworth, S. and Lindstedt, S.
L. (1996). The whistle and the rattle: the design of sound
producing muscles. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
93,8095
-8100.
Saldanha, C. J., Schultz, J. D., London, S. E. and Schlinger, B. A. (2000). Telencephalic aromatase but not a song circuit in a sub-oscine passerine, the Golden-collared Manakin (Manacus vitellinus). Brain Behav. Evol. 56, 29-37.[CrossRef][Medline]
Sankaran, R. (1996). Territorial displays of the Bengal Florican. J. Bombay Nat. Hist. Soc. 93,167 -177.
Schultz, J. D. and Schlinger, B. A. (1999).
Widespread accumulation of 3H-testosterone in the spinal cord of a wild bird
with an elaborate courtship display. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA 96,10428
-10432.
Schultz, J. D., Schlinger, B. A., Hertel, F. and Bauch, M. (2001). Adaptations for rapid and forceful contraction in wing muscles of the male golden-collared manakin: Sex and species comparisons. J. Comp. Physiol. A 187,677 -684.[Medline]
Schwartz, P. and Snow, D. W. (1978). Display and related behavior of the Wire-tailed Manakin. Living Bird 17,51 -78.
Sick, H. (1959). Die Balz der Schmuckvogel (Pipridae). J. Ornithol. 100,269 -302.
Skutch, A. F. (1949). Life history of the Yellow-thighed Manakin. Auk 66, 1-24.
Skutch, A. F. (1969). Life Histories of Central American Birds III. Pacific Coast Avifauna 35, 97-163.
Snow, D. W. (1962). A field study of the Black and White Manakin, Manacus manacus, in Trinidad. Zoologica 47,65 -104.
Snow, D. W. (1963). The display of the Orange-headed Manakin. Condor 65, 44-48.
Snow, D. W. (1977). The display of the Scarlet-horned Manakin Pipra cornuta. Bull. Br. Orn. Club 97,23 -27.
Tello, J. G. (2001). Lekking behavior of the Round-tailed Manakin. Condor 103,298 -321.
Tuck, L. M. (1972). The snipes: a study of the genus Capella. Can. Wildl. Ser. Mono. 5.
Versluis, M., Schmitz, B., von der Heydt, A. and Lohse, D.
(2000). How snapping shrimp snap: through cavitating bubbles.
Science 289,2114
-2117.
Winkler, H. and Short, L. L. (1978). A comparative analysis of acoustical signals in pied woodpeckers (Aves, Picoides). Bull. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist. 160, 1-109.
Yack, J. E., Otero, L. D., Dawson, J. W., Surlykke, A. and
Fullard, J. H. (2000). Sound production and hearing in the
blue cracker butterfly Hamadryas feronia (Lepidoptera, Nymphalidae)
from Venezuela. J. Exp. Biol.
203,3689
-3702.
Related articles in JEB: