Limits to human performance: elevated risks on high mountains
1 Department of Zoology, Box 351800, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195-1800, USA and
2 29 Landsdowne Rise, Worcester, WR3 8LN, UK
*e-mail: hueyrb{at}u.washington.edu
Accepted July 2, 2001
![]() |
Summary |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Key words: hypoxia, altitude, mountaineering, death, temperature, barometric pressure, Homo sapiens.
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
...for we must remember that even the highest of mountains is capable of severity, a severity so awful and so fatal that the wiser sort of men do well to think and tremble even on the threshold of their high adventure.G. H. Leigh-Mallory (1922, p. 279).
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Climbing above 8000m is a severe test of human performance (West, 2000). The attendant drama has long been riveting to a vast popular audience and has also helped inspire a wealth of research on high-altitude medical physiology (Houston, 1998; Hultgren, 1997; Richalet, 1999; Roach et al., 1999). Remarkably, few studies have quantified risk (Town, 1986; Pollard and Clarke, 1988; Huey and Eguskitza, 2000), even though the reality of Himalayan mountaineering tragedies is apparent (Krakauer, 1997). Many questions need to be addressed. How risky is reaching the summit of an 8000m peak? Does supplemental oxygen enhance survival (Huey and Eguskitza, 2000)? Does the size of a team influence success rate or death rate? How do factors such as height, steepness, difficulty and weather affect success and mortality? Does risk of death from different causes (e.g. avalanche versus illness) change with altitude (Pollard and Clarke, 1988)? Answers to such questions can ultimately provide insight into what limits human performance in an extreme environment and also provide mountaineers with information relevant to risk-taking.
We can now begin to address such questions. The necessary historical data derive from extensive interviews obtained over many years by E. Hawley and by X. Eguskitza supplemented by published records in the mountaineering literature (e.g. American Alpine Journal, Alpine Journal, Himalayan Journal and High and scores of books). To search for patterns in these data, we adopt analytical techniques that were developed for evolutionary studies of Natural Selection on organisms (Endler, 1986; Schluter and Nychka, 1994). For example, we attempt to determine whether rates of success or of death correlate with factors such as mountain height, use of supplemental oxygen or team size. Our analyses are still preliminary but nevertheless provocative.
We begin by reviewing some of the physical and physiological challenges associated with high-altitude mountaineering. Then, we will use available mountaineering data to test two hypotheses: (i) that the success rate in reaching the summit is inversely related to a mountains height and (ii) that death rates are directly related to mountain height.
![]() |
In the Death Zone |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|
Low temperatures and strong winds compound physiological stresses at extreme altitude (although these are sometimes ameliorated by intense radiation loads). Some remarkable temperature data have recently become available for Everest (M. Hawley, personal communication). For example, temperatures measured on the South Col (approximately 960m below the summit) during the spring 1999 climbing season averaged -11.6°C and regularly dropped at night to -20°C (Fig.2). On the summit, temperatures should be colder, by roughly 6°C on average (Peixoto and Oort, 1992) (p. 141). [Note: P. Athans recorded an air temperature at the summit of -28°C at 10:00h on 5 May 1999 (C. Corfield, personal communication), when the temperature at the South Col was probably around -10°C.] Although not especially cold by polar standards (Peixoto and Oort, 1992), the combination of low temperatures, strong winds (Peixoto and Oort, 1992), hypoxia and dehydration can readily induce hypothermia and frostbite (Houston, 1998; West, 1998). Indeed, with an air temperature of only -10°C and a wind speed of only 50kmh-1 (a mere breeze by Himalayan standards), the estimated wind-chill temperature would drop to approximately -25°C (Quayle and Steadman, 1998), which is approaching the temperature at which exposed human flesh freezes (-35°C). [This calculation ignores the complex changes in convective heat loss (Steadman, 1979) that are associated with the decline in air density at altitude (C. Houston, personal communication).]
|
![]() |
Success and death versus altitude |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Testing these hypotheses is easier said than done, in part because the probabilities of success and of death must be influenced by many other factors, not just altitude. For example, peaks (and even different routes on a given peak) obviously differ in difficulty, danger and even with other conditions (see below). A full analysis of the probabilities of success and death should consider the influence of as many such factors as possible; but of necessity here, we begin with a single-factor (altitude) approach.
First, do death rates of trekkers increase with altitude? Indeed, death rate increases with altitude (Burtscher et al., 1995): 2.3 deaths per 106 days of exposure for England and Wales (Avery et al., 1990), 5.7 for Austria (Burtscher et al., 1995), compared with 10.6 for Nepal (Shlim and Gallie, 1992; Shlim and Houston, 1989). Even so, as Burtscher et al. (Burtscher et al., 1995) cautioned, trekking death rates will be influenced by many factors other than by altitude (medical services are less accessible in Nepal, for instance), so these data are suggestive at best. Nevertheless, they are consistent with the hypothesis that risk increases with altitude.
Next, does success rate decrease with the altitude of a peak and does death rate increase? Success and death rates have been compiled for very few peaks so far, but we have been able to obtain reasonably complete data for five peaks [Table1; the two highest peaks in the world (K2, Everest) versus three lower-altitude peaks (Rainier, Foraker and Denali, all in North America)]. These five mountains all involve major altitudinal gains from base camp to summit (Table1), but differ strikingly in success (i.e. reaching the summit) and mortality (Table1).
|
Overall death rate increases with altitude (Table1; r=0.84, P=0.04) and is significantly higher for the two 8K peaks (P=0.04). Similarly, death rate during descent from the summit increases with a summits altitude (Table1; r=0.83, P=0.04) and is higher for the two 8K peaks (P=0.04). Note that the probability of death during descent from either of the two highest Himalayan peaks is more than an order of magnitude higher than that from Rainier, Foraker or Denali (Table1). Moreover, death rates during descent from the summits of all other 8K Himalayan peaks [range 0.5% (Cho Oyu) to 7.4% (Annapurna), data from Eguskitza (Sale and Cleare, 1999)] are also very high relative to the three lower peaks in Table1, further reinforcing the evidence in Table1 that big peaks are more dangerous. [Note: the permit fee for climbing scales positively with a peaks altitude (Table1), and this pattern holds even within the Himalayas (Huey, 2001). Obviously climbers are willing to pay a premium for attempting big peaks, despite low success and high risk.]
These comparisons are simplistic and based on only a few peaks. Moreover, these five mountains differ in many ways (not just altitude) that might influence or dominate success and death rates. For example, helicopter rescue, which could reduce death rates, is an option on the three North American peaks (weather permitting), but only at low altitude on Everest and not at all on K2. Moreover, the experience and skill of the climbers that attempt these peaks are undoubtedly far from homogeneous (see below). Even so, the available data support the expectation that climbing high peaks is in a different league from climbing low ones.
![]() |
Concluding caveats |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
First, mountaineering data will be intrinsically noisy. Weather and snow conditions can change rapidly, as can a mountaineers physical state, adding variability to any real pattern.
Second, such analyses are inherently retrospective, and so inferring causation is additionally risky (Hill, 1965). The standard way to evaluate causation is, of course, to do an experiment. That is not an option here: imagine how K2 mountaineers would respond to being randomly assigned into groups using versus not using supplemental oxygen! Accordingly, we must interpret any observed pattern with caution (Huey and Eguskitza, 2000) using standard epidemiological criteria (Hill, 1965; Susser, 1991) to evaluate causality.
Third, we must assume that climbers on different mountains are comparable in experience, skill and willingness to accept risk, but this is certainly not the case (climbers on Rainier are less skilled and experienced than are climbers on K2). Climbers also differ in ability to acclimatize to altitude, concurrent illness and tolerance of physiological adversity. Such individual differences, which unfortunately are difficult to ascertain retrospectively, can potentially confound or mask comparisons across peaks (Huey and Eguskitza, 2000). In the present examples, however, the induced biases should be in the opposite direction to the observed patterns: thus, for example, climbers on 8K peaks have relatively low success rates and relatively high death rates despite their greater skill and experience.
Finally, the most readily accessible data are for individual climbers (Table1); yet, because climbers are usually in groups (and sometimes die in groups), data for individuals are not entirely independent, as assumed by traditional statistical methods. An alternative is to use a climbing team as the unit of analysis (Huey and Eguskitza, 2000). Unfortunately, such team-based data are not yet available for most peaks.
Despite these and other concerns, our work so far suggests that patterns can indeed be detected. Mountaineering data do appear to give strong signals (Huey and Eguskitza, 2000), despite their inherent noisiness. Because the observed patterns are consistent with physiologically based expectations, we gain some reassurance that cause and effect may well be involved. Nevertheless, more rigorous studies will be necessary to validate causality. At the present time, a cautious skepticism is appropriate.
![]() |
Acknowledgments |
---|
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Avery, J. G., Harper, P. and Ackryod, S. (1990). Do we pay too dearly for our sport and leisure activities? Public Health 104, 417423.[Medline]
Burtscher, M., Philadelphy, M., Nachbauer, W. and Likar, R. (1995). The risk of death to trekkers and hikers in the mountains. J. Am. Med. Ass. 273, 460.
Christensen, E. D. and Lacsina, E. Q. (1999). Mountaineering fatalities on Mount Rainier, Washington, 19771997. Am. J. Forensic Med. Path. 20, 173179.
Edwards, H. T. (1936). Lactic acid in rest and work at high altitude. Am. J. Physiol. 116, 367375.
Eguskitza, X. (1996). Appendices IIII (unpaginated). In J. Curran, K2: The Story of the Savage Mountain. London: Hodder & Stoughton.
Endler, J. A. (1986). Natural Selection in the Wild. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Hill, A. B. (1965). The environment and disease: association or causation? Proc. R. Soc. Med. 58, 295300.[Medline]
Hornbein, T. F., Townes, B. D., Schoene, R. B., Sutton, J. R. and Houston, C. S. (1989). The cost to the central nervous system of climbing to extremely high altitude. New Eng. J. Med. 321, 17141719.[Abstract]
Houston, C. S. (1998). Going Higher: the Story of Man and Altitude, fourth edition. Boston, MA: Little, Brown & Co.
Huey, R. B. (2001). The economics of adventure: on the high cost of Himalayan climbing permits. Alpine J. (in press).
Huey, R. B. and Eguskitza, X. (2000). Supplemental oxygen and death rates on Everest and K2. J. Am. Med. Ass. 284, 181.
Hultgren, H. N. (1997). High Altitude Medicine. Stanford, CA: Hultgren Publications.
Krakauer, J. (1997). Into Thin Air: a Personal Account of the Mount Everest Disaster. New York, NY: Villard.
Leigh-Mallory, G. L. (1922). The reconnaissance of the mountain. In Mount Everest: the Reconnaissance, 1921 (ed. C. K. Howard-Bury), pp. 214215. London: Edward Arnold & Co.
Messner, R. (1979). Everest: Expedition to the Ultimate. London: Kaye & Ward.
Peacock, A. J. and Jones, P. L. (1997). Gas exchange at extreme altitude: results from the British 40th Anniversary Everest Expedition. Eur. Resp. J. 10, 14391444.
Peixoto, J. P. and Oort, A. H. (1992). Physics of Climate. New York: American Institute of Physics.
Pollard, A. and Clarke, C. (1988). Deaths during mountaineering at extreme altitude. Lancet 1, 1277.
Poretti, G. (1999). 150 years of measuring the Himalayas. Leica Reports 41, 1317.
Pugh, L. G. C. E., Gill, M. B., Lahiri, S., Milledge, J. S., Ward, M. P. and West, J. B. (1964). Maximal exercise at extreme altitudes on Mount Everest. J. Appl. Physiol. 19, 431440.
Quayle, R. G. and Steadman, R. G. (1998). The Steadman wind chill: an improvement over present scales. Weather Forecasting 13, 11871193.
Richalet, J.-P. (1999). Médecine de lAlpinisme. Paris: Masson.
Roach, R. C., Wagner, P. D. and Hacket, P. H. (1999). Hypoxia: into the Next Millennium. New York: Kluwer Academic.
Sale, R. and Cleare, J. (1999). Climbing the Worlds 14 Highest Mountains: the History of the 8,000meter Peaks. Seattle, WA: The Mountaineers Press.
Schluter, D. and Nychka, D. (1994). Exploring fitness surfaces. Am. Nat. 143, 597616.
Shlim, D. R. and Gallie, J. (1992). The causes of death among trekkers in Nepal. Int. J. Sports Med. 13, S74S76.[Medline]
Shlim, D. R. and Houston, R. (1989). Helicopter rescues and deaths among trekkers in Nepal. J. Am. Med. Ass. 261, 10171019.[Abstract]
Steadman, R. G. (1979). The assessment of sultriness. Part II. Effects of wind, extra radiation and barometric pressure on apparent temperature. J. Appl. Meteorol. 18, 874885.
Susser, M. (1991). What is a cause and how do we know one? A grammar for pragmatic epidemiology. Am. J. Epidemiol. 133, 635648.[Abstract]
Town, J. (1986). Death and the art of database maintenance. Mountain 110, 4245.
West, J. B. (1986a). Highest inhabitants in the world. Nature 324, 517.[Medline]
West, J. B. (1986b). Lactate during exercise at extreme altitude. Fedn. Proc. 45, 29532957.
West, J. B. (1996). Prediction of barometric pressures at high altitude with the use of model atmospheres. J. Appl. Physiol. 81, 18501854.
West, J. B. (1998). High Life: a History of High-Altitude Physiology and Medicine. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
West, J. B. (1999). Barometric pressures on Mt. Everest: new data and physiological significance. J. Appl. Physiol. 86, 10621066.
West, J. B. (2000). Human limits for hypoxia. The physiological challenge of climbing Mt Everest. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 899, 1527.
West, J. B., Boyer, S. J., Graber, D. J., Hackett, P. H., Maret, K. H., Milledge, J. S., Peters, R. M., Jr, Pizzo, C. J., Samaja, M., Sarnquist, F. H., Schoene, R. B. and Winslow, R. M. (1983). Maximal exercise at extreme altitudes on Mount Everest. J. Appl. Physiol. 55, R688R698.