The effects of the light environment on prey choice by zebra finches
Centre for Behavioural Biology and Ecology of Vision Group, School of Biological Sciences, Woodland Road, Bristol, BS8 1UG, UK
*Author for correspondence (e-mail: i.cuthill{at}bris.ac.uk)
Accepted April 19, 2001
![]() |
Summary |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Key words: foraging, behaviour, light environment, colour vision, ultraviolet vision, zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata.
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Avian ultraviolet vision is of particular interest since humans are blind to these wavelengths, and research indicates that ultraviolet vision is extensively utilised in birds (see references above). However, there are no strong reasons for assuming that it is an especially important region of the avian visual spectrum, and other regions of the visual spectrum may be equally or more important depending on the visual task in question (but see Andersson, 1999; Bennett and Cuthill, 1994). Hence, this study investigates the effects of blocking different regions of the avian visual spectrum on foraging choice in the zebra finch. In one experiment, we examine the effects of removing ultraviolet wavelengths on preferences for different seed types and test the effect of decreasing conspicuousness through the use of non-edible distracters. In a second experiment, we manipulate the light environment across the entire spectrum, including the ultraviolet wavelengths, to determine how blocking different spectral regions affects the foraging choices of zebra finches.
![]() |
Materials and Methods |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Prey items in the foraging trials consisted of red and white millet seeds, with or without similarly sized stone distracters (termed D+ and D-, respectively). The distracters were also red and white. Prior to the trials, the reflectance spectra of both seed and stone types were measured. Measurements were made, normal to the plane of the sample, using a Zeiss MCS 500 spectrophotometer, with samples illuminated at 45° to normal by a Zeiss CLX xenon light source. Five measurements were taken from random locations within each sample, and each area measured was a circle of 2mm diameter. Spectra were measured against a 99% SpectralonTM reflectance standard, at intervals of 0.81nm from 300 to 700nm (Church et al., 1998a).
The apparatus consisted of a central area and four arms, each leading to a foraging arena within a moveable cage (Fig.1). The cages were lined with aluminium foil overlaid with frosted ultraviolet-transmitting acrylic sheet to maximise the quantal flux within each cage and ensure that the birds were photopically adapted (as in Bennett et al., 1996).
|
|
On experimental days, food was removed from all birds before the lights came on (08:00h; approximately 3 or 4h of food deprivation for the early and the late trial, respectively). During trials, four companion birds were placed in the central area (with ad libitum commercial seed and water), and one test bird was placed in each of the four arms. The companion birds were introduced since zebra finches are gregarious (Zann, 1996) and were relatively inactive in isolation during habituation trials. The mesh section in each arm was offset; the test birds could view the companion birds but not the other test birds. The experiment had a repeated-measures design. Test birds, stimulus cages, filters and the presence or absence of stone distracters were arranged according to a hyper-Graeco-Latin square design (Fisher and Yates, 1963), with four treatments (UV+ or UV-, and D+ or D-). All test birds had one trial on each of four experimental days (two early and two late trials).
Each trial lasted 40min; for the first 10min, test birds were prevented from entering the foraging arena by a vertically mounted UV+ filter to allow habituation to the light environment; the filter was then removed, and the birds were allowed to forage for 30min because, during habituation trials, this was enough time for them to eat a significant proportion of the seeds without eating all the seeds (which would prevent any determination of preference). The foraging arenas were hardboard trays containing a random mixture of 80 seeds (40 red millet seeds and 40 white millet seeds) either with or without 80 similarly sized stone distracters (again 40 red and 40 white).
At the end of each trial, birds were returned to their home cages, and the number of each type of seed eaten was recorded. As these data were not normally distributed, they were BoxCox-transformed (Aitken et al., 1989) prior to analysis of variance (ANOVA). Where significant univariate results are reported, multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) on the number of white and red seeds, as joint dependent variables, was also significant (based on Wilks ) (see Chatfield and Collins, 1995).
Experiment 2: the effect of blocking different regions of the avian-visible spectrum
The protocol was similar to experiment 1, using the same birds. However, there were four filter types (Fig.4A), each specifically chosen to block regions of the spectrum corresponding approximately to the regions of sensitivity of each single cone type in the zebra finch (Bowmaker et al., 1997). The filters, henceforth referred to as ultraviolet-blocking (UV-), short-wavelength-blocking (SW-), medium-wavelength-blocking (MW-) and long-wavelength-blocking (LW-), consisted of Lee Filter 226 and Rosco SupergelTM filters 14, 339 and 73, respectively. These filters were balanced as closely as possible for total quantal flux, again to control for potential effects due to variation in brightness (ratio 1.00:0.91:0.89:0.81 for UV-, SW-, MW- and LW- filters, respectively). As in experiment 1, birds were acclimated to the different light environments by manipulating the lights in their holding room using the filters described above (for a minimum of 6h per filter) and habituated to the experimental apparatus (for approximately 9h). Trials differed in two important aspects from those in experiment 1. First, distracters were not used because they had no significant effect in the first experiment. Second, the duration of trials was increased to 1h and the number of each seed type was doubled to 80 (160 seeds in total). As before, the number of each seed type eaten (BoxCox-transformed) was the dependent variable in MANOVA and ANOVA.
|
To calculate the position in zebra finch colour space of the two seed types and the background, we first calculated the photon catches of the four receptor types by multiplying the irradiance spectrum of the Truelite tubes by the reflectance spectrum of each object and the effective spectral sensitivity of the cones. Following the notation of Vorobyev et al. (Vorobyev et al., 1998), the quantal catch of the ith cone, Qi, is given by Equation 1:
| (1) |
where Ri() is the spectral sensitivity of cone i, S(
) is the reflectance spectrum of the object being viewed, and I(
) is the irradiance spectrum of the illuminant. Integration was over the avian-visible spectrum (wavelengths,
, from 300 to 750nm; this is a conservative estimate, as effective spectral sensitivity drops rapidly below 320nm and above 700nm). The reflectance spectra of the seeds and background (Fig.2B) and the irradiance spectrum of the Truelite tubes (Fig.2A) were measured as described previously. The spectral sensitivity, at a given wavelength, of each cone type was calculated as
![]() | (2) |
where Pi() is the normalised absorptance of the visual pigment in the ith cone, Di(
) the transmission of the oil droplet in that cone, and M(
) the transmission of the other optical media (cornea, aqueous humour, lens, vitreous humour) in the light path. Data on the absorbance of zebra finch visual pigments were taken from Bowmaker et al. (Bowmaker et al., 1997), using the visual pigment templates provided rather than the raw data. Following Hart (Hart, 1998), oil droplets were modelled as perfect cut-off filters (see also Bowmaker et al., 1997), based on the cut-off wavelengths (
cut) provided (Bowmaker et al., 1997). The oil droplet
cut values correspond well to the equivalent data for four closely related Estrildid species measured by Hart et al. (Hart et al., 2000). We used the mean transmittance of these species optical media in our model (wavelengths of 50% transmission of 316318nm) (Hart et al., 2000), as these data were not available for the zebra finch.
The photon catches, Qi, define the position of each object in zebra finch colour space. We used the Euclidean distance between any two objects in this colour space as our measure of colour difference. In these calculations we have to make an assumption about colour constancy and the adapted state of the eye when viewing an object (Vorobyev and Osorio, 1998). With perfect colour constancy, the colour would not change with the illuminant, which can be modelled by assuming that, whatever the illuminating spectrum, a grey object is located at the centre of the colour space (equal stimulation of all cones) (Vorobyev and Osorio, 1998; Vorobyev et al., 1998; Fleishman and Endler, 2000). Our filters were chosen to remove the entire waveband to which each cone is sensitive, so we assume that colour constancy will fail in the conditions of our experiment (indeed, previous experiments indicated that this is true for the ultraviolet filter) (Bennett et al., 1996; Hunt et al., 1997). To model this, we assume that the finch cones are always adapted to a grey background illuminated by an unfiltered Truelite tube, rather than adapting independently to the illumination in each filter treatment. This assumption is tested indirectly, because if zebra finches have perfect colour constancy in our experiment, then we expect to see no effect of treatment. We acknowledge that the actual perceived colour difference may not correspond directly to the metric we have used, but the latter provides a first approximation in the absence of psychophysical data (Thompson et al., 1992). We note here that we present our results in terms of a colour space centred on the achromatic locus (grey/white), as this is conventional for such representations (e.g. Burkhardt, 1989; Goldsmith, 1990; Fleishman and Endler, 2000). Analysis with the brown hardboard experimental background as the adapting background produces directly equivalent results in terms of distances between objects in colour space.
As blocking light of a particular waveband may affect both perceived brightness and hue, we calculated two further metrics. The first, which we call single cone brightness, is the summed output of the four single cones. The weighted sum of cone outputs would correlate with perceived brightness in humans (Endler, 1990) and, in the absence of information on the relative weightings in zebra finches, we take the simple sum to approximate the avian equivalent. However, birds have another retinal cell type, the double cones, which are thought by some, on account of their abundance and broad spectral sensitivity, to constitute a secondary, or even the primary, achromatic (brightness) channel in birds (see Cuthill et al., 2000; Vorobyev and Osorio, 1998). Thus, we calculated a further metric, double cone brightness, based on the calculated quantal catch of the double cones (again based on data in Hart et al., 2000; Bowmaker et al., 1997). Vorobyev et al. (Vorobyev et al., 1998) based their double-cone sensitivity calculations on that of the primary member of the cone-pair, thus ignoring the partial short-wavelength-sensitivity of the secondary member. We calculated double-cone photon catch assuming either that only the primary member contributes (as in Vorobyev et al., 1998), or that both members contribute in proportion to their cross-sectional area. In practice, for the objects and backgrounds in our experiment, this made a trivial difference, so we present only the data based on primary-member-only calculations here. All colour metrics are scaled such that the maximum possible distance between two points (e.g. black and white, or two saturated primary colours) is 1.
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Foraging behaviour
Removing the ultraviolet component of the light environment had no significant effect on the numbers of each seed type eaten (red seeds: F1,7=1.01, P=0.348; white seeds: F1,7=4.52, P=0.071), despite a non-significant trend to eat more white seeds under UV+ (Fig.3). There was also no effect of stone distracters (red seeds: F1,7=0.02, P=0.893; white seeds: F1,7=0.02, P=0.884) and no interaction between filter type and distracter type (red seeds: F1,7=2.48, P=0.159; white seeds: F1,7=0.27, P=0.621).
|
Foraging behaviour
The number of each type of seed eaten varies significantly when different regions of the spectrum are blocked (MANOVA; F6,40=2.36, P=0.048; Fig.5). Although the number of red seeds eaten does not vary significantly between filters (red seeds; F3,21=0.75, P=0.565), the data follow a similar pattern to the distances between seeds and background in colour space (Fig.4B) or single-cone brightness (Fig.4C), although the removal of ultraviolet wavelengths has somewhat less effect than predicted. The number of white seeds eaten varies significantly across the light environments (white seeds; F3,21=4.63, P=0.012), increasing as red seeds become more difficult to distinguish (i.e. as the distance in colour space between the seeds and the background decreases). This is particularly apparent when long wavelengths are blocked (LW-). It should also be noted that these variations do not match the predicted effect of waveband removal on a putative double-cone brightness mechanism, where MW- should be most deleterious.
|
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
In experiment 1, distracters did not affect foraging choices, suggesting that the birds did not find the foraging task more difficult when presented with additional similarly coloured, but non-edible, items. This raises the question of whether the distracters actually affected the crypticity of the seeds as perceived by the birds. Hence, it would be interesting to assess the effects on foraging behaviour of backgrounds that would have a greater effect on textural cues, for example those made entirely of distracters. These results also showed no effect of the absence of ultraviolet wavelengths on foraging choice. This is consistent with the low level of ultraviolet reflectance from the seeds and background (and the corresponding distances in colour space; Fig.2B, Fig.4B). However, in these experiments, it was not possible to determine the exact time the first seed was eaten nor its type. Such data would have been useful since previous research into foraging rates suggests that ultraviolet cues may be important in initiating searches, especially for cryptic prey (Browman et al., 1994; Church et al., 1998b).
In experiment 2, the nature of the ambient light spectra affects prey choice. The removal of long wavelengths had the greatest effect on choice, increasing the number of white seeds eaten (Fig.5) whilst tending to reduce the number of red seeds eaten (though not significantly). This is consistent with the predicted differences in perceived colour between the seeds and the background because the distance in colour space between the red seeds and the background under this filter is negligible (Fig.4B). The result is also consistent with an analogous experiment on female mating preferences in zebra finches, in which blocking the long-wavelength component of male plumage reflectance produced the greatest reduction in attractiveness (Hunt et al., 2001). The data presented here and elsewhere (Hunt et al., 2001) do not allow us to say whether the effect of blocking long wavelengths is through alterations in perceived hue or brightness, or both, because the predicted effect of treatments is similar (Fig.4B,C). Likewise, effects on hue perception are not necessarily the result of changes in tetrachromatic colour space, but from altered two- or three-way comparison of cone types. The tetrachromatic goldfish Carassius auratus becomes trichromatic under some lighting conditions (Neumeyer and Arnold, 1989). However, the fact that blocking the long waveband has a greater effect than blocking the medium waveband suggests that the single cones are more important than the double cones for the foraging task in this experiment. Double cones, whilst possessing the same visual pigment as the long-wavelength single cones, have a less densely pigmented oil droplet, so have a shorter wavelength of effective peak sensitivity than the long-wavelength single cones (Bowmaker et al., 1997; Hart et al., 2000). This is why removal of the medium (500600nm) waveband would affect double-cone function more than that of the long-wavelength single cones.
These results also indicate a lack of perfect colour constancy since, if this were the case, there should not have been variations in seed choice under the different light environments. When explicitly modelling separate adaptation to each filtered light environment (results not shown), there were no shifts in the separation of objects in colour space between treatments. This was expected as the model assumes perfect independent adaptation of each photoreceptor to the background, even when there is minimal light (see Vorobyev and Osorio, 1998; Vorobeyev et al., 1998). Despite research into avian spectral sensitivity (Emmerton and Delius, 1980; Prescott and Wathes, 1999), there is little research into the extent of avian colour constancy because of the difficulties associated with its determination. However, it has been suggested that avian colour constancy should be good, as a result of the small overlap in spectral sensitivity between the single cone types (Vorobyev et al., 1998). The goldfish, another tetrachromat, exhibits colour constancy (Neumeyer et al., 1997), so it would be surprising if birds did not. The key question that requires exploration if visually directed behaviours are to be fully understood is, under which condition is constancy expected to break down? It seems to under the large manipulations of the light environment in this experiment, but future research should also examine the extent of these effects under a range of natural light environments. This should be investigated under ambient light spectra and with natural backgrounds, particularly those with a relatively high ultraviolet reflectance (such as sand or snow) versus those without (such as clay soil).
Since the appearance of objects can alter significantly in different light environments, there are obvious implications for visually orientated behaviours (Endler, 1993; Endler and Théry, 1996). We speculate that if prey choice also varies between more natural light environments, selection pressure on prey and plant species that rely on animal dispersal could also vary in different environments. Since zebra finch vision is also similar to that of other passerines (Bowmaker et al., 1997; Cuthill et al., 2000), variations in the light environment will potentially affect many bird species. Zebra finches tend to inhabit drier areas with seeding grasses (Zann, 1996), which will experience a fairly limited range of light environments compared with some habitats (although there will obviously still be large fluctuations as a result of climate changes and time of day) (Endler, 1993). There could be even stronger implications for prey of other avian species, particularly of tree-foraging guilds, since woodlands possess a far greater range of light environments (Endler, 1993).
Changes in the spectral quality of light also have the potential to affect the behaviour of visually guided predators in ways that have direct consequences for prey population dynamics. Our results only reflect simple choices between equally available seed types; however, it may be fruitful in future to consider the effects of frequency-dependent availability on seed choice (see Church et al., 2001), since these will have greater implications for plant population stability and dynamics (Greenwood, 1985; Pacala and Crawley, 1992). Overall our results suggest that future work incorporating visually orientated behaviours needs to take into account the potential effects of any variation in the light environment.
![]() |
Acknowledgments |
---|
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Aitken, M., Anderson, D., Francis, B. and Hinde, J. (1989). Statistical Modelling in GLIM. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Andersson, S. (1999). Morphology of ultraviolet reflectance in a whistling thrush: implications for the study of structural colour signalling in birds. J. Avian Biol. 30, 193204.
Bennett, A. T. D. and Cuthill, I. C. (1994). Ultraviolet vision in birds: What is its function? Vision Res. 34, 14711478.[Medline]
Bennett, A. T. D., Cuthill, I. C., Partridge, J. C. and Maier, E. J. (1996). Ultraviolet vision and mate choice in zebra finches. Nature 380, 433435.
Bowmaker, J. K., Heath, L. A., Wilkie, S. E. and Hunt, D. M. (1997). Visual pigments and oil droplets from six classes of photoreceptor in the retinas of birds. Vision Res. 16, 21832194.
Browman, H. I., Novales-Flamarique, I. and Hawryshyn, C. W. (1994). Ultraviolet photoreception contributes to prey search behaviour in two species of zooplanktonivorous fishes. J. Exp. Biol. 186, 187198.
Burkhardt, D. (1989). Ultraviolet vision: a birds eye view of feathers. J. Comp. Physiol. A 164, 787796.
Chatfield, C., and Collins, A. J. (1995). Introduction to Multivariate Analysis. London: Chapman and Hall.
Church, S. C., Bennett, A. T. D., Cuthill, I. C., Hunt, S., Hart, N. S. and Partridge, J. C. (1998a). Does Lepidopteran larval crypsis extend into the ultraviolet? Naturwissenschaften 85, 189192.
Church, S. C., Bennett, A. T. D., Cuthill, I. C. and Partridge, J. P. (1998b). Ultraviolet cues affect the foraging behaviour of bluetits. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 15091514.
Church, S. C., Merrison, A. S. L. and Chamberlain, T. M. M. (2001). Avian ultraviolet vision and frequency-dependent seed preferences. J. Exp. Biol. 204, 24912498.
Cuthill, I. C., Partridge, J. C. and Bennett, A. T. D. (1999). Avian UV vision and sexual selection. In Animal Signals (ed. Y. Espmark, T. Admundsen and G. Rosenqvist), pp. 6182. Trondheim: The Royal Norwegian Society of Sciences and Letters; The Foundation Tapir Publishers.
Cuthill, I. C., Partridge, J. C., Bennett, A. T. D., Church, S. C., Hart, N. S. and Hunt, S. (2000). Ultraviolet vision in birds. Adv. Stud. Behav. 29, 159214.
Emmerton, J. and Delius, J. D. (1980). Wavelength discrimination in visible and ultraviolet spectrum by pigeons. J. Comp. Physiol. A 141, 4752.
Endler, J. A. (1990). On the measurement and classification of colour in studies of animal colour patterns. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 41, 315352.
Endler, J. A. (1993). The color of light in forests and its implications. Ecol. Monogr. 63, 127.
Endler, J. A. and Théry, M. (1996). Interacting effects of lek placement, display behavior, ambient light and color patterns in three neotropical forest-dwelling birds. Am. Nat. 148, 421452.
Fisher, R. A. and Yates, F. (1963). Statistical Tables for Biological, Agricultural and Medical Research. 6th edition. New York: Hafner.
Fleishman, L. J. and Endler, J. A. (2000). Some comments on visual perception and the use of video playback in animal behavior studies. Acta Ethologica 3, 1527.
Goldsmith, T. H. (1990). Optimization, constraint and history in the evolution of eyes. Q. Rev. Biol. 65, 281322.[Medline]
Greenwood, J. (1985). Frequency-dependent selection by seed predators. Oikos 44, 195210.
Hart, N. S. (1998). Avian Photoreceptors. PhD thesis, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK.
Hart, N. S., Partridge, J. C., Bennett, A. T. D. and Cuthill, I. C. (2000). Visual pigments, cone oil droplets and ocular media in four species of estrildid finch. J. Comp. Physiol. A 186, 681694.[Medline]
Hart, N. S., Partridge, J. C. and Cuthill, I. C. (1998). Visual pigments, oil droplets and cone photoreceptor distribution in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). J. Exp. Biol. 201, 14331446.
Hunt, S., Cuthill, I. C., Bennett, A. T. D., Church, S. C. and Partridge, J. C. (2001). Is the ultraviolet waveband a special communication channel in avian mate choice? J. Exp. Biol. 204, 24992507.
Hunt, S., Cuthill, I. C., Swaddle, J. P. and Bennett, A. T. D. (1997). Ultraviolet vision and band-colour preferences in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata. Anim. Behav. 54, 13831392.[Medline]
Jacobs, G. H. (1992). Ultraviolet vision in vertebrates. Am. Zool. 32, 544554.
Koivula, M. and Viitala, J. (1999). Rough-legged Buzzards use vole scent marks to assess hunting areas. J. Avian Biol. 30, 329332.
Neumeyer, C. (1992). Tetrachromatic color vision in goldfish: evidence from color mixture experiments. J. Comp. Physiol. A 171, 639649.
Neumeyer, C. and Arnold, K. (1989). Tetrachromatic color vision in the goldfish becomes trichromatic under white adaptation light of moderate intensity. Vision Res. 29, 17191727.[Medline]
Neumeyer, C., Doerr, S. and Fritsch, J. (1997). Color constancy in goldfish: Effects of surround size and lightness. Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. 38, 4198.
Osorio, D., Jones, C. D. and Vorobyev, M. (1999a). Accurate memory for colour but not pattern contrast in chicks. Curr. Biol. 9, 199202.[Medline]
Osorio, D., Vorobyev, M. and Jones, C. D. (1999b). Colour vision of domestic chicks. J. Exp. Biol. 202, 29512959.
Pacala, S. and Crawley, M. (1992). Herbivores and plant diversity. Am. Nat. 140, 243260.
Prescott, N. B. and Wathes, C. M. (1999). Spectral sensitivity of the domestic fowl (Gallus g. domesticus). Br. Poultry Sci. 40, 332339.[Medline]
Siitari, H., Honkavaara, J. and Viitala, J. (1999). Ultraviolet reflectance of berries attracts foraging birds. A laboratory study with redwings (Turdus iliacus) and bilberries (Vaccinium myritillus). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266, 21252129.
Silberglied, R. (1979). Communication in the Ultraviolet. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 10, 373398.
Thompson, E., Palacios, A. and Varela, F. (1992). Ways of coloring: Comparative color vision as a case study for cognitive science. Behav. Brain Sci. 15, 174.
Viitala, J., Korpimäki, E., Palokangas, P. and Koivula, M. (1995). Attraction of kestrels to vole scent marks visible in ultraviolet light. Nature 373, 424425.
Vorobyev, M. and Osorio, D. (1998). Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 351358.[Medline]
Vorobyev, M., Osorio, D., Bennett, A. T. D., Marshall, N. J. and Cuthill, I. C. (1998). Tetrachromacy, oil droplets and bird plumage colours. J. Comp. Physiol. A 183, 621633.[Medline]
Walls, G. L. (1963). The Vertebrate Eye and its Adaptive Radiation. New York: Hafner.
Willson, M. F. and Whelan, C. J. (1989). Ultraviolet reflectance of fruits of vertebrate-dispersed plants. Oikos 55, 341348.
Zann, R. A. (1996). The Zebra Finch: A Synthesis of Field and Laboratory Studies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.