Phase reversal of vibratory signals in honeycomb may assist dancing honeybees to attract their audience
1 Biozentrum, Zoologie II, Am Hubland, 97074 Würzburg, Germany,
2 Université de Tours, Institut de Recherches sur la Biology and
3 School of Biological Science, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia
*e-mail: tautz{at}biozentrum.uni-wuerzburg.de
Accepted August 9, 2001
![]() |
Summary |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Key words: waggle dance, honeycomb vibration, recruitment, communication, honeybee, Apis mellifera carnica.
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
An obvious requirement, if the dancer is to be successful in communicating with dance-followers, is that they are in attendance. This is complicated by the fact that only a small proportion of the worker bees in the hive are potential dance-followers (Thom et al., 2000) and these may not be close when the dancer commences her performance.
There is evidence that several strategies are employed to bring dancers and dance-followers together. Certain regions of the hive, for example, are selected each day by the first foragers that return to the hive and are marked, probably by pollen clinging to the dancing foragers and the extrusion from glands on the dancers tarsi of chemicals that are taken up in the surface layers of the wax comb (Tautz and Lindauer, 1997). Such markers could provide cues about the location of future dances for waiting dance-followers. Also, not just any area of the comb is chosen to be a dance floor. In natural hives, the areas of the combs on which the bees most frequently dance are near the entrance to the hive; these cells are empty and uncapped and are close to the lower and freely suspended margin of the comb (Seeley and Morse, 1976). In commercial hives, comb templates are supplied to bees in wooden frames. In these frames, the combs are attached around all four sides so that the lower edges of the combs are fixed. It has been found that bees will modify such combs by removing the wax along the lower edge of the part of the comb on which they perform their dances, perhaps in an attempt to achieve a situation closer to that prevailing in natural hives (Sandeman et al., 1996).
Many of the observations that have been made on bee dancing have been carried out using an observation hive in which two frames containing combs, mounted one above the other, can be observed from either side through transparent walls. The bees do not seem to be unduly disturbed, provided that lighting conditions are subdued, and continue to perform their dances and various activities associated with the maintenance of brood and the defence of the hive. In a natural hive, however, there is far less light than in observation hives, raising the question of how a forager can pass on information in the dark that is so important for the nourishment of the colony. Even in well-lit observation hives, it is doubtful whether potential recruits that are more than a few body lengths from the dancing bee are able to see her, given the crowded conditions that often prevail on the combs.
Behavioural measurements on bees in both illuminated and non-illuminated observation hives have shown that potential dance-followers signal that they have detected a dancer in their vicinity by turning their heads towards the dancer and then moving close and following her through the turns of the dance (Tautz and Rohrseitz, 1998; Rohrseitz and Tautz, 1999). Many of these dance-followers head towards the dancer from some distance away, providing strong evidence for the existence of some signal or signals other than vision or direct tactile stimulation that advertises the presence of a dancer.
One of the possibilities is that the low-frequency (approximately 200300 Hz) signals that bees emit from their thoraces during the waggle stage of the dance (Esch, 1961; Wenner, 1962; Michelsen et al., 1986; Spangler, 1991) are transmitted laterally across the comb to nest mates that can perceive these through proprioceptors in their legs (Sandeman et al., 1996). This hypothesis is attractive because of the resonant properties of open cell combs (particularly along an unsupported edge), the sensitivity of some leg proprioceptors to low-frequency displacement and the greater efficiency with which forager bees recruit nest mates on empty, in comparison with filled and sealed, combs (Tautz, 1996).
The amplitude of the comb displacements produced by dancing bees, however, is small and barely exceeds the threshold of the receptors that could be responsible for their detection (Sandeman et al., 1996; Nieh and Tautz, 2001). However, the receptors from which recordings have been made may not be those that the bees use to detect the displacements, or the receptors may be much more sensitive in intact animals. Alternatively, the bees may have some sophisticated way of extracting the low-frequency displacement from the general noise.
A further possibility is that, as a result of an inherent elasticity in the wax of the comb, the cell walls do not act as a rigid structure in which the displacement moves at infinite velocity across the surface, but that some delay, or phase lag, is introduced between the displacement of one cell wall and that of the next. A bee straddling a cell would, therefore, be subjected to a signal in which, for a very short period of the cycle, pairs of its legs would be moving in opposite directions with respect to one another, effectively doubling the size of the input signal. Such an event would not have been detected in earlier measurements that employed a single laser to measure the amplitude first at the displaced cell and then at various distances along the cell row.
This study was initiated to explore the above possibility by employing two identical laser systems that could simultaneously monitor the displacement of the cell walls along a row of open comb cells. The result was the discovery of a remarkable and unexpected phenomenon: the lateral displacement of the comb walls proceeds across the comb at high velocity but, at a particular distance from the signal source and at a frequency within the range of that emitted from the bees thorax during the waggle dance, the phase of the lateral displacements of the cell walls is abruptly reversed across a single cell.
This discovery led us to re-examine a large amount of the collected behavioural data on the distances and directions from which dance-followers will come to follow dancers. These show that the region from which most dance-followers are attracted coincides with the predicted location of the phase-reversal phenomenon.
![]() |
Materials and methods |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
The experimental arrangement was the same as that used in the previous study described in detail by Sandeman et al. (Sandeman et al., 1996). Briefly, the frame was supported horizontally, and the cell wall of one cell was displaced in the plane of the comb face using a bilayered piezo-electric transducer (BM/ML 60/40/300; Piezomechanik, München). Two identical laser-Doppler vibrometers (Ometron VS 100) were used, and their beams were directed separately through mirrors onto small reflective strips placed on the rims of the cells. One laser beam was focused on the wall of the stimulated cell (the reference laser), while the other (the difference laser) was focused on the walls of cells at different distances from the stimulated cell. Many regions of the comb were tested, but the data reported here come from three horizontal rows of cells in the centre of the comb (Fig. 1).
|
The above values were taken into account in designing a method of imposing vibrations on the comb that would approximate the natural situation. There are, nevertheless, several unknowns and constraints that make a precise replication of the natural situation difficult. First, the bee exerts a force on the comb through more than one leg for each lateral excursion, and we limited ourselves to a single probe. Second, the amplitude of the cyclical excursion of the cell wall in the natural situation is calculated to be approximately 0.5 µm. The accurate production and measurement of cyclical displacements in the region of 0.5 µm were close to the limits of the apparatus we used.
Our compromise was to apply the probe of the piezo-electric transducer laterally at the centre of the rim of a cell wall in the direction of the horizontal long axis of the comb and in the plane of the comb face. The probe was advanced laterally against the rim with a force of 1 mN, which lies between the extremes (0.52 mN) calculated for the low-frequency oscillation of the bee (Storm, 1998). The force of the probe against the comb was constant, and the vibratory stimulus was superimposed on it. The stimulated cell was chosen to lie half-way along the long axis of the comb, and measurements of the displacement of individual cell walls were taken on both sides of the stimulated cell. Hence, we could observe cells that were pulled behind when the probe moved against the cell wall and also those that were pushed ahead when the probe moved against the cell wall.
The frequency of the vibratory stimulus was held at 270 Hz because our previous study showed this frequency to be optimal for transmission along the face of the comb and in the centre of the range of frequencies that the bees produce during dancing. The amplitude of the stimulus was maintained at 2 µm, peak to peak, and was measured directly with one of the laser vibrometers. While larger than the cyclical amplitude estimated for vibration by a single leg, it is still close to the range that could be produced by a bee with more than one leg gripping the wall. Also, the impedance of honeycomb for vibration at 200300 Hz is between 20 and 30 N s m1 (Rohrseitz, 1998), which is approximately 34 times lower than for the waggle frequency. Hence, the cyclical amplitude for the 200300 Hz signals would be expected to be proportionately larger than 0.5 µm. Large-amplitude displacements (20 µm) produce the same result as the small-amplitude displacements, suggesting that, over this range, the amplitude of the imposed vibrations is not a critical factor in the production of the phase reversal.
Behavioural observations
Behavioural observations on the dancers and the dance-followers were made in the same way as reported by Tautz and Rohrseitz (Tautz and Rohrseitz, 1998) but are described here in some detail where the results have relevance to the laser measurements of the comb displacement. The colony on which the observations were made contained approximately 4000 bees (Apis mellifera carnica Pollm.) and occupied a two-frame observation hive located in a laboratory from which the bees were unable to view the sky but from which they could gain access to the outside along a short tunnel. A high-speed video camera (HSV 400) was focused on the dance floor, an area of the comb nearest the entrance at the lower edge of the hive where the bees normally danced. An area of 75 mmx60 mm, or approximately 180 cells, was included in the field of view. Video recordings were made at 200 frames s1 under daylight conditions or with additional visible stroboscopic light, which improved the quality of the images. A CCD camera was used with infrared illumination provided by infrared light-emitting diodes (=875 nm) to obtain pictures under dark conditions.
The cells of the comb that constituted the dance floor varied from being open, empty cells (referred to as open cells) located near the lower freed edge of the comb to the capped brood cells (referred to as sealed cells) located closer to the centre of the comb. Videotaping was undertaken over 10 days during warm and sunny weather. During this period, the lower comb (with the dance floor) needed to be exchanged three times because the bees filled the cells with honey. Videotaping was resumed on the day following comb exchange.
Twenty marked forager bees were trained to a feeder (on non-scented 2 mol l1 sugar solution) 1 km away from the hive. The dances of these foragers were videotaped.
We needed to determine the moment a dance-follower became interested in a dancer and her location relative to the dancer at that moment. We achieved this by inspecting the video recording until we found a dance-follower that followed the dancer for at least one waggle phase and one return phase of the dance. We then reversed the video recording so that we could track the image of the dance-follower, frame by frame, backwards in time, until we detected her first overt reaction, namely a turn of her head towards the dancer. Because the head turn always preceded movement of the dance-follower towards the dancer, we took this as the criterion of her becoming aware of a dancer in the vicinity.
The distance between the dance-follower and the dancer was measured as the shortest line between any part of the bodies (head, thorax or abdomen) of the dance-follower and dancer. We also determined the position of the head of the dance-follower within a system of polar coordinates centred on the thorax of the dancer. In this system, which was divided into 30° sectors, 0° was directly ahead of, and 180° directly behind, the dancer.
In all, 471 dance-followers in 132 dances were analysed. Of these, 40 dance-followers were videotaped under infrared conditions. The density of the bees (0.8±0.1 bees cm2) within the field of view of the camera was the same in all cases for the different lighting conditions and dance floors.
Details of the statistics used in the analysis of the behaviour are given by Tautz and Rohrseitz (Tautz and Rohrseitz, 1998).
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|
|
Focusing the lasers on walls 2 and 3 reveals the abrupt phase shift in the displacement velocity that is introduced at this distance from the stimulated cell. The distortions of the original signal introduced at cell 2 are carried over into the displacement velocities of wall 3, and additional peaks appear in some excursions. A comparison between the displacement velocities of walls 3 and 4, however, shows that these two are again nearly in phase with one another. Thus, the 180° phase shift introduced across walls 2 and 3 is now preserved across walls 3 and 4. No further phase change was detected, although measurements were taken up to 63 cells from where the displacement was applied.
The phase reversal that occurred across walls 2 and 3 along the primary row, horizontal to the stimulus, was also found to be represented between cell walls of secondary and tertiary horizontal cell rows, which lie lateral to the primary row (cells 2b and 3b and cells 2c and 3c, Fig. 1). The same type of distortions that appeared in the primary horizontal row were also found in the displacement velocities of the cell walls in both secondary and tertiary rows (Fig. 4A,B). When screening an entire comb, seven cell widths was the greatest distance between the stimulus and a phase-reversal cell.
|
|
We have re-examined the video recordings taken by Tautz and Rohrseitz (Tautz and Rohrseitz, 1998) in relation to the phase-reversal phenomenon. The question we pose here is whether the responses of the dance-follower bees observed in video recordings would in any way suggest the possible presence of a phase-reversal signal in the comb vibrations that may occur under the natural conditions of a bee dance. We explore an answer to the question in terms of the effectiveness of the dancers in attracting followers from a particular distance and direction across open cells. We chose these variables because they are both tied to a location, as is the phase-reversal phenomenon itself, relative to the signal source (i.e. the dancing bee).
To estimate the direction and distance from which dance-followers were attracted to the dancer, we counted the number of individuals attracted in each sector over a range of distances from the dancer. Each set of columns in Fig. 6AF therefore represents the numbers of dance-followers in a single sector that came in from a particular distance. The distance, in cell widths, across which the dance-followers ran to attend the dance is given on the abscissa. The region over which the phase reversal would be expected to occur is shown by the line below the abscissa stretching from one to seven cell widths away from the dancer. Statistical differences between the columns were tested using a 2-test in which P<0.05 is taken to be significant. Testing each sector against every other shows that all combinations are significantly different except sector 2 against sector 3 and sector 4 against sector 5.
|
Dance-followers in sector 6 (almost directly behind the dancer) do not fit the pattern. Most come from very close (one cell away), and the number attracted falls away with distance. Our suggestion is that these dance-followers could be responding to a different signal (see Discussion).
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Our demonstration of the phase-reversal phenomenon leads to the question of whether this phenomenon could play a role in alerting dance-followers to the presence of a dancer with whom they are in neither bodily nor visual contact. In the context of this question, it is important to take into account that the phase-reversal phenomenon was observed under conditions that only approximated those prevailing in the hive. The comb in which it was demonstrated consisted entirely of open cells, the oscillatory input displacements of the walls were continuous, and somewhat larger than, those calculated for a dancing bee and were applied precisely at a right angle to a single point on the rim of the cell wall, and the comb was not loaded with bees.
Nevertheless, if the phase-reversal phenomenon occurs under natural conditions and is being detected by dance-followers, we could expect to measure improved recruitment from those distances and directions where we would expect the phase reversal to occur. This is precisely what we find. Superimposing the histograms of Fig. 6 into a single graph (Fig. 7) shows that the region where we would expect phase reversal to occur corresponds with the distance from which most dance-followers (except from sector 6) run in to attend the dance. Sectors 2 and 3 have a second optimum around 47 cell widths from the dancer, suggesting a particularly effective spread of the vibratory signal along these radii in open cell combs.
|
Why waggle?
To an observer, the waggle dance is one of the most striking behaviours that take place on the surface of the comb. A large literature has accumulated on the function of the waggle dance in terms of transmitting the distance and direction of the food source to the followers. Why should bees spend the effort in waggling during the straight run of their dance? Two obvious reasons come to mind why this could be useful: (i) that the waggle run distinguishes that part of the dance from the return run and (ii) that the waggle identifies foragers. In the context of the present paper, we would like to propose two additional possibilities.
The first of these is that the waggle may enhance the spread of the 270 Hz vibratory signal by improving the transmission of the signal from the vibrating thorax through the legs and to the walls of the comb (Tautz et al., 1996). The 270 Hz signal is emitted by the bee during that part of the waggle in which the bee is fully laterally displaced (Esch, 1961). The mass of the bee gains momentum through its lateral motion, and the bee must brace itself by gripping the walls of the cells; otherwise, it would fall over. Storms (Storm, 1998) elegant calculations of the forces exerted by a bee on the rims of the cell walls show that it is both pulling and pushing with its mesothoracic legs, but mainly pulling with its prothoracic and metathoracic legs. The maximum application of force occurs at the peak of the lateral excursion of the waggle. As a result, the bee has optimised the conditions for injecting the vibratory signal in its thorax into the comb through an increase in its effective mass and a stiffened mechanical link (the actively extending or flexing legs) with the walls of the comb. A similar strategy is seen in bees that transmit the 370 Hz piping signals across the comb by pressing their vibrating thoraces down against the rims of the cells (Esch, 1964; Pratt, 1996). If the above is true, then recruitment of followers to the dancing bee would be expected to be better in those areas of the comb that lie to the side and slightly in front of the waggling bee because it is these areas that Storm (Storm, 1998) has shown receive the greatest force applied to them during the dance. The behavioural observations confirm that this is the case.
The second possibility concerns the difficulty of using the comb as a transmitting system given that it is loaded with a large number of active bees and probably constitutes a very noisy environment. A frequently used strategy in such systems is to pulse-modulate a tone. Instead of a continuous signal to which receptors quickly adapt, or do not recognise against the noise in the first place, the tone is represented as a flashing signal that is readily perceived because of the added temporal properties (Ronacher et al., 2000). The 270 Hz signal is indeed pulsed and in phase with the waggle frequency. As such, it would be more easily distinguished against the general background noise in the comb, which is relatively continuous (Nieh and Tautz, 2001).
Alternative hypotheses
The mechanical properties of honeycomb are complex, and the response of such a system to imposed vibration is certainly affected by many variables. We have shown that, under conditions that approximate the natural situation, the vibratory signal that spreads along a row of cells undergoes an abrupt phase shift after it has been transmitted across a number of cells. We also show that, in the natural situation, the majority of the remote dance-followers (i.e. those not in immediate contact with the dancer) come from the region where we would predict a phase reversal to occur. A conclusion that can be drawn from this is that the two are causally related, a hypothesis that could be tested in the natural situation by, for example, jamming the natural signals with imposed comb vibrations. If there is no causal relationship between the phase reversal and the recruitment of the remote bees, then one is faced with explaining why the majority of the remote bees come from this particular distance and direction.
Vibratory comb signals emanating from a dancing bee are certainly not the only clue used by dancers to advertise their presence. A dancing bee raises her temperature (Stabentheiner and Hagmüller, 1991), which would make her immediately identifiable to her nest mates that are close to her but not to those that are remote. The increased temperature of the dancer would also increase the rate at which volatile substances accumulated at the food site would diffuse from her body into her neighbourhood. Such odours would be spread relatively broadly by diffusion and by air currents generated by the vibrating wings. However, neither a temperature increase nor the diffusion of odours would explain the distribution of the recruited bees that has been recorded. Indeed, odour diffusion would be expected to be concentrated behind the animals because air currents from the vibrating wings are directed there and not to the side and in front of the dancers. Air currents themselves are a possibly potent source of information but, again, particularly for followers that are situated behind the dancer (Michelsen, 1999).
Dancers will use every means they have at their disposal to attract dance-followers and may employ different strategies in different combinations to suit different situations. Our demonstration of the phase-reversal phenomenon in the vibratory signals in the comb suggests that substratum vibrations cannot be ruled out as a communication pathway simply because their amplitude appears to be too small.
![]() |
Acknowledgments |
---|
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Esch, H. (1961). Über die Schallerzeugung beim Werbetanz der Honigbiene. Z. Vergl. Physiol. 45, 111.
Esch, H. (1964). Beiträge zum Problem der Entfernungsweisung im Schwänzeltanz der Honigbiene. Z. Vergl. Physiol. 48, 534546.
Michelsen, A. (1999). The dance language of honeybees: recent findings and problems. In The Design of Animal Communication (ed. M. D. Hauser and M. Konishi), pp. 111113. Cambridge, MA, London: MIT Press.
Michelsen, A., Kirchner, W. H. and Lindauer, M. (1986). Sound and vibrational signals in the dance language of the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 18, 207212.
Nieh, J. and Tautz, J. (2001). Behaviour-locked signal analysis reveals weak 200300 Hz comb vibrations during the honeybee waggle dance. J. Exp. Biol. 203, 15731579.
Pratt, S. (1996). Worker piping associated with foraging in queenright colonies. Apidologie 27, 1320.
Rohrseitz, K. (1998). Biophysikalische und ethologische Aspekte der Tanzkommunikation der Honigbienen (Apis mellifera carnica Pollm.). PhD thesis, Universität Würzburg.
Rohrseitz, K. and Tautz, J. (1999). Honey bee dance communication: waggle run direction coded in antennal contacts? J. Comp. Physiol. 184, 463470.
Ronacher, B., Krahe, R. and Hennig, R. M. (2000). Effects of signal duration on the recognition of masked communication signals by the grasshopper Chorthippus biguttulus. J. Comp. Physiol. 186, 10651072.[Medline]
Sandeman, D. C., Tautz, J. and Lindauer, M. (1996). Transmission of vibration across honeycombs and its detection by bee leg receptors. J. Exp. Biol. 199, 25852594.
Seeley, T. and Morse, R. (1976). The nest of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.). Insectes Sociaux 23, 495512.
Spangler, H. G. (1991). Do honey bees encode distance information into the wing vibrations of the waggle dance? J. Insect Behav. 4, 1520.
Stabentheiner, A. and Hagmüller, K. (1991). Sweet food means hot dancing in honeybees. Naturwissenschaften 78, 471473.
Storm, J. (1998). The dynamics and flow field of the wagging dancing honeybee. PhD thesis, Odense University, Denmark.
Tautz, J. (1996). Honeybee waggle dance: recruitment success depends on the dance floor. J. Exp. Biol. 199, 13751381.
Tautz, J. and Lindauer, M. (1997). Honeybees establish and maintain specific sites on the comb for their waggle dances. J. Comp. Physiol. 180, 537539.
Tautz, J. and Rohrseitz, K. (1998). What attracts honeybees to a waggle dancer? J. Comp. Physiol. 183, 661667.
Tautz, J., Rohrseitz, K. and Sandeman, D. C. (1996). One-strided waggle dance in bees. Nature 382, 32.[Medline]
Thom, C., Seeley, T. and Tautz, J. (2000). A scientific note on the dynamics of labor devoted to nectar foraging in a honey bee colony: number of foragers versus individual foraging activity. Apidologie 31, 737738.
von Frisch, K. (1967). The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees (translated by L. E. Chadwick). Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
Wenner, A. M. (1962). Sound production during the waggle dance of the honey bee. Anim. Behav. 10, 7995.