Mechanism of tongue protraction in microhylid frogs
1 Physiology and Functional Morphology Group, Department of Biological
Sciences, Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff, AZ 86011-5640,
USA
2 Department of Biology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL 33124-0421,
USA
* Author for correspondence (e-mail: jjm{at}dana.ucc.nau.edu)
Accepted 23 September 2003
![]() |
Summary |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Key words: tongue protraction, m. genioglossus dorsoventralis, muscular hydrostat, Microhylidae, frog, Phrynomantis bifasciatus, feeding, prey capture
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Mechanical pulling is the primitive mechanism of tongue protraction in
frogs. This mechanism is found in all archaeobatrachians
(Nishikawa, 1997;
Nishikawa and Cannatella,
1991
; Nishikawa and Roth,
1991
), as well as in some meso- and neobatrachian frogs
(Deban and Nishikawa, 1992
;
O'Reilly and Nishikawa, 1995
;
Nishikawa, 2000
). Mechanical
pulling is characterized by a modestly protrusible tongue (less than 70% of
jaw length), which is protracted by contraction of the m. genioglossus. As the
m. genioglossus shortens, the tongue bunches at the front of the jaws and is
extended beyond the mandibular symphysis. Unless a prey item is extremely
close to a mechanical-pulling frog, the modest extent of tongue protraction
requires forward body movement (lunging) in concert with tongue protraction in
order for the tongue to come in contact with the prey
(Deban and Nishikawa, 1992
;
Valdez and Nishikawa,
1996
).
Inertial elongation is a derived mechanism of tongue protraction among
anurans and has evolved at least seven times independently
(Nishikawa, 2000). With
respect to sheer numbers of species, it is probably the most prevalent
mechanism of tongue protraction among living frogs (Nishikawa,
1997
,
2000
; Nishikawa and Gans,
1995). Inertial elongation is accomplished by tightly coordinated tongue and
jaw movements that flip the tongue over the mandibles and extend it well
beyond its resting length (Nishikawa,
1992
,
2000
;
Nishikawa and Gans, 1996
). The
tongue is protracted very fast and is delivered to the target with minimal
body movement, allowing the animal to remain relatively cryptic during feeding
bouts (Gray, 1997
). However,
possibly because the tongue movements are ballistic, the frogs are apparently
unable to change the trajectory during protraction and have little or no
ability to laterally aim the tongue independent of the head.
The third known mechanism of tongue protraction in anurans is hydrostatic
elongation. In contrast to the other two mechanisms of tongue protraction,
muscular hydrostatic elongation has been described only once, in the African
pig-nosed frog Hemisus sudanensis
(Ritter and Nishikawa, 1995;
Nishikawa et al., 1999
).
Although this mechanism is similar to inertial elongation in that the tongue
is rotated forward over the mandibular symphysis, it differs in that the
tongue can be aimed laterally and in elevation relative to the head
(Ritter and Nishikawa, 1995
).
Initially, it was suggested that Hemisus used a hydraulic protraction
mechanism (Ritter and Nishikawa,
1995
). However, a more detailed study of its tongue morphology
suggests a muscular hydrostatic mechanism. In Hemisus, the tongue has
a separate compartment of dorsoventrally arranged muscle fibers that are
surrounded by connective tissue. The connective tissue fibers are arranged to
restrict lateral expansion, so that shortening of the dorsoventral fibers
results in elongation of the tongue
(Nishikawa et al., 1999
).
Molecular and morphological data suggest that Hemisus is closely
related to frogs of the family Microhylidae
(Wu, 1994;
Emerson et al., 2000
;
Haas, 2003
). It is therefore
interesting that observations of feeding behavior from representatives of
several genera within this family indicate that they have a similar tongue
protraction mechanism to that seen in Hemisus
(Meyers et al., 1996
;
Monroy and Nishikawa, 2000
).
When capturing prey, microhylids are capable of aiming the tongue
independently of head movements. The tongue can be protracted to either the
left or right side, allowing them to effectively capture prey positioned over
90° from the midline of the head. Thus, their behavior suggests that
microhylids may have a muscular hydrostatic tongue protraction mechanism
similar to that seen in Hemisus. However, morphological work by
Emerson (1976
) suggested
another possible explanation. She noted that microhylids possess accessory
slips of the m. intermandibularis that may be involved in bending the mandible
at the mentomeckelian joint during tongue protraction, allowing the tongue to
deviate from a straight trajectory.
Here, we examine the mechanism of tongue protraction in microhylids using high-speed videography and muscle denervation techniques. Although we examined 17 species of microhylids, our studies focused on one species in particular: the South African snake-necked frog Phrynomantis bifasciatus. The goals of this study were: (1) to determine whether tongue aiming is widespread among microhylids and (2) to elucidate the mechanism(s) that microhylids use to aim their tongue independently of the lower jaw. Our results indicate that all microhylids are capable of lateral tongue movements and that they share a muscular hydrostatic mechanism of tongue protraction with Hemisus.
![]() |
Materials and methods |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
High-speed videography
Animals were videotaped with a high-speed video camera (model 660; Display
Technologies) with synchronized stroboscopic illumination and a Panasonic
AG-6300 video cassette recorder. Feeding sequences were filmed at either 120
fields s1 or 180 fields s1 at room
temperature (2024°C). The frogs were placed on a damp paper towel
facing the camera. The camera was elevated above the animal and tilted to an
angle of 45°. During prey capture, the lower jaw rotates downward to an
angle of approximately 45°, resulting in a perpendicular view of the
tongue during protraction. Animals were filmed in several planes, including
horizontal and directly overhead, but we found that filming at 45°
provided the greatest detail about tongue trajectory and angle.
To initiate tongue aiming, forceps were used to place individual termites around the head. Placement of the termites ranged from directly in front of the animal to positions near the feet and on the forearms. Although the frogs often turn their heads during prey capture, by positioning the termites on the lateral parts of the body we were able to elicit extremes of tongue aiming.
Quantification of aiming
Tongue angle was measured as the maximum angle between the midline of the
head (determined by drawing a line down the long axis of the body so that it
was placed midway between the eyes and the nares) and the midline of the
protracted tongue. One potential problem with measuring tongue angles in this
manner is that the angle of the tongue relative to the head will be distorted
as the camera angle deviates from perpendicular. To address this concern, we
measured known angles drawn on paper with the camera placed at 20°,
45° and 90° (dorsal view). When the camera is placed directly
overhead, the angles measured are identical with those drawn on the paper.
When the camera angle is at 20°, there is up to a 5° increase in our
measurement and at 45° there is a 10° increase. Most of this occurs
when the tongue is 45° to either side of the midline. As the tongue angle
approaches either the midline or 90°, the actual angle and the measured
angle differ by no more than 2°. When Phrynomantis reaches peak
tongue protraction, the lower jaw is at an angle of approximately 45° to
the horizon, and with the camera positioned at 45° to the horizon we get a
perpendicular view of the tongue. Thus, the magnitude of the error in these
measurements is always less than 10° and in most cases much less than
10°.
In order to examine tongue aiming ability, we divided the normal aiming range of Phrynomantis bifasciatus into five quadrants relative to the head (see Fig. 1). Since, in addition to aiming the tongue laterally, the head can also be rotated in the direction of the prey, all measurements were taken relative to the midline of the head. This prevents us from confounding the effects of tongue aiming relative to the lower jaw and head turning. Tongue angle was measured for at least three feeding attempts in each quadrant. The maximum range of 105° was the greatest angle observed in Phrynomantis. Left and right sides were denoted as negative and positive, respectively, to avoid confusion of tongue trajectory after muscle denervation (i.e. after right unilateral genioglossus denervation the tongue is protracted to +45°, even when attempting to capture prey placed at 45°).
|
Morphology
Two preserved individuals of P. bifasciatus were sectioned to
examine the arrangement of tongue and hyobranchial muscles. Histological
sections of the lower jaw and tongue were made in the transverse and sagittal
planes. Specimens were decalcified, embedded in paraffin and sectioned
serially at 10 µm. Sections were stained using Milligan's Trichrome stain
(Humason, 1979). The presence
of m. intermandibularis accessory slips was confirmed through gross dissection
of two individuals. Photos taken of the dissection with a Nikon Coolpix camera
were used to make drawings of the m. intermandibularis musculature. To
determine which branches of the hypoglossal nerve innervated the muscles of
the lower jaw, we cleared and stained the peripheral nerves of one individual
(Fig. 2; Nishikawa, 1987
).
|
Muscle denervation
Animals that received muscle denervation treatments were first anesthetized
in 7% ethanol. For most anurans, tricaine methanosulfonate (MS222) is
sufficient to anesthetize the animal within 30 min. However, using MS222 it
took several hours to fully anesthetize Phrynomantis bifasciatus.
Using 7% ethanol, the animals could be anesthetized in approximately 30 min.
We determined that the animals were under surgical anesthesia when tactile
stimulation elicited no response.
Once anesthetized, frogs were placed on the stage of a dissecting microscope. Except for the lower jaw, the entire animal was covered with damp paper towels to prevent dehydration. A small incision was made in the skin above where the nerve branch of interest was located. The surrounding musculature and blood vessels were teased apart to expose the nerve. To minimize damage to individual muscle fibers, muscles were always teased apart parallel to their long axis. A 12 mm section of the nerve was removed and then the incision was closed using Nexaband veterinary surgical adhesive. Post-surgery feeding attempts were made as soon as the animals recovered from anesthesia. To confirm the surgeries before regeneration of the nerve, animals were euthanized within three weeks of the surgery date. Animals were over-anesthetized in 10% ethanol and then fixed in 10% formalin and stored in 70% ethanol.
Two different surgical treatments were performed: denervation of the m. genioglossus and denervation of the m. intermandibularis. The ramus mandibularis of the trigeminal nerve innervates both the m. intermandibularis (posteriorly) and the m. submentalis (anteriorly); it was transected distal to the innervation of the m. intermandibularis. In the second treatment, the hypoglossal nerve branch innervating the m. genioglossus dorsoventralis and longitudinalis was unilaterally transected. Although this nerve also innervates the m. hyoglossus and m. geniohyoideus, we transected the nerve distal to these branches (Fig. 2).
In both treatments, the animals were anesthetized, an incision was made in
the skin of the buccal floor, and the intermandibular muscles were teased
apart to expose the underlying nerves. The difference between treatments is
that in the m. genioglossus treatment, the m. geniohyoideus was also teased
apart to expose the ramus hypoglossus of the hypoglossal nerve. Because there
was no effect of m. intermandibularis denervation on feeding kinematics or
tongue aiming, it is unlikely that the observed effects of m. genioglossus
denervation were due to treatment alone. Previous studies of other species
support this conclusion, as they revealed no effect of this procedure in sham
surgeries in which the hypoglossal nerve was exposed but not transected
(Deban and Nishikawa, 1992;
Ritter and Nishikawa,
1995
).
Statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was accomplished using Statview software on a G3 Power
Macintosh computer. We performed an analysis of variance (ANOVA) to determine
the effect of unilateral genioglossus denervation on normal tongue
protraction. This analysis allowed us to compare feeding attempts before and
after denervation when the prey is presented directly in front of the animal.
In addition, in one individual we were able to record post-denervation feeding
sequences in all of the aiming quadrants. For this individual, a
t-test was used to examine the effect on aiming in each quadrant
after m. genioglossus denervation.
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|
|
As in other frogs, the m. genioglossus longitudinalis (ggl) originates at the mandibular symphysis. It is attached to the mandible by a thin band of fascia and runs postero-dorsally along the ventral surface of the mucosa. However, unlike in other frogs, it does not spread extensively into the tongue pad or interdigitate with fibers of the m. hyoglossus. In addition to the ggl, there is an m. genioglossus dorsoventralis (ggdv) that lies ventral to the ggl and shares a similar origin and connective tissue attachment. Near its origin, the ggdv fibers run posteriorly into the tongue tip, but as they proceed posteriorly they turn and are directed ventrad, inserting into a thick layer of surrounding connective tissue. Finally, there is an additional intrinsic muscle, the m. genioglossus transversalis (ggt), which originates laterally from connective tissue and runs transversely beneath the ggdv (Fig. 4).
Tongue aiming
The ability to aim the tongue is well developed in Phrynomantis,
although there appears to be individual variation in the propensity to aim the
tongue. A comparison of aiming ability in the different quadrants revealed
that all individuals were able to aim in each quadrant. In addition to the
tongue being protruded at an angle, the head may also be moved towards the
prey item when the tongue is aimed laterally about the head. This strategy
increases the range over which the frogs are able to capture prey and
effectively allows them to capture prey items off the forearms. Although the
animals are able to capture prey items over a wide range, qualitative
observations of prey capture suggest that prey capture success decreases at
extreme angles.
Although individuals of Phrynomantis exhibit little variation in aiming ability (Table 1), there are clear species differences within the Microhylidae. All microhylid species examined were able to aim the tongue independent of head movements. However, the maximum tongue angles measured for the species varied greatly, with the most extreme angles (>100°) measured in Phrynomantis and Dermatonotus muelleri (Mullers' termite frog; Table 2; Fig. 5). The most extreme tongue angles were measured from animals showing the greatest propensity to aim. Hence, we may not have elicited maximum aiming attempts in some species. Unlike microhylids, Rana pipiens and Bufo woodhousii exhibited little or no ability to aim the tongue (Table 2). Although R. pipiens was able to aim the tongue up to 5°, this is substantially less than in all the microhylids examined.
|
|
|
Effect of m. intermandibularis denervation
To determine whether mandibular bending plays a role in tongue aiming, we
transected the ramus of the trigeminal nerve innervating the m.
intermandibularis. After bilateral transection of the m. intermandibularis,
the tongue is still able to protract normally, and prey capture sequences are
qualitatively similar to sequences recorded before denervation (compare
Fig. 6A and
Fig. 6B). In addition, feeding
attempts after surgery revealed no deficits in the ability to aim to the
extreme angles seen before surgery. One individual consistently aimed more
than 58°, with one attempt at 90°. Although we did not test for
differences, it appeared that prey capture success rate did not differ from
normal feeding sequences.
|
Effect of m. genioglossus denervation
Unilateral denervation of the m. genioglossus in P. bifasciatus
resulted in deficits in the ability to both aim the tongue and to capture
prey. When the nerve branch innervating the m. genioglossus dorsoventralis and
m. genioglossus longitudinalis is transected on the right side, the tongue is
flipped out of the mouth and bends towards the denervated side (right side)
upon protraction. Even when the frog attempts to feed on prey placed directly
in front of it (0°), its tongue deviates towards the denervated side
(ANOVA, F=262.2, P=0.0001; compare
Fig. 6A and
Fig. 6C). Prior to denervation,
the tongue deviated only 3.4±2.4°. However, after denervation, the
tongue is protracted at a mean angle of 43±9.4° towards the
denervated side. Regardless of which side of the m. genioglossus is
denervated, animals are never able to aim the tongue towards the active side
after unilateral denervation.
In the one individual that attempted to aim in all the quadrants after denervation, there was a significant deficit when attempting to capture prey in quadrants A, B and C (Table 1). Asmentioned above, when attempting to capture prey in quadrant C (0°), the tongue deviated towards the denervated side, as also occurred when the animal attempted to aim the tongue into quadrants A and B. Interestingly, in Phrynomantis we noticed no deficit in the ability to aim towards the denervated side after surgery, and prey could still be captured at the extreme angles seen before surgery (Table 1).
We also unilaterally denervated the m. genioglossus in at least one individual of several other species of microhylids, a bufonid (B. woodhousii) and a ranid (R. pipiens) (Table 2). Not surprisingly, in all other species of microhylids that were examined, we found a similar effect in which the tongue always deviated towards the denervated side. There appear to be species differences in the angle at which the tongue deviates after denervation (Table 2), varying from 7.1° in Gastrophryne olivacea (Great Plains narrowmouth toad) to 46.9° in Dermatonotus muelleri. These results are in sharp contrast to those seen in both B. woodhousii and R. pipiens. In B. woodhousii, unilateral denervation resulted in a shortening of tongue protraction distance, but the tongue did not deviate from a straight trajectory. However, in R. pipiens, the tongue deviated on average 20° towards the intact side instead of towards the denervated side as was seen in the microhylids.
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Morphological evidence suggests that Phrynomantis also utilizes
hydrostatic elongation for tongue protraction. The tongue of
Phrynomantis is anatomically similar to that of Hemisus in
that both a longitudinal and a dorso-ventral component of the genioglossus
muscle are present. However, the tongue of Phrynomantis differs in
several respects from that of Hemisus. First, the m. genioglossus
longitudinalis and m. genioglossus dorsoventralis both have a connective
tissue origin on the mandible. Second, in Phrynomantis, individual
fibers of the m. genioglossus dorsoventralis are directed longitudinally and
dorso-ventrally (Fig. 4),
whereas in Hemisus they are only in the dorso-ventral plane (compare
Fig. 4 in the present study
with fig. 3 in the study by
Nishikawa et al., 1999). The
functional significance of this arrangement in Phrynomantis remains
unclear, since it seems that a strictly dorso-ventrally arranged compartment
would be the most effective in lengthening the tongue.
Denervation experiments in Phrynomantis also support a muscular hydrostatic mechanism. After unilateral denervation of the entire m. genioglossus (gg) complex, the tongue is protruded towards the denervated (inactive) side when animals attempt to feed on prey lying directly in front of them. This is the expected result if a hydrostatic pushing mechanism is being utilized in tongue protraction. If inertial elongation were at play, the tongue would either show no lateral deviation (as in B. woodhousii) or would be pulled to the active side (as in R. pipiens). In combination with the anatomical data, deviation to the inactive side after unilateral denervation supports the use of a muscular hydrostatic mechanism of tongue protraction in Phrynomantis.
The mechanism of tongue aiming in Phrynomantis
At least three different mechanisms could be used by microhylids to aim
their tongues laterally independent of head movements, including: (1) rotating
the base of the tongue by contraction of accessory slips of the m.
intermandibularis; (2) pulling the tongue to the side by differential
contraction of longitudinal fibers of the m. genioglossus; or (3) pushing the
tongue to one side with hydrostatic pressure generated by dorso-ventral fibers
of the m. genioglossus (as previously described in Hemisus;
Nishikawa et al., 1999). We
will describe each potential mechanism in turn and discuss the evidence for
and against its playing a role in laterally directed tongue protraction in
microhylids.
In the plesiomorphic m. intermandibularis of frogs, all of the fibers are
arranged in parallel, forming a sheet that runs laterally from the mid-ventral
line to the mandibles and the rod-shaped mentomeckelian bones. By contrast,
the m. intermandibularis of some frogs can include one or more accessory slips
with fibers that run from the mandibles to the fascia covering the
submentalis, which in turn lies directly below the base of the tongue pad
(e.g. Trewavas, 1933;
Liem, 1970
;
Tyler, 1971
;
Emerson, 1976
). Accessory slips
of the m. intermandibularis are well developed in many microhylids (see figs
2,
3,
4,
5 in
Emerson, 1976
). Emerson
(1976
) suggested that these
accessory slips of the m. intermandibularis might play a role in tongue
protraction but did not specifically speculate on a potential role in lateral
tongue movement. We hypothesized that differential activation of the right or
left accessory slips might pull the mentomeckelian bones and tilt or rotate
the base of the tongue. Tilting or rotation of the tongue base could plausibly
contribute to lateral movements. Our results do not support this functional
hypothesis, because when the m. intermandibularis complex was denervated
unilaterally, there was no measurable deficit in the range of lateral
protraction to either side. These results unambiguously indicate that the
complex anatomy of the m. intermandibularis and unusually shaped
mentomeckelian bones in microhylids are not functionally related to the
ability to protract the tongue laterally.
The second potential mechanism would involve the m. genioglossus pulling the tongue to one side as it is protracted. The fibers of the ggl originate on the mandible tips and run caudally along the dorsal surface of the resting tongue pad. Asymmetrical recruitment of these fibers could pull the tongue either to the right or left side (as in R. pipiens), the tongue deviating towards the side showing greater activation. If this mechanism were present in microhylids, we would expect that unilateral denervation of the m. genioglossus would cause the tongue to bend towards the intact (active) side. Although we were unable to denervate the ggl and ggdv individually in Phrynomantis, unilateral denervation of both muscles caused the tongue to deviate towards the inactive side. While it is possible that the ggl, when acting alone, pulls the tongue as it does in R. pipiens, the overriding effects of ggdv suggest that the ggl is not determining tongue trajectory.
The third potential mechanism would involve using the hydrostatic pressure generated in the ggdv to aim the tongue to one side or the other during protraction. Because the pressure generated in the ggdv pushes the tongue out of the mouth, differential activation of the ggdv would bend the tongue towards the less active side. This mechanism is consistent with the results of the denervation experiments. Unilateral gg denervation invariably reduced the range of motion of the tongue to part of the range on the denervated (inactive) side.
Although the results of unilateral gg denervation in Phrynomantis
are similar to those in Hemisus, some important differences should be
noted. The tongue of Hemisus initially moves directly forward after
unilateral gg denervation such that prey can still be captured directly in
front of an individual as long as it is not far from the mouth
(Ritter and Nishikawa, 1995).
By contrast, after unilateral gg denervation, the trajectory of the tongue of
Phrynomantis is initially to the denervated side. Thus, food directly
in front of the animal cannot be captured. If Hemisus misses prey
directly anterior to the head after unilateral gg denervation, the tongue tip
follows a semi-circular trajectory and eventually runs into the side of the
head, close to 180° off course (Ritter
and Nishikawa, 1995
). By contrast, the tongue of
Phrynomantis travels in a relatively straight line and lands at
approximately 45° off course (Fig.
6C). Ritter and Nishikawa
(1995
) did not determine if
Hemisus could capture prey positioned to either side of the head
after unilateral gg denervation. Our results indicate that
Phrynomantis can still accurately aim the tongue through a limited
range on the denervated side.
The results of the unilateral gg denervation experiment suggest that both sides of the ggdv are active during protraction regardless of where the tongue is aimed. If prey are presented on the intact (active) side, the tongue is still protruded when attempting to feed. If the ggdv were stimulated unilaterally during aiming, then we would expect no tongue protrusion when attempting to aim towards the active side after unilateral gg denervation. This hypothesis could be further explored by recording bilateral muscle activity from both ggl and ggdv or by selectively denervating each muscle compartment.
The evolution of tongue protraction in microhylids
Previous authors have noted internal compartments of the gg from gross
dissection in several other microhylids, including Callulops
stictogaster (irumbofoie callulops frog;
Burton, 1983),
Breviceps sp., Cophixalus ornatus (ornate rainforest frog)
and Austrochaperina robusta (chirping land frog;
Horton, 1982
). The fact that
all microhylids surveyed to date can aim the tongue laterally and react
similarly to unilateral gg denervation suggests that the ggdv muscle and
hydrostatic elongation are common features of all microhylids. Although the
ggdvs of Hemisus and microhylids differ, in that the former has no
mandibular origin, they may represent a morphocline in the development of an
internal genioglossus compartment. The similarities in morphology and the fact
that Hemisus consistently falls out near Microhylidae in recent
phylogenetic hypotheses (Ford and
Cannatella, 1993
; Wu,
1994
; Emerson et al.,
2000
; Hass, 2003) suggest that the ggdv may be a derived character
of Hemisus and Microhylidae
(Nishikawa et al., 1999
;
Emerson et al., 2000
).
Intrinsic tongue muscles have also been reported in the tongue of
Rhinophrynus dorsalis (Mexican burrowing toad). However, these fibers
are thought to be derived from the m. hyoglossus and only play a role in
changing tongue shape, with tongue protraction being powered mainly by
hyobranchial movements (Trueb and Gans,
1983
).
The results of the present study reveal considerable variation in aiming
prowess among microhylids. This variation in performance suggests that
important morphological variation in the tongue musculature among microhylids
awaits description. Variation in the arrangement of connective tissue and
collagen fibers in the tongue may also play an important role in elongation
and aiming. The orientation of the connective tissue fibers determines the
direction of shape change (Kier and Smith,
1985) and may also influence the extent of tongue elongation
(Zepnewski and Nishikawa,
2000
). Fiber angles less than 54°44' tend to inhibit
elongation, whereas those greater than 54°44' facilitate elongation.
In Hemisus (Nishikawa et al.,
1999
), collagen fibers surrounding the m. genioglossus are
oriented at an angle of nearly 80°, resisting lateral expansion and
facilitating elongation. Although we did not measure fiber angles in
Phrynomantis, we would predict them to be greater than
54°44'. Connective tissue orientation might also explain species
differences in tongue deviation angles after unilateral transection.
Initially, we suspected that post-transection tongue angle could be predicted
by the maximum aiming angle observed during normal feeding, so that species
with the largest aiming angles also exhibited the largest deviations after
transection. However, this is not necessarily the case. For example,
Gastrophryne olivacea aims to a slightly greater extent than
Scaphiophryne marmorata (marbled rain frog) but has a mean deviation
angle substantially less than that of S. marmorata
(Table 2). The underlying
mechanism for these differences in behavior remains unclear. It is likely that
a combination of morphological characteristics, including connective tissue
and muscle fiber orientation, are important, and detailed anatomical studies
are needed.
In summary, it appears that microhylid frogs protract the tongue using a muscular hydrostatic mechanism. This mechanism was previously known only in Hemisus. While protraction is probably accomplished by recruiting the lateral and dorso-ventral portions of the m. genioglossus, lateral displacement is due to the ggdv. Due to the orientation of the surrounding connective tissue, muscle contraction of the ggdv results in lateral rather than longitudinal displacement. This mechanism of protraction increases the range of possible movement relative to that of tongues protracted by mechanical pulling or inertial elongation. Feeding behavior of Phrynomantis and 17 other species of microhylids, representing six subfamilies, suggests that this general mechanism is used by all microhylids. The presence of an intrinsic component of the m. genioglossus, a hydrostatic elongation protraction mechanism and lateral tongue aiming may be synapomorphies of Microhylidae and Hemisus.
Stephen Deban made the original observation that Phrynomantis was capable of aiming its tongue independently of the lower jaw. Sheng-Hai Wu and David Cannatella helped in the identification of specimens. David Cannatella and Ronald Nussbaum kindly provided some of the specimens used in this study. Louis Porras (Zooherp Inc.) and Rob MacInnes (Glades Herp Inc.) helped us acquire many specimens. Mark Mandica created Fig. 1. Christian Jaeger and Stephen Deban helped record feeding sequences. Anthony Herrel, Jen Glass, Kurt Schwenk and an anonymous reviewer provided helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. This study was supported by grant numbers NSF IBN0215438, NSF IBN-0240349 and NIH R25-GM56931 to K.C.N.
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Burton, T. C. (1983). The musculature of the Papuan frog Phrynomantis stictogaster (Anura, Microhylidae). J. Morph. 175,307 -324.
Deban, S. M. and Nishikawa, K. C. (1992). The kinematics of prey capture and the mechanism of tongue protraction in the green tree frog Hyla cinerea. J. Exp. Biol. 170,235 -256.
Emerson, S. B. (1976). A preliminary report on the superficial throat musculature of the Microhylidae and its possible role in tongue action. Copeia 3, 546-551.
Emerson, S. B., Richards, C., Drewes, R. C. and Kjer, K. M. (2000). On the relationships among ranoid frogs: a review of the evidence. Herpetologica 56,209 -230.
Ford, L. S. and Cannatella, D. C. (1993). The major clades of frogs. Herp. Monogr. 7, 94-117.
Gans, C. and Gorniak, G. C. (1982a). How does the toad flip its tongue? Test of two hypotheses. Science 216,1135 -1137.
Gans, C. and Gorniak, G. C. (1982b). Functional morphology of lingual protrusion in marine toads (Bufo marinus). Am. J. Anat. 163,195 -222.[Medline]
Gray, L. A. (1997). Tongue morphology, feeding behavior and feeding ecology in anurans. Ph.D. Dissertation. Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff, AZ, USA.
Haas, A. (2003). Phylogeny of frogs as inferred from primarily larval characters (Amphibia: Anura). Cladistics 19,23 -89.[CrossRef]
Horton, P. (1982). Diversity and systematic significance of anuran tongue musculature. Copeia 3, 595-602.
Humason, G. L. (1979). Animal Tissue Techniques. 4th edition. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Co.
Kier, W. M. and Smith, K. K. (1985). Tongue, tentacles and trunks: the biomechanics of movement in muscular-hydrostats. Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 83,307 -324.
Liem, S. S. (1970). The morphology, systematics, and evolution of Old World treefrogs (Rhacophoridae and Hyperoliidae). Fieldiana Zool. 57, 1-145.
Magimel-Pelonnier, O. (1924). La langue des Amphibiens. These. Faculte des Sciences, Université de Paris, France. A. Saugnac and E. Provillard, Bordeaux.
Meyers, J. J., O'Reilly, J. C. and Nishikawa, K. C. (1996). Tongue aiming in the microhylid frog Phrynomerus bifasciatus. Am. Zool. 36,81A .
Monroy, J. A. and Nishikawa, K. C. (2000). Aiming during prey capture in microhylid frogs. Am. Zool. 40,1135A .
Nishikawa, K. C. (1987). Staining amphibian peripheral nerves with Sudan Black B: progressive vs. regressive methods. Copeia 2,489 -491.
Nishikawa, K. C. (1992). The role of hypoglossal sensory feedback during feeding in the marine toad, Bufo marinus. J. Exp. Biol. 264,245 -252.
Nishikawa, K. C. (1997). Emergence of novel functions during brain evolution. Bioscience 47,341 -354.
Nishikawa, K. C. (1999). Neuromuscular control of prey capture in frogs. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. 354,941 -954.[CrossRef][Medline]
Nishikawa, K. C. (2000). Feeding in frogs. In Feeding in Tetrapod Vertebrates: Form, Function, Phylogeny (ed. K. Schwenk), pp.117 -141.
London: Academic Press. Nishikawa, K. C. and Cannatella, D. C. (1991). Kinematics of prey capture in the tailed frog Ascaphus truei (Anura: Ascaphidae). Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 103,289 -307.
Nishikawa, K. C. and Gans, G. (1996).
Mechanisms of tongue protraction and narial closure in the marine toad
Bufo marinus. J. Exp. Biol.
199,2511
-2529.
Nishikawa, K. C. and Roth, G. (1991). The mechanism of tongue protraction during prey capture in the frog Discoglossus pictus. J. Exp. Biol. 159,217 -234.
Nishikawa, K. C., Kier, W. M. and Smith, K. K.
(1999). Morphology and mechanics of tongue movement in the
African pig-nosed frog Hemisus marmoratum: a muscular hydrostatic
model. J. Exp. Biol.
202,771
-780.
O'Reilly, S. R. and Nishikawa, K. C. (1995). Mechanism of tongue protraction during prey capture in the spadefoot toad Spea mutiplicata (Anura: Pelobatidae). J. Exp. Zool. 273,282 -296.[Medline]
Regal, P. J. and Gans, C. (1976). Functional aspects of the evolution of frog tongues. Evolution 30,718 -734.
Ritter, D. and Nishikawa, K. C. (1995). The kinematics and mechanism of prey capture in the African pig-nosed frog (Hemisus marmoratum): description of a radically divergent anuran tongue. J. Exp. Biol. 198,2025 -2040.[Medline]
Trewavas, E. (1933). The hyoid and larynx of the Anura. Trans. R. Phil. Soc. Lond. 222,401 -527.
Trueb, L. and Gans, C. (1983). Feeding specializations of the Mexican burrowing toad, Rhinophrynus dorsalis (Anura: Rhinophrynidae). J. Zool. Lond. 199,198 -208.
Tyler, M. J. (1971). Observations of anuran myo-integumental attachments associated with the vocal sac apparatus. J. Nat. Hist. 5,225 -231.
Valdez, C. M. and Nishikawa, K. C. (1996). Sensory modulation and behavioral choice during feeding in the Australian frog, Cyclorana novaehollandiae. J. Comp. Physiol. A 180,187 -202.
Wu, S. (1994). Phylogenetic relationships, higher classification, and historical biogeography of the microhyloid frogs (Lissamphibia: Anura: Brevicipitidae and Microhylidae. Ph.D. Dissertation. University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA.
Zepnewski, E. and Nishikawa, K. C. (2000). Connective tissue in ballistic tongues. Am. Zool. 40, 1271A.
Related articles in JEB: