Laboratory for Visual Perception and Cognition, Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Boston University School of Medicine, 700 Albany Street, Boston, MA 02118, USA
![]() |
Abstract |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Analogous studies on animals foster similar conclusions (Schneider, 1979; Goldman-Rakic et al., 1983
; Kolb and Whishaw, 1996
; Payne et al., 1996b
) and amplify upon the initial examples demonstrating the general plasticity of brain connections (Rakic, 1977
; Wiesel, 1982
). Moreover, the degree of functional sparing varies systematically with the age at which the cortical damage was incurred, and they show that the sparing is accompanied by anatomically demonstrable changes in brain wiring (Goldman-Rakic et al., 1983
; Cornwell et al., 1989
; Shupert et al., 1993
; Kolb, 1995
; Payne et al., 1996b
, 2000
; Payne and Lomber, 2001
). Such studies frequently prompt the question: Does discrete damage of the cerebrum early in life induce a redistribution of spared functions and neuronal operations across the remaining cerebral regions? We attempt to answer this question by using temporary cooling deactivation of circumscribed cerebral regions and behavioral analyses of the visually guided behavior of cats that sustained lesions of primary visual cortex shortly after birth; a time when the visual system has substantial capacity to modify its program for developing connections (Payne and Cornwell, 1994
; Payne et al., 1996b
), the outcome of which can be linked to neuronal compensations (Spear, 1995
).
Visual cortex lesions incurred shortly after birth spare the capacity to learn certain visual behavioral tasks involving complex patterns (Cornwell et al., 1989), and spare a number of largely reflexive tasks such as redirection of attention and orienting from a fixation point to a new target (Shupert et al., 1993
). Sparing of the latter behavior has an obligatory requirement on the suprasylvian belt of cortex that includes the cortex lining the middle suprasylvian (MS) sulcus and forming the posterior suprasylvian (PS) gyrus (Fig. 1A
) (Shupert et al., 1993
). These are the same regions that are known, or suspected, to receive substantially expanded projections from other visual structures (Payne et al., 1996b
). The involvement of MS and ventral (v) PS cortices is buttressed by electrophysiological studies that show that both regions contain neurons that frequently exhibit well defined receptive field properties and close-to-normal neural activity levels (Doty, 1961
; Cornwell et al., 1978
; Tumosa et al., 1989
; Guido et al., 1990
, 1992
). However, even with the evidence pointing to posterior (p) MS and vPS cortices as major cerebral players in the sparing of visual functions, no behavioral studies have been carried out to specifically test the actual contributions the two regions each make to spared perceptual and cognitive processing.
|
![]() |
Materials and Methods |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Three domestic female cats and one castrated male cat were used. They were all born in the Laboratory Animal Science Center at the Boston University Medical Center. For all surgical procedures antiseptic precautions were used throughout, and all procedures were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the Boston University Medical Center and carried out in accordance with the policies outlined in the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (No. 86-23). Primary visual cortical areas 17 and 18 were removed bilaterally on the day of birth from all four cats (Fig. 1B) (Lomber et al., 1993
, 1995
), and 6 months later, when the cats had reached adulthood, all four started training on three behavioral tasks requiring detection and orienting to a visual or sound stimulus (Lomber and Payne, 2001
). After extensive training on these and other tasks, bilateral pairs of cooling loops were surgically placed (Fig. 1B
) within the posterior portion of the middle suprasylvian sulcus (pMS sulcal cortex) and in contact with the ventral portion of the posterior suprasylvian gyrus (vPS cortex) in all four cats using established procedures (Lomber et al., 1996b
). The cats were then tested on three detection-and-orienting tasks with and without the cooling loops operational. At the conclusion of all testing, 2-deoxyglucose (2DG) was administered while two, asymmetrically positioned, cooling loops were cooled to experimental temperatures. Following tissue fixation and processing, and preparation of radiograms, regions of low 2DG uptake were identified, the extent of deactivated cortex measured, and the position and extent of the area 17/18 ablation verified.
Behavior Apparatus, Training and Testing
Two arenas were employed for testing the cats' abilities to detect and orient to visual and auditory cues.
Standard Arena (Moving Visual Stimulus)
The standard apparatus is designed to test the cat's ability to detect and orient to a visual stimulus suddenly moved into the visual field through an aperture located on or below the horizontal meridian (Lomber et al., 1996b). The apparatus is a white semicircular platform with a diameter of 88 cm and a perimeter wall 30 cm high. At the base of the wall and around the perimeter are thirteen 3 x 4 cm openings. The openings are separated by distances that subtend an angle of 15° relative to the center of the arena. Openings are positioned from left 90° to right 90°. Background illumination was provided by standard room illumination and was at photopic levels.
Visual and Auditory Arena (Static Visual and Auditory Stimuli)
The overall dimensions of the visual and auditory apparatus are similar to the standard perimetry arena. The significant differences are that the perimeter wall is 45 cm high and has positioned on it, at a height of 25 cm, 13 light emitting diode (LED)microspeaker complexes that are separated by distances that subtend an angle of 15° relative to the center of the arena. Twelve peripheral LEDs, model T-1, and one central LED, model T-1, are illuminated using a 2 V DC current source. Background lighting was reduced and set at low photopic levels. The peripheral speakers emitted broadband white noise 2 dB above background (~52dB) and the central speaker emitted noise at 5 dB above background. The speakers and LEDs were connected to a switchboard located behind the arena. Both visual and auditory stimuli were maintained for the duration of each trial. Below each LEDspeaker complex is a 3.5 cm diameter opening for the delivery of food rewards. This arena has many features in common with the arena described elsewhere (Stein et al., 1989
).
Training
Behavioral training began in adulthood and was in all instances binocular. Standard procedures were adopted (Lomber and Payne, 2001) and included a predetermined, balanced, yet non-systematic, sequence for stimulus presentation. Cats progressed from the moving-visual, onto static-visual and finally onto auditory training only after asymptotic performance was reached on the prior task. Blank trials were incorporated to guard against and measure frequency of scanning of the visual and auditory fields.
Testing
All testing was performed under binocular viewing conditions, and a testing block consisted of 28 trials (two presentations at each of the 12 peripheral locations and four presentations at the central position). Testing sessions were typically carried out once per day, with 1015 blocks of trials being collected in each session. All six combinations of loci deactivations were carried out but not all in the same session. They included: (i) pMS unilaterally, (ii) pMS bilaterally, (iii) vPS unilaterally, (iv) vPS bilaterally, (v) pMS and vPS unilaterally, and (vi) pMS and vPS bilaterally.
Cooling loops were chilled to 3°C, a temperature known to deactivate the full thickness of cortex adjacent to the cooling loop (Payne and Lomber, 1999; Lomber and Payne, 2000a
, b
). For each locus, or pair of loci, being tested a five-step paradigm was followed: (i) baseline data was collected with all sites active. (ii) Testing began while either the left pMS or left vPS alone, or left pMS and vPS regions together, were cooled and deactivated. (iii) Cooling of the homotopic site(s) in the right hemisphere was/were then added to permit the effects of bilateral deactivation to be examined. (iv) The cooling of the left site(s) was terminated and cortex released from cooling while the site(s) in the right hemisphere remained deactivated. (v) Baseline levels were reestablished after release of the right site(s) from cooling. The procedure was repeated at least six times (three times in the order given above above and three times in reverse order). Procedures were repeated at regular intervals over several months to test the stability of the cooling impact. Neither attenuation nor exaggeration of cooling-induced deficits suggest that either cooling-induced neural compensations or cooling damage was detected. In total, >24 000 testing trials were carried out.
2-Deoxyglucose Administration
After a 910 month period of testing, each cat was acclimated over a 23 day period to a cat-restraining bag. After acclimation, two cooling loops (one pMS and one vPS in opposite hemispheres) were chilled to the experimental temperature of 3°C and the cat was injected i.v with four doses of 2-deoxy-D-[U-14C]glucose (25 µCi/kg) at 5 min intervals as described previously (Payne and Lomber, 1999). Following a final 15 min uptake period, 2000 units of Heparin®, 2 ml of 1% sodium nitrite and 35 mg/kg of sodium pentobarbital were injected i.v. in that order. Once deep anesthesia had been reached, the brain was fixed by arterial perfusion with a phosphate buffered paraformaldehyde solution containing 4% sucrose. This method of euthanasia by exsanguination is consistent with the recommendations of the American Veterinary Medical Association Panel on Euthanasia (Andrews et al., 1993
) and for the preparation of scientifically useful tissue. The fixed brain was removed, photographed, blocked, coated in egg albumin and frozen in methylbutane at 35°C. Thirty minutes later, the brain was transferred to a 80°C freezer until it was sectioned.
Tissue Processing and Examination
Histology and Radiography
Twenty-six-micrometer-thick coronal, frozen sections were cut and mounted onto coverslips affixed to cards. Sections were placed against Agfa-Gevaert Structurix X-ray film and stored at 80°C in light-tight cassettes for 4060 days (Payne and Lomber, 1999), when they were developed with Kodak D-19B developer. Adjacent sections were stained for Nissl substance or cytochrome oxidase to verify location of the cooling loop and absence of surgical or cooling induced damage. The same sections were also used to estimate the extent and location of the primary visual cortex lesion.
Extent of Cooling Deactivation
Cooling-induced decreases in 2DG uptake are obvious (Payne et al., 1996c) and only require sophisticated imaging equipment to assay the gradients on the fringes of the deactivation. For these purposes we used an AISTM microcomputer-based image analyzer (Imaging Research, Inc., St Catherines, Ontario, Canada), in conjunction with 14C standards (Amersham Corp., Arlington Heights, IL) and calibration curves (Gonzalez-Lima, 1992
).
Lesion Extent
The size and location of the visual cortex ablation was assessed directly and by using standard procedures to examine cerebral cortex and LGN in the coronal sections stained for Nissl substance and cytochrome oxidase reaction product (Payne and Lomber, 1996).
Data Analyses
For each animal, initial within subject t-tests were carried out between the pre- and post-cool active conditions to confirm stationarity of behavior across the group of test blocks. Individual means during the active, deactivated and reactivated' conditions were calculated for each testing session. For all the histograms presented, group means and standard errors are based upon individual means from equal numbers of testing sessions for each cat. Data for the 0° position in the static and auditory arena were divided equally between the two hemifields because in this instance the cynosure was also the stimulus. This step was unnecessary for the moving stimulus because the stimulus was introduced into the arena adjacent to the cynosure. The mean performance while the cortex was either warm or cold was compared for all subjects examined on a given task using within-subject t-tests. Within the Results section, unless otherwise stated, if a difference is described as being significant or reliable, it has a P 0.01.
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Gross examination of the fixed brain showed that nearly all of the marginal and posterolateral gyri, as well as the medial aspect of the junction of the middleposterior suprasylvian gyri, were removed from every hemisphere. Figure 2 shows reconstructions of the cortical ablations plotted onto standardized drawings of the dorsal view of the cerebral cortex. Microscopical analyses also revealed minor, variable degrees of tissue sparing on the upper bank of the splenial sulcus and caudally on the posterolateral gyrus. These regions can be readily identified on the coronal sections taken through regions containing the ablation (Fig. 2
). The extent of the ablation of areas 17 and 18 was verified by examination of the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN). Severe retrograde degeneration was evident throughout layers A and A1 of the dLGN, although a small number of neurons survived in these layers, as is typical following early lesions of primary visual cortical areas 17 and 18 (Doty, 1961
; Cornwell et al., 1978
; Spear et al., 1980
; Lomber et al., 1995
; Payne and Lomber, 1998
). In addition, there was minor to moderate degeneration evident in the medial interlaminar nucleus and the C-laminae. Degeneration was not detected in either the pulvinar or lateral posterior nuclei. These patterns of degeneration are consistent with both our earlier descriptions (Lomber et al., 1993
, 1995
) and the known pattern of anatomical connections between dLGN and visual cortex at the time the lesion was made (Cornwell et al., 1984
; Payne et al., 1988
). Overall, between 93 and 97% of combined areas 17 and 18 were removed from each hemisphere. In addition, there was moderate to major inclusion of area 19 and inclusion of portions of the splenial visual area (Kalia and Whitteridge, 1973
) in the ablation. The inclusion of these regions was a natural product of our attempts to maximize the ablation of areas 17 and 18.
|
Examination of Nissl and myelin tissue revealed that neither the surgical procedures nor the chronic presence of the cooling loops and the repeated deactivations had any impact on the structural integrity of the MS and vPS cortices adjacent to the cryoloops. This conclusion was extended to long-term functional integrity in both the MS and vPS regions because cytochrome oxidase reaction product was of a high or medium level, respectively, as is typical of brains with early lesions of primary visual cortex (Long et al., 1996). Concordant with this view, the 2DG radiograms of cortex adjacent to the non-operational MS and vPS cryoloops during 2DG administration exhibited rich gray densities indicative of normal 2DG, uptake and normal brain function at the time of the 2DG administration (Fig. 3
, left). They contrasted with the very pale regions indicative of greatly reduced 2DG uptake, and absence of neural activity in the MS and vPS cortices adjacent to the two cryoloops that were operational at the time the 2DG was administered (Fig. 3
, right).
|
In the vPS region, low 2DG uptake characterized all of the vPS cortex (Fig. 3 lower right) and much of the hippocampal fusiform gyrus, as intended. This combined region corresponds to virtually all of area 20 on the lateral and tentorial surfaces (Updyke, 1986
) and the ventral-most portion of area 21 (Tusa and Palmer, 1980
). There was little or no spread of cooling to area PS of Updyke (Updyke, 1986
).
Visual Detection and Orienting Task: Moving Stimulus
After more than 2000 training trials, cats with ablations of areas 17 and 18 incurred on the day of birth are extremely proficient at detecting and orienting towards a high contrast stimulus moved into the perimetry arena through any of the arena-floor openings [Fig. 4A(i)]. This level of performance approaches that of intact cats in other studies (Shupert et al., 1993
; Lomber and Payne, 1996
), and is far superior to the 3060% level of proficiency exhibited by cats that incurred equivalent, or smaller, lesions in adulthood (Shupert et al., 1993
; Payne and Lomber, 2001
).
|
Deactivation of either left or right pMS sulcal cortex [Fig. 5A(ii,iv)] resulted in a mean decrease of 34% in orienting proficiency to stimuli moved into the contracooled hemifield (opposite side to the deactivation). The impairment was spread across the hemifield with more peripheral positions exhibiting a greater fragility [Fig. 4B
(i)]. Interleaved presentation of stimuli in the ipsicooled hemifield (same side as the deactivation) revealed virtually no impact of the cooling on orienting performance [Fig. 5A
(ii,iv)]. As we have learned to expect from the intact brain, additional deactivation of the homotopic pMS site in the opposite hemisphere restored visual orienting into the previously partially neglected hemifield [Fig. 5A
(iii)] for all positions examined [Fig. 4B
(ii)]. Subsequent release from all cooling resulted in levels of orienting proficiency that matched precool levels [Figs 4A
(ii) and 5A
(v)].
|
Unilateral deactivation of vPS cortex did not modify the cat's ability to detect and orient towards stimuli moved into either the contracooled or ipsicooled hemifields [Figs 4C(i) and 5B
(ii,iv)]. However, bilateral deactivation of vPS cortex cooling significantly reduced orienting throughout the visual field from 94 ± 2 to 77 ± 2%, and this reduction was manifest with only a slightly greater decrease for peripheral than for more central positions in the visual field [Fig. 4C
(ii)]. Following termination of all cooling, orienting performance returned to precool levels [Figs 4A
(ii) and 5B
(v)].
Moving Visual Stimulus: Deactivation of both pMS and vPS cortices
The partial impairments induced by unilateral pMS and bilateral vPS deactivations suggested to us that both regions in cats with lesions of areas 17 and 18 sustained on postnatal day 1 (day of birth; P1) contribute in important ways, but under different circumstances, to proficient detection and orienting toward visual stimuli. Based on these observations, we tested for, and showed, the additive impact of combined unilateral cooling of pMS and vPS cortices [Fig. 6A(iii)] on orienting performance, which was reduced to 18.9 ± 4.3% and approached a full neglect of that hemifield. Interleaved trials showed that orienting into the ipsicooled hemifield was unimpaired.
|
Figure 7 shows the impact of these sequential coolings relative to visual field position. Unilateral cooling of pMS cortex reduced orienting proficiency across all parts of the contracooled hemifield with the peripheral-to-center asymmetry of impact noted earlier (Fig. 7B
). The addition of cooling of vPS cortex on the same side substantially diminished the poor to modest orienting performance further, and modest orienting performance was only maintained for the contralateral juxta-central position. The further addition of cooling of pMS cortex in the opposite hemisphere resulted in improved, but less than perfect, orienting proficiency throughout the previously neglected hemifield (Fig. 7D
). Auxiliary cooling of the contralateral vPS cortex improved orienting proficiency further (Fig. 7E
), and it reached close to perfect levels for right juxta-central, 15° and 30° positions, but partial impairments remained at more peripheral positions. Concurrent with this improvement, a deficit in orienting performance was detected in the periphery of the initially robust left hemifield ipsilateral to the initial unilateral right coolings. Finally, cessation of all cooling completely reversed the impairments (not shown).
|
Overall, cats with lesions of areas 17 and 18 sustained on P1 orient less proficiently to illuminated, high contrast LEDs in a darkened arena [65 ± 4%; Fig. 8A(i)] than to a moving stimulus in a bright arena [94 ± 3%; Fig. 4A
(i)], and comparison of radial diagrams reveals that the weakness in orienting is towards LEDs illuminated in the peripheral one-third of the field from 60° outwards [Fig. 9A
(i)], orienting to more central positions being close-to-perfect or perfect [Fig. 9A
(i)]. Since performance is inferior to that of highly trained intact cats, and not markedly better than that exhibited by adult cats with equivalent lesions of primary visual cortex (Payne et al., 2000
; Payne and Lomber, 2001
), performance represents residual visual capabilities rather than spared vision. Visual capacities after lesions of primary visual cortex can be divided into three main categories: (i) residual vision describes the visual capacities that remain after lesions; (ii) recovered vision describes the visual capacities that remain after lesions if these capacities emerged from, and are superior to, to residual vision; recovery is rarely complete; and (iii) spared vision describes the visual capacities that are present after lesions incurred in the earlier part of life before faculties have fully matured; spared vision is always greater than both residual and recovered vision.
|
|
Unilateral deactivation of pMS sulcal cortex induced a profound neglect of the LEDs illuminated in the contracooled hemifield that was initially complete and more profound [10.1 ± 5.7%; Fig. 8A(ii,iv)] than the neglect of the moving stimulus [59 ± 4%; Fig. 4A
(ii,iv)]. In contradistinction, no negative impact of the cooling on orienting responses into the ipsicooled hemifield was identified [Fig. 8A
(ii)], and the radial plot was largely indistinguishable from the data collected during the precool trials [Fig. 9B
(i)], although there was a hint of a slight improvement in orienting into the ipsicooled hemifield [compare Fig. 8A
(ii,iv) with Fig. 8A
(i)].
In contrast to bilateral deactivation of pMS cortex and the test with the moving stimulus, which resulted in unimpaired orienting, the same bilateral deactivation of pMS cortices induced a substantial neglect of the illuminated LEDs in both hemifields [Fig. 8A(iii)]. The radial plots reveal that the limited orienting capability that remained was only for central-most positions [Fig. 9B
(ii)]. Terminations of cooling reversed the cooling-induced deficits and performance duplicated precool levels.
vPS Cortex Deactivation
Unilateral deactivation of vPS cortex did not impair orienting to an illuminated LED, performance was indistinguishable for targets illuminated in the ipsicooled and contracooled hemifields [Fig. 8B(ii,iv)], and the radial diagrams are indistinguishable from those representing data collected prior to cooling [compare Fig. 9C
(i) and Fig. 9A
(i)]. In contrast, bilateral deactivation of vPS cortex impaired orienting to LEDs illuminated in both hemifields [Fig. 8B
(iii)], and residual orienting capability was focused solely on the cynosure and flanking regions [Fig. 9C
(ii)]. In all instances, orienting performance following cessation of cooling reproduced the performance measured prior to cooling [Figs 8B
(v) and 9A(ii)].
Auditory Detection and Orienting Task
Overall, cats with ablations of primary visual cortical areas 17 and 18 sustained on the day of birth are moderately proficient (6575% correct) at orienting to a stationary, broadband white-noise auditory stimulus [Fig. 10A(i)], and radial diagrams reveal differential performance levels for central versus more peripheral positions. Orienting to the auditory stimulus is perfect, or nearly so, for positions extending out to 45° left and right, with poorer or no orienting performance to more peripheral locations (Fig. 11A
). The characteristics of this performance are comparable to those exhibited by intact cats and cats that incurred equivalent lesions of primary visual cortex sustained in adulthood (Payne et al., 2000
; Payne and Lomber, 2001
).
|
|
Neither unilateral nor bilateral cooling of either pMS or vPS cortex had any material impact on orienting to the broadband white-noise auditory stimulus (Figs 10 and 11).
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|
One of the most robust pathway expansions is from the retina via the surviving neurons in the LGN to the middle suprasylvian cortex in the visual parietal region (Tong et al., 1984; Kalil et al., 1991
; Lomber et al., 1993
, 1995
; Payne and Lomber, 1998
), where some of the most robust neural compensations have been identified, including sparing of at least some aspects of motion processing amongst other properties (Spear et al., 1980
; Guido et al., 1990
). The identified compensations also likely rely heavily upon signals transmitted from the retina through the midbrain to the medial part of the lateral posterior complex, which is induced by primary visual cortex to double its already massive projection to the MS cortex, and upon expanded projections from vPS cortex (Lomber et al., 1995
; MacNeil et al., 1996
). However, the physiological studies only provide corollary data, and do not test directly for the contributions specific structures make to the sparing and compensations. Even so, in accord with the physiological data, cats with early lesions of primary visual cortex exhibit sparing of visually guided orienting to stimuli moved into an arena but not to illumination of LEDs, although substantial ability to orient to the latter stimuli remains following the lesion (Shupert et al., 1993
; Payne et al., 2000
; Payne and Lomber, 2001
).
From studies using cooling deactivation to probe the functions of the cerebral cortex in normal cats, these same two visually guided behaviors are critically dependent upon pMS cortex, although the superior colliculus is also a critically dependent structure (Lomber and Payne, 1996, 2001
; Lomber et al., 1996b; Payne et al., 1996a
). Even so, the dependence on pMS cortex is task dependent because unilateral, but not bilateral, deactivations of it impair orienting to a stimulus moved into the contra-cooled hemifield, whereas for the illumination of the static LEDs orienting into the contracooled hemifield is blocked by either unilateral or bilateral deactivation (Lomber and Payne, 2001
). In terms of the deficits induced by unilateral and bilateral deactivations on the moved and static versions of the detection and orienting tasks, the dependencies established for the intact cat are also represented in cats with early lesions of primary visual cortical areas, but only partially so because there are some notable differences. These differences are summarized and can be compared in Table 1
(lower cells).
Both pMS and vPS cortices of cats that incurred lesions of areas 17 and 18 on the day of birth contribute importantly to the sparing of detection and orienting performance toward moved targets. However, the respective contributions vary in magnitude and become apparent under different experimental circumstances. For example, even though we are confident from our 2DG measures that the full extent of pMS cortex was deactivated by the cooling, we could only induce dependable (meaning present in all four cats) inconsistencies in orienting performance to the moved stimulus when the deactivation was unilateral. These inconsistencies are in marked contrast to the profound, virtually complete, neglect induced by equivalent cooling deactivation of the intact cat. We thus have a result that demonstrates a muted contribution of pMS cortex to spared orienting, and one that argues forcefully for contributions to spared orienting performance by regions that were not deactivated. At least one of these regions is vPS cortex, because addition of its deactivation to an already existing unilateral pMS deactivation exaggerates the impairment in performance and induces a neglect that, in its profundity, approaches the neglect identified during unilateral cooling of pMS cortex alone in intact cats. However, unilateral deactivation of vPS cortex alone has no impact. Thus, there is a lessened role for pMS cortex in visual orienting and a heightened involvement of vPS cortex. This conclusion promotes the view that the circuitry underlying the sparing of visually guided detection and orienting behavior in response to moving stimuli has been modified by the early lesion of primary visual cortical areas 17 and 18, and it has become redistributed across the cortical surface from its normally highly localizable site on the banks of the pMS sulcus to include vPS cortex. This view is buttressed by the results of bilateral cooling deactivation of vPS cortices that induce global inconsistencies across the visual field in orienting performance, but no profound neglect. It is important to state that we are confident that the deficit induced by vPS deactivations is one of a failure to disengage and redirect attention and not one of blindness or some other global visual impairment, because as yet unpublished companion studies of ours show that the same cats are able to reliably make certain types of basic visual form discriminations when vPS cortex is deactivated bilaterally.
At present, we do not know the basis for the broad, modest decrease in orienting behavior during bilateral cooling deactivation of vPS cortex in the absence of any impact during unilateral vPS deactivation. We can only speculate that each vPS cortex contains a representation of all, or almost all, of the visual field and both representations need to be silenced to induce a weakness in orienting. Such, suprahemifield representations have not been identified in visual field maps of the temporal cortex in intact cats (Tusa and Palmer, 1980; Payne, 1993
), although they have been identified in monkeys (Gross, 1973a
, b
).
Even with the rewiring and redistribution of functions, incremental addition of deactivation of first pMS cortex and then vPS cortex in the hemisphere opposite a combined pMS and vPS deactivation first partially, and then completely, restores orienting ability to moving stimuli presented across large regions of the visual field. This result parallels observations made in intact cats when pMS cortex is deactivated first unilaterally and then bilaterally (Lomber and Payne, 1996). However, the restoration in the central two-thirds of the previously neglected hemifield in the early-lesioned cats is at a cost of orienting to the peripheral one-third of the previously responsive field. These results suggest that pMS and vPS cortices are critical to orienting behavior only under certain conditions, and that other structures, such as the superior colliculus (Lomber and Payne, 1996
; Lomber et al., 1996b
; Payne et al., 1996a
), are sufficient to support orienting to stimuli in the central two-thirds, but not the peripheral one-third, of the visual field when the major cortical structures are silenced bilaterally. The basis for the functional restoration in orienting supported by the bilateral deactivations is thought to be intimately related to a restoration in the balance of the competitive mechanisms operating to allow attentional neural machinery to gain control over motor responses (Lomber and Payne, 1996
; Lomber et al., 1996b
).
Largely concordant results and conclusions were obtained for the residual detection and orienting visual capacities tested by the illumination of LEDs. However, with this stimulus, as in intact cats, bilateral deactivations even of pMS cortices virtually abolished orienting throughout the visual field, and not restoration of orienting ability. These observations suggest that the competitive mechanisms enunciated to account for the differences in the results of unilateral and bilateral cooling when cats were tested with moving stimuli do not apply when stationary LED stimuli are used. These results also argue compellingly that pMS and vPS cortices alone are critically important structures that contribute to orienting to stationary LED stimuli, and no other structures can subsume their roles. Reassuringly, neither the lesion of primary visual cortex nor any of the deactivations in visual parietal and temporal regions had any impact on the detection and orienting of sound stimuli, thus verifying the specificity of the lesions and deactivations for visual functions, and that the deactivations had no direct impact on the motor reporting system.
An important element in comprehending the differences in the conclusions reached for the moving and static stimuli may be that a moving stimulus has a greater potency to activate neurons. This greater potency may be based on the spatial translations and the larger ensembles of neurons that are activated compared with illumination of the static LED stimulus at a single visual coordinate. The greater potency of the moved stimulus may also permit us to detect the visual sparing above residual visual function, whereas neural activity generated by the static stimulus may be insufficient to permit such a separation.
![]() |
Epilogue |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Finally, what we have not be able to glean from our data is whether there is a price to pay for the redistribution, and that functions normally associated with vPS cortex are displaced, or crowded out (Teuber, 1975), as the region acquires or emphasizes functions normally only weakly represented, if at all. We also do not know what functions may have been acquired by pMS cortex through pathway rewirings and the diminution of contributions to detection and orienting functions. Hopefully, these will be topics of future reports.
![]() |
Notes |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Address correspondence to Stephen G. Lomber, Laboratory for Visual Perception and Cognition, Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Boston University School of Medicine, 700 Albany Street, Boston, MA 02118, USA. Email: slomber{at}bu.edu.
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Andrews EJ, Bennett BT, Clark JD, Houpt KA, Pascoe PJ, Robinson GW, Boyce JR (1993) Report of the American Veterinary Medical Association Panel on Euthanasia. J Am Vet Med Assoc 202:229249.[Medline]
Bates E (1999) Plasticity, localization and language development. In: The changing nervous system: neurobehavioral consequences of early brain disorders (Broman SH, Fletcher JM, eds), pp. 214253. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cornwell P, Overman WH, Ross C (1978) Extent of recovery from neonatal damage to the cortical visual system in cats. J Comp Physiol Psychol 92:255270.[ISI][Medline]
Cornwell P, Ravizza R, Payne BR (1984) Extrinsic visual and auditory cortical connections in the four-day-old kitten. J Comp Neurol 229: 97120.[ISI][Medline]
Cornwell P, Herbein S, Corso C, Eskew R, Warren JM, Payne B (1989) Selective sparing after lesions of visual cortex in newborn kittens. Behav Neurosci 103:11761190.[ISI][Medline]
Doty RW (1961) Functional significance of the topographical aspects of the retinocortical projection. In: The visual system: neurophysiology and psychophysics (Jung R, Kornhuber H, eds), pp. 228247. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Goldman PS, Alexander GE (1977) Maturation of prefrontal cortex in the monkey revealed by local reversible cryogenic depression. Nature 267:613617.[ISI][Medline]
Goldman-Rakic PS, Isseroff A, Schwartz ML, Bugbee NM (1983) The neurobiology of cognitive development. In: Handbook of child psychology: biology and infancy development (Mussen P, ed.), pp. 281344. New York: Wiley.
Gonzalez-Lima F (1992) Brain imaging of auditory functions in cats: studies with fluoroxyglucose autoradiography and cytochrome oxidase histochemistry. In: Advances in metabolic mapping techniques for brain imaging of behavioral and learning functions (Gonzalez-Lima F, Finkenstaedt T, Scheich H, eds). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Grant S, Shipp S (1991) Visuotopic organization of the lateral suprasylvian area and of an adjacent area of the ectosylvian gyrus of cat cortex: a physiological and connectional study. Vis Neurosci 6:315338.[ISI][Medline]
Gross CG (1973a) Visual functions of inferotemporal cortex. In: Handbook of sensory physiology, Vol. VII/3B. Central processing of visual information, part B (Jung R, ed.), pp. 451482. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Gross CG (1973b) Inferotemporal cortex and vision. Prog Physiol Psychol 5:77123.
Guido W, Spear PD, Tong L (1990) Functional compensation in the lateral suprasylvian area following bilateral visual cortex damage in kittens. Exp Brain Res 83:219224.[ISI][Medline]
Guido W, Spear PD, Tong L (1992) How complete is the physiological compensation in extrastriate cortex after visual cortex damage in kittens? Exp Brain Res 91:455466.[ISI][Medline]
Kalia M, Whitteridge D (1973) The visual areas in the splenial sulcus of the cat. J Neurophysiol (Lond) 232:275283.
Kalil RE, Tong LL, Spear PD (1991) Thalamic projections to the lateral suprasylvian visual area in cats with neonatal or adult visual cortex damage. J Comp Neurol 314:512525.[ISI][Medline]
Kolb B (1995) Plasticity and behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kolb B, Whishaw IQ (1996) Fundamentals of human psychology, 4th edn. New York: Freeman.
Lomber SG (1999) The advantages and limitations of permanent or reversible deactivation techniques in the assessment of neural function. J Neurosci Methods 86:109117.[ISI][Medline]
Lomber SG, Payne BR (1996) Removal of two halves restores the whole: reversal of visual hemineglect during bilateral cortical or collicular inactivation in the cat. Vis Neurosci 13:11431156.[ISI][Medline]
Lomber SG, Payne BR (2000a) Contributions of posterior parietal cortex to visuospatial discrimination functions in cat. Vis Neurosci 17:701709.[ISI][Medline]
Lomber SG, Payne BR (2000b) Translaminar differentiation of visuallyguided behaviors revealed by restricted cerebral cooling deactivation. Cereb Cortex 10:10661077.
Lomber SG, Payne BR (2001) Task-specific reversal of visual hemineglect following bilateral virtual lesions of posterior parietal cortex: a comparison with virtual lesions of the superior colliculus. Vis Neurosci (in press).
Lomber SG, Payne BR, Cornwell P, Pearson HE (1993) Capacity of the retinogeniculate pathway to reorganize following ablation of visual cortical areas in developing and mature cats. J Comp Neurol 338:432457.[ISI][Medline]
Lomber SG, Cornwell P, Sun JS, MacNeil MA, Payne BR (1994) Reversible inactivation of motion processing operations in visual cortex of the behaving cat. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91:29993003.[Abstract]
Lomber SG, MacNeil MA, Payne BR (1995) Amplification of thalamic projections to middle suprasylvian cortex following ablation of immature primary visual cortex in the cat. Cereb Cortex 5:166191.[Abstract]
Lomber SG, Payne BR, Cornwell P (1996a) Learning and recall of form discriminations during reversible cooling deactivation of ventralposterior suprasylvian cortex in the cat. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:16541658.
Lomber SG, Payne BR, Cornwell P, Long KD (1996b) Perceptual and cognitive visual functions of parietal and temporal cortices in the cat. Cereb Cortex 6:673695.[Abstract]
Lomber SG, Payne BR, Horel JA (1999) The cryoloop: an adaptable reversible cooling deactivation method for behavioral and electrophysiological assessment of neural function. J Neurosci Methods 86:179194.[ISI][Medline]
Long KD, Lomber SG, Payne BR (1996) Increased oxidative metabolism in middle suprasylvian cortex following removal of areas 17 and 18 from newborn cats. Exp Brain Res 110:335346.[ISI][Medline]
MacNeil MA, Lomber SG, Payne BR (1996) Rewiring of transcortical projections to middle suprasylvian cortex following early removal of cat areas 17 and 18. Cereb Cortex 6:362376.
Mendola JD, Payne BR (1993) Direction selectivity and physiological compensation in the superior colliculus following removal of areas 17 and 18. Vis Neurosci 10:10191026.[ISI][Medline]
Milner B (1974) Sparing of language function after early unilateral brain damage. Neurosci Res Prog Bull 2:213217.
Ogden J (1989) Visuospatial and other right-hemispheric functions after long recovery periods in left-hemispherectomized subjects. Neuropsychologia 27:765776.[ISI][Medline]
Palmer LA, Rosenquist AC, Tusa RJ (1978) The retinotopic organization of lateral suprasylvian visual areas in the cat. J Comp Neurol 177: 237256.[ISI][Medline]
Payne BR (1993) Evidence for visual cortical area homologs in cat and macaque monkey. Cereb Cortex 3:125.[Abstract]
Payne BR, Cornwell P (1994) System-wide repercussions of immature visual cortex damage. Trends Neurosci 17:126130.[ISI][Medline]
Payne BR, Lomber SG (1996) Age dependent modification of cytochrome oxidase activity in the cat dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus following removal of primary visual cortex. Vis Neurosci 13:805816.[ISI][Medline]
Payne BR, Lomber SG (1998) Neuroplasticity in the cat's visual system: origin, termination, expansion and increased coupling in the retinogeniculo-middle suprasylvian visual pathway following early ablations of areas 17 and 18. Exp Brain Res 121:334349.[ISI][Medline]
Payne BR, Lomber SG (1999) A method to assess the functional impact of cerebral connections on target populations of neurons. J Neurosci Methods 86:195208.[ISI][Medline]
Payne BR, Lomber SG (2001) Rehabilitative training ameliorates deficits in visual detection and orienting following lesions of primary visual cortex sustained in adulthood and in infancy. Restor Neurol Neurosci (in press).
Payne B, Pearson H, Cornwell P (1988) Development of visual and auditory connections in the cat. In: Cerebral cortex, Vol. 7. Development and maturation of the cerebral cortex (Peters A, Jones EG, eds), pp. 309389. New York: Plenum.
Payne BR, Lomber SG, Geeraerts S, van der Gucht E, Vandenbussche E (1996a) Reversible visual hemineglect. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93: 290294.
Payne BR, Lomber SG, MacNeil MA, Cornwell P (1996b) Evidence for greater sight in blindsight following damage of primary visual cortex early in life. Neuropsychologia 34:741774.[ISI][Medline]
Payne BR, Lomber SG, Villa AE, Bullier J (1996c) Reversible deactivation of cerebral network components. Trends Neurosci 19:535542.[ISI][Medline]
Payne BR, Lomber SG, Gelston CD (2000) Graded sparing of visuallyguided orienting following primary visual cortex ablations within the first postnatal month. Behav Brain Res 117:111.[ISI][Medline]
Raczkowski D, Rosenquist AC (1983) Connections of the multiple visual cortical areas with the lateral posteriorpulvinar complex and adjacent thalamic nuclei in the cat. J Neurosci 3:19121942.[Abstract]
Rakic P (1977) Prenatal development of the visual system in rhesus monkey. Phil Trans Roy Soc Lond B 278:245260.[ISI]
Reinoso-Suarez F (1961) Topographischer Hirnatlas der Katze für experimental-physiologische Untersuchungen [Topographical atlas of the cat brain for experimental-physiological research]. Darmstadt: Merck.
Rudel RG, Teuber HL, Twitchel TE (1974) Levels of impairment of sensori-motor functions in children with early brain damage. Neuropsychologia 12:95108.[ISI][Medline]
Schneider GE (1979) Is it really better to have your brain lesion early? A revision of the "Kennard Principle". Neuropsychologia 17: 557583.[ISI][Medline]
Sherk H (1986) Location and connections of visual cortical areas in the cat's suprasylvian sulcus. J Comp Neurol 247:131.[ISI][Medline]
Shupert C, Cornwell P, Payne B (1993) Differential sparing of depth perception, orienting and optokinetic nystagmus after neonatal versus adult lesions of cortical areas 17, 18 and, 19 in the cat. Behav Neurosci 107:633650.[ISI][Medline]
Spear PD (1995) Plasticity following neonatal visual cortex damage in cats. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 73:13891397.[ISI][Medline]
Spear PD, Kalil RE, Tong L (1980) Functional compensation in lateral suprasylvian visual area following neonatal visual cortex removal in cats. J Neurophysiol 43:851869.
Stein BE, Meredith MA, Huneycutt WS, McDale L (1989) Behavioral indices of multisensory integration: orientation to visual cues is affected by auditory stimuli. J Cogn Neurosci 1:1224.
Stiles J, Nass R (1991) Spatial grouping activity in young children with congenital right or left hemisphere brain injury. Brain Cogn 15: 201222.[Medline]
Teuber H-L (1975) Recovery of function after brain injury in man. In: Ciba Foundation Symposium 34. Outcome of severe damage to the nervous system. Amsterdam: Elsevier North-Holland.
Tong L, Kalil RE, Spear PD (1984) Critical periods for functional and anatomical compensation in lateral suprasylvian visual area following removal of visual cortex in cats. J Neurophysiol 52:941960.
Tumosa N, McCall MA, Guido W, Spear PD (1989) Responses of lateral geniculate neurons that survive long-term visual cortex damage in kittens and adult cats. J Neurosci 9:280298.[Abstract]
Tusa RJ, Palmer LA (1980) Retinotopic organization of areas 20 and 21 in the cat. J Comp Neurol 193:147164.[ISI][Medline]
Updyke BV (1986) Retinotopic organization within the cat's posterior suprasylvian sulcus and gyrus. J Comp Neurol 246:265280.[ISI][Medline]
van den Hout BM, Stiers P, Haers M, Van der Schouw YT, Vandenbussche E, van Niuewenhuizen O, de Vries LS (2000) Relation between visual perceptual impairment and neonatal ultrasound diagnosis in haemorrhagicischaemic brain lesions in 5-year-old children. Devl Med Child Neurol 42:376386.
Wiesel TN (1982) Postnatal development of the visual cortex and the influence of environment. Nature 299:583591.[ISI][Medline]
Woods BT (1981) The restricted effects of right-hemisphere lesions after age one; Wechsler test data. Neuropsychologia 18:6570.[ISI]