1 Center for Non-invasive Brain Stimulation, Harvard Medical School and Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Boston, MA, USA, 2 Functional Brain Mapping Laboratory, Department of Neurology, University Hospital Geneva, Switzerland, 3 Neurology Department, Charité, Humboldt University, Berlin, Germany and 4 Institut Guttman, Barcelona, Spain
Address correspondence to Alvaro Pascual-Leone, Center for Non-invasive Brain Stimulation, Harvard Medical School, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, 330 Brookline Avenue, Kirstein Building KS 454, Boston, MA 02215, USA. Email: apleone{at}bidmc.harvard.edu.
![]() |
Abstract |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Key Words: attention posterior parietal cortex rTMS spatial disengagement spatial orienting
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Regarding the role of PPC in spatial attention, a distinction has been drawn between ventral PPC, which is a part of the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ), and dorsal PPC areas along the intraparietal sulcus (IPs) (e.g. Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). While the contribution of dorsal PPC/IPs lesions to neglect is still a matter of debate (e.g. Corbetta and Shulman, 2002
), there is a general consensus that damage to the ventral PPC and/or other areas of the TPJ is a principle substrate of neglect and other clinical phenomena with visuo-spatial deficits such as extinction (e.g. Vallar, 2001
; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002
, Karnath et al., 2003
). Likewise, while there are contradictory results on the role of dorsal PPC in attentional control when tested within Posner et al.'s influential attention framework (Posner et al., 1984
), converging evidence has been provided as to the role of ventral PPC. Posner's framework breaks down the mechanisms of directing visual attention in space into the following three elementary mental operations that can be performed covertly, without overt movements of the eyes or head: (i) disengagement from a current focus of attention (spatial disengagement); (ii) directing attention to a new spatial location (spatial orienting); and (iii) engagement of the target. The framework further distinguishes between attention operations performed under endogenous control (e.g. voluntary orienting) and those driven by a peripheral cue (e.g. reflexive orienting). Within this framework, contradictory results have been obtained regarding dorsal PPC function as many neuroimaging studies have implicated the dorsal PPC (along with areas in the IPs) in voluntary spatial orienting under endogenous control (e.g. Corbetta et al., 1993
, 2000
; Nobre et al., 1997
; Kastner et al., 1999
; Hopfinger et al., 2000
) but this function appears to be relatively unimpaired in patients with PPC lesions. In fact, patients with PPC damage can use endogenous information to facilitate spatial orienting of attention in any direction (Posner et al., 1984
; Friedrich et al., 1998
; Smania et al., 1998
; Bartolomeo and Chokron, 2002
). Converging results, on the other hand, have been obtained for the ventral PPC, suggesting involvement in spatial disengagement along with other areas of the TPJ. Functional imaging in healthy subjects has revealed TPJ activation with disengagement of spatial attention (Corbetta et al., 2000
). In agreement with this, PPC lesions have been related to prominent spatial disengagement deficits in contralesional direction (e.g. Posner et al., 1984
, 1987
; for review, see Losier and Klein, 2001
), especially when TPJ damage is involved (Friedrich et al., 1998
).
In the present repetitive TMS (rTMS) study, we aimed to shed further light on dorsal PPC involvement in voluntary spatial orienting. In addition, we sought to further investigate whether disruption of this dorsal PPC function might contribute to spatially lateralized attention deficits as evidenced in neglect. For this purpose, rTMS was applied over the right dorsal PPC and a variant of the Posner paradigm was used (Posner et al., 1984). In our study, binaural cues prompted voluntary orienting and maintaining of visual attention at either a left or a right hemifield position (directional cues). This was followed by presentation of a visual target flashed more often at cued locations (valid trials) than at uncued locations or bilaterally (invalid trials). To be able to isolate rTMS-effects on voluntary space exploration, additional neutral cue trials, in which targets were equally likely to appear in either visual field (no spatial cueing but otherwise identical task components), were presented for control purposes. Based on previous evidence of dorsal PPC involvement in voluntary orienting, and given the finding that dorsal PPC/IPs lesions are not related to neglect, we expected that dorsal PPC stimulation would affect voluntary orienting task components to directional cues more than other attentional components tested through neutral cue trials.
We considered two possibilities to explain the conflicting results on the role of dorsal PPC in voluntary spatial orienting. One possible hypothesis is that the dorsal PPC is critically implicated in voluntary spatial orienting, in line with previous neuroimaging studies, and that the lack of deficits in this function in patients with PPC damage is due to advanced functional reorganization at the time of testing. If so, one would expect that right parietal rTMS in healthy subjects would disrupt spatial orienting, because the acute TMS-effects largely avoid the possibility of reorganization (Pascual-Leone et al., 1999). More precisely, the advantage for detecting targets at cued versus uncued locations (Posner et al., 1984
) should collapse for left cue conditions, as subjects would be expected to be impaired in voluntarily directing their attention to the left hemifield, contralateral to rTMS. Conversely, one might expect an accentuation of this advantage for right cue conditions, as voluntary orienting to the right (ipsilateral) hemifield might be enhanced. An alternative hypothesis is that the dorsal PPC has a subsidiary rather than a pivotal role in voluntary spatial orienting, hence leading to significant activation of this area during functional imaging, but only to discrete behavioral changes following lesions. In this case, subjects might still benefit from endogenous information to direct visual attention in space, both following PPC damage and in spite of parietal TMS. However, interference with a subsidiary node of the attention-directing network via right parietal TMS would be expected to increase the difficulty of performing the voluntary orienting task, especially in regards to leftward orienting, and should thereby lead to discrete but detectable changes in behavior. Increased difficulty performing voluntary attention shifts to the left has been observed in patients with right parietal damage, which translated into slowing of leftward orienting, although benefits at cued location following completion of the attention shifts were comparable in these patients and a healthy control group (Bartolomeo et al., 2001
).
![]() |
Materials and Methods |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Ten healthy subjects (five women, five men) aged 2136 years (mean 26 years) participated in the study. Nine were right-handed and one ambidextrous according to the Oldfield Handedness Questionnaire. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to participation in the study, which had been approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center.
Experimental Conditions, Stimuli and Paradigm
The paradigm was a cued target detection task. We used three different cueing conditions followed by three different target conditions, giving rise to nine cuetarget combinations. Cues could be either directional [cueing of visuospatial attention to one of two designated positions in either the left visual field (LVF) or the right visual field (RVF)] or neutral (no spatial cueing). The target appeared subsequently at one of the two designated positions, or bilaterally. Following directional cues, the probability of targets appearing at cued locations was 0.66 (cue L/target L and cue R/target R respectively), and the probability of targets appearing at uncued locations 0.33 (cue L/target R or target B, cue R/target L or target B). Following neutral cues, targets appeared at left or right location markers with equal probability (0.5) but the proportion of targets appearing bilaterally versus unilaterally was 2/1. In order to facilitate covert shifting and maintaining of spatial attention at cued locations, possible target positions were continuously marked by two gray squares. Figure 1 illustrates stimulus configurations, paradigm and the time course of events within a single trial.
|
A central fixation cross (0.5° of visual angle) and the gray squares (3° x 3° of visual angle) remained on the screen throughout testing (Fig. 1). The gray squares (red:green:blue phosphors = 155:155:155) were centered on the two possible target positions. The position and distance of the participant's eyes with respect to the screen was stabilized by a head and chin rest (28 cm distance).
Participants were asked to fixate the central cross and to avoid eye movements and saccades at any time point throughout testing, including the period after target presentation. Fixation was monitored via electrooculogram recordings (two bipolar derivations for monitoring horizontal and vertical eye movements respectively). After extensive training (see Procedure), all subjects were able to perform attention shifts without moving their eyes such that none of the trials had to be eliminated.
Auditory Warning Stimulus and Cue
An auditory warning signal consisting of a burst of white noise lasting 14001750 ms was presented at the beginning of each trial (Fig. 1). The warning signal was followed by the cue, which was also auditory in nature. The cue consisted of a brief sine-wave tone of 50 ms duration that was presented either at 100, 300 or 800 Hz frequency (n = 3 cueing conditions). The warning signal was delivered at 30% of the auditory cue volume; the latter was adjusted by the subjects to the most comfortable intensity level at the beginning of the experiment. Auditory warning signal and cues were both delivered via earphones to both ears.
The burst of white noise indicated the beginning of a new trial. Upon presentation of this auditory warning signal, subjects were instructed to discontinue performance in the previous trial, to resume baseline position (attention on fixation cross) and to prepare for the upcoming auditory cue. The 100 and 800 Hz tones (directional cues) prompted the subjects to covertly shift to and then to maintain visual attention at the designated target positions in the LVF or RVF respectively. The 300 Hz tone (neutral cue) prompted the subjects to covertly attend to both positions simultaneously. The cuetarget interval (Fig. 1, delay period) was of relatively long duration (2560 ms) in order to provide sufficient time for cue recognition, disengagement from the auditory mode and voluntary shifts of visual attention without eye movements, if required by a directional cue.
Visual Targets
Visual targets were small black rectangles whose sizes were individually adjusted to peri-threshold levels (on average 0.11° x 0.14° of visual angle, see Procedure). They were flashed for 40 ms in either the lower LVF, the lower RVF, or bilaterally (Fig. 1) in the center of the gray squares (n = 3 target conditions). Target positions corresponded to 8° of vertical and 26.5° of horizontal eccentricity from the central fixation cross. Lower visual field targets were chosen because attentional resolution is greater in the lower than the upper visual field (He et al., 1996; Intriligator and Cavanagh, 2001
, Mounts and Gavett, 2004
), and because left visual hemispatial neglect is more severe in the lower as compared to the upper visual field (e.g. Halligan and Marshall, 1989
; Ladavas et al., 1994
; Pitzalis et al., 1997
).
Subjects were required to respond with either index, middle or ring finger of the right hand (buttons b, n, m of keyboard) according to perceived target locations (left, bilateral, right). Participants were explicitly told that targets would be presented at peri-threshold size such that they would perceive only 50% of the stimuli, to respond following perception of a target only, and to withhold responses or guesses if no target was detected. Independent of whether targets were perceived or missed, subjects were asked to resume baseline position (attention on fixation cross, see above) promptly following the beginning of a new trial, which was signaled by the auditory warning stimulus.
Procedure
The experiment was performed over two successive days. On the first day, subjects completed a training session that also served for target titration but did not include TMS. The experimental session took place on the second day.
Target Titration and Training Session
Subjects took part in a training session that served to familiarize them with the task and to determine the optimal target size for each subject separately via target titration. We aimed to present targets at peri-threshold size in order to avoid floor or ceiling effects. Subjects performed the target detection task in three runs of 120 trials each, in which targets of five different sizes were presented [dimensions of targets in pixels: 1 x 2, 2 x 2, 2 x 3, 3 x 3 or 3 x 4, pixel size 0.2 mm x
0.2 mm (
0.05° x
0.05°), longer axis horizontal if target rectangular]. Target dimensions were adapted after the first run if the largest target was not perceived in at least 85% of trials. After each run, subjects were given feedback. Each run lasted
10 min. Out of the five targets, two adjacent peri-threshold targets were selected per subject for presentation during the following day.
Experimental Session
Subjects performed the target detection task in a total of 288 trials over 30 min, both prior to and immediately following a single, continuous, 25 min train of 1 Hz rTMS to the right parietal cortex. Performance was interspersed with regular 1 min breaks. The experiment lasted 1 h 30 min (30 min task, 25 min rTMS, 30 min task). The design gave rise to 96 trials per condition if collapsed over cue or target location conditions (left, bilateral, right) and allowed the time-course of rTMS effects to be investigated.
TMS Protocol
TMS pulses were applied in trains in an offline rTMS protocol at stimulation parameters shown to reduce visual (Boroojerdi et al., 2000) and motor cortex excitability (e.g. Chen et al., 1997
; Muellbacher et al., 2000
; Touge et al., 2001
; Romero et al., 2002
) as well as to disrupt cognitive brain function (e.g. Kosslyn et al., 1999
; Hilgetag et al., 2001
; Robertson et al., 2001
; Shapiro et al., 2001
; Mottaghy et al., 2002
; Sack et al., 2002
) beyond the period of rTMS itself. Accordingly, TMS-effects were assessed by comparing task performance prior (baseline) and following rTMS (pre versus post).
rTMS Frequency, Duration and Apparatus
A single, continuous train of rTMS was administered for 25 min at 1 Hz using a 70 mm figure-of-eight coil and a Magstim Super Rapid Transcranial Magnetic Stimulator (Magstim Company, Dyfed, UK).
rTMS Site
The stimulation site was defined for each subject individually within the 1020 electroencephalogram (EEG) coordinate system and corresponded to position P4, localized over right parietal cortex. The anatomical site of stimulation was determined offline in three subjects using their magnetic resonance images and optical tracking via a frameless stereotaxic system (Brainsight, Rogue Research, Montreal). TMS sites were localized to areas over right IPs as illustrated in Figure 2.
|
We chose to set TMS intensity to a fixed level for all subjects (80% maximum stimulator output), instead of tuning to individual phosphene or motor thresholds, as there is no evidence that phosphene or motor thresholds correlate with the effects of rTMS outside visual and motor cortex respectively (Stewart et al., 2001; Boroojerdi et al., 2002
; Robertson et al., 2003
). In our subjects, 80% rTMS intensity corresponded on average to 129% of resting motor threshold of the right hand (range 107145%).
Analysis
Data were subjected to overall repeated-measure ANOVAs with the factors TMS (Pre versus Post), Cue (L, R, Neutral) and Target Position (L, B, R). In order to assess the time-course of TMS effects and not to miss potential TMS effects of short duration (<30 min), data were broken down into three blocks covering a period of 10 min each. This gave rise to a fourth within-subject factor: Time (1st, 2nd, 3rd block). Statistically significant main effects or interactions were further explored by conducting simple effects. We chose 10 min epochs for evaluating the time-course of TMS effects because previous 1 Hz rTMS studies have found that offline rTMS-effects on cognition outlast the magnetic stimulation by at least half of its duration (reviewed by Robertson et al., 2003
). With 25 min of magnetic stimulation, 10 min epochs thus minimize the risk for missing short-term offline effects and still lead to a satisfactory number of repetitions per condition in each block, as opposed to shorter periods.
Although target detection rate was the primary (dependent) variable of interest, reaction times are also reported. Detection rates reflect a perception measure that is influenced by the allocation of attention to spatial positions through cueing, as is reaction time (RT). Due to the design of our task, however, RT might have been influenced by additional motor factors. Because subjects responded with one of three designated fingers as a function of perceived target configuration (L, B or R) and because targets were more likely to appear at cued locations, some effects on RT may have been influenced by motor preparation for the most probable response (effects of Cue x Target) and by dexterity differences between the three fingers (effects of Target).
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Detection rate during the last run of the training session is illustrated in Figure 3A for left, bilateral and right targets respectively, as well as for the three cue conditions and five target sizes (mean titration curves over all subjects). The mean titration curves have been calculated after aligning individual curves to the two adjacent targets (targets 1 and 2) that were selected for the experimental runs. Targets 1 and 2 were correctly detected with mean frequencies of 0.30 and 0.66 respectively. Frequency of target detection significantly increased with target size from a detection rate of near 0 (sub-threshold targets) to near 1 (supra-threshold targets) [F(5,45) = 31.8, P < 0.0001, see Fig. 3A line drawings]. This shows that, as instructed, subjects responded only when they truly perceived the targets and did not attempt to guess if targets were not detected. Guessing through anticipation of target onset and position, which would have been possible because of the constant cuetarget interval and probabilistic cuetarget relationship inherent in our design, would be associated with relatively high detection rates (at least 0.33) for sub-threshold targets, which was clearly not the case (see Fig. 3A, line drawings). Figure 3B, illustrating false alarm rates at cued positions for each of the three target conditions, further emphasizes that subjects did not guess when sub-threshold targets were presented. If subjects were guessing, the false alarm rate would be expected to be much higher than zero for sub-threshold targets, especially for the illustrated false alarms at cued positions, because of the probabilistic cuetarget relationship. However, this was clearly not the case as the false alarm rate in sub-threshold trials was near zero in all conditions (Fig. 3B).
|
Both the advantage for target detection at cued versus uncued location (Fig. 3A, left and right panels), as well as the increased number of false unilateral alarms in bilateral target trials following directional cueing (Fig. 3B, middle panel), strongly suggest that subjects performed the spatial orienting task correctly. In other words, the results indicate that the advantage for target detection at cued locations as well as the high number of false unilateral alarms in bilateral target trials at cued positions are a consequence of spatial orienting components of the task, in the absence of any evidence for confounding effects due to guessing of target onset or position through anticipation.
Finally, note that subjects showed an advantage for detecting left targets over right targets [F(1,9) = 5.4, P = 0.046; Fig. 3A] and produced more false left- than right-target alarms in bilateral target trials following left or right cueing respectively (Fig. 3B, middle panel). An identical leftward bias has been reported previously by Hilgetag et al. (2001), who tested healthy subjects in a similar target detection paradigm at peri-threshold levels. This left bias is consistent with pseudoneglect, the natural tendency of neurologically normal subjects to attend more easily to the left than to the right visual hemifield (e.g. Jewell and McCourt, 2000
; Fierro et al., 2000
, 2001
; Bjoertomt et al., 2002
; Brighina et al., 2002
).
Experimental Session: rTMS-independent Effects
The proportion of correctly detected targets (detection rate, Fig. 4A) for the different target locations was influenced by cue [overall ANOVA: interactions Cue x Target: F(4,36) = 10.3, P < 0.0001], independently of TMS (TMS x Cue x Target: F < 1, n.s.). Subjects showed an advantage for detection of left targets (Fig. 4A, left box plots) following left as compared to neutral [F(1,9) = 4.2, P = 0.07] or right cueing [F(1,9) = 7.3, P = 0.023]. Right-target hits (Fig. 4A, right box plots) were significantly increased after right as compared to neutral [F(1,9) = 17.5, P = 0.002] or left cueing [F(1,9) = 20.2, P = 0.0015]. Finally, detection rate was increased in bilateral target conditions (Fig. 4A, middle box plots) following neutral with respect to left [F(1,9) = 14.5, P = 0.004] or right cueing [F(1,9) = 11.3, P = 0.008]. As in the training session, the reduced detection rate for bilateral targets following left or right cueing relative to neutral cueing was explained by a high proportion of false unilateral alarms at cued positions (Fig. 4B, middle box plots, one sample t-tests versus zero: L false alarms: t = 6.2, P < 0.001/R false alarms: t = 5.7, P < 0.001), again independent of TMS (no significant effect of TMS). These data reproduce the results of the training session and strongly suggest that subjects performed the required task, including attention shifts to cued locations, correctly.
|
In analogy to detection rate, reaction times to the different target locations depended on cue [overall ANOVA: interactions Cue x Target: F(4,36) = 27.2, P < 0.0001], irrespective of TMS (three-way interaction TMS x Cue x Target: F < 1, n.s.). Participants responded faster to left targets following left (466 ± 20 ms) than neutral [571 ± 30 ms; F(1,9) = 21.4, P = 0.001] or right cueing [585 ± 33 ms; F(1,9) = 24.6, P < 0.001]. Similarly, participants responded faster to right targets following right (496 ± 23 ms) than after neutral [603 ± 30 ms; F(1,9) = 27.3, P < 0.001] or left cueing [648 ± 32 ms; F(1,9) = 26.4, P < 0.001]. Finally, responses to bilateral targets were faster after neutral (512 ± 17 ms) than left [613 ± 27 ms; F(1,9) = 50.4, P < 0.0001] or right cueing [622 ± 23 ms; F(1,9) = 61.4, P < 0.0001]. Subjects also showed a RT advantage for left over right targets [F(1,9) = 8.2, P = 0.02].
Experimental Session: Effects of Right Posterior Parietal rTMS
While rTMS did not affect the advantage for target detection at cued location (Cue x Target interaction independent of TMS, see above), the overall ANOVA on detection rate revealed a TMS-effect depending on cue [interaction TMS x Cue: F(2,18) = 3.8, P = 0.04; see Fig. 5]. As compared to baseline (Pre rTMS; see Fig. 5B for difference plots), the number of correctly detected targets was significantly decreased following rTMS in conditions where cues instructed voluntary leftward orienting [F(1,9) = 12.8, P = 0.006]. The decrease in detection rate following left cues was in the order of 10% (Fig. 5B, Cue L) and was independent of target position (simple test: TMS x Target; F < 1, n.s.). This global rTMS effect was not observed for right [F(1,9) = 2.2, P = 0.17, Fig. 5B, Cue R] or neutral cue conditions [F(1,9) = 1.5, P = 0.25, Fig. 5B, Cue N].
|
Repetitive, parietal TMS had no effect on reaction times as the overall ANOVA on RT showed neither significant main effects nor interactions of factor TMS (all F < 1, n.s.).
Experimental Session: Time-dependency of Right Parietal rTMS-effects
Evaluation of TMS-effects as a function of time after TMS showed that only in one out of the nine conditions did rTMS induce effects that changed significantly over the 30 min testing period, as revealed by TMS x Time interactions. The overall ANOVA on detection rate revealed a significant four-way interaction TMS x Cue x Target x Time [F(8,72) = 2.3, P = 0.03]. Breaking down this interaction showed a significant three-way interaction TMS x Target x Time for the right-cue condition only [F(4,36) = 3.7, P = 0.01], as well as a significant two-way interaction TMS x Time for the right-cue/right-target condition only [F(2,18) = 3.9, P = 0.038]. In this condition (cue R/Target R, Fig. 6A), participants showed an advantage for target detection (increased detection rate) following rTMS as compared with baseline. This effect was observed in the first block after rTMS [F(1,9) = 5.4, P = 0.04] but was washed out in the two subsequent blocks [block 2: F < 1, P = n.s.; block 3: F(1,9) = 2.7, P = 0.14]. The increase in detection rate was paralleled by an advantage in terms of reaction time (decreased RT, Fig. 6B), again only present during the first 10 min immediately after rTMS [block 1: F(1,9) = 5.3, P = 0.047; blocks 2 and 3: F < 1, n.s.]. It might be argued that comparison between the first pre-TMS block and the first post-TMS block might isolate a practice effect. However, there was no significant performance change across the pre-TMS blocks in terms of either detection rate [F(2,18) < 1, n.s.] or reaction time [F(2,18) = 2.4, n.s.]. Moreover, the performance enhancement during the first 10 min following rTMS was also significant when values of the first post-rTMS block were compared with the average of all baseline trials [block 1: F(1,9) = 5.7, P = 0.041 for detection rate, F(1,9) = 4.6, P = 0.06 for reaction time, block 2 and 3: n.s.]. Altogether, both detection rate and reaction time data point toward a TMS-induced enhancement of target detection in the right visual field after rightward cueing that is observed in the first 10 min following rTMS.
|
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
PPC and Voluntary Spatial Orienting
Our results are in agreement with previous neuroimaging studies in healthy subjects revealing PPC activation during voluntary attention shifts (Kastner et al., 1999; Corbetta et al., 2000
; Hopfinger et al., 2000
). These studies identified the dorsal PPC (along the IPs) to be involved in the voluntary control of visuospatial attention through the isolation of cue-related signals from target-related activity (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002
). In the present study, we show that leftward spatial orienting under endogenous control is affected by right posterior parietal TMS (over P4). Perceptual deficits similar to hemispatial neglect can be induced by TMS over both P4 (Pascual-Leone et al., 1994
; Hilgetag et al., 2001
; Müri et al., 2002
) as well as P6 (Fierro et al., 2000
, 2001
; Brighina et al., 2002
), with P4 being the more dorsal parietal position. Individual MRI scans co-registered with scalp coordinates indeed suggest dorsal PPC areas surrounding the IPs as our most probable target site (Fig. 2).
In applying TMS over the right dorsal PPC, we interfered with spatial orienting to the left (cue L trials), but also facilitated rightward orienting (cue R/target R trials). These findings replicate and further clarify the results of Hilgetag et al. (2001) who reported TMS-induced attentional deficits contralateral to magnetic stimulation (P3/P4) as well as ipsilateral enhancement in a target detection task not isolating spatial orienting from other components of attentional space representation. Our results thus extend those of Hilgetag et al. (2001)
in suggesting that rTMS over P4 predominantly affects spatial orienting. In analogy to the present study, Hilgetag et al. (2001)
applied rTMS at 1 Hz frequency and over P4 and investigated offline rTMS effects on target detection at peri-threshold levels. Hilgetag et al. used shorter rTMS trains (10 min) at lesser intensity (90% motor threshold) than employed in the present study. Despite these differences, maximum rTMS-effects on detection rate are of similar magnitude in the two studies (10% change from baseline). The duration of effects cannot be compared, as Hilgetag et al. discontinued the behavioral task after 5 min of testing and did not assess the timing of their rTMS-effects. As to effect-size and -duration, comparisons with other parietal TMS studies on spatial attention are difficult because these previous experiments applied TMS either online to task performance (Pascual-Leone et al., 1994
; Walsh et al., 1999
; Fierro et al., 2000
, 2001
; Oliveri et al., 2001
; Pourtois et al., 2001
; Bjoertomt et al., 2002
; Brighina et al., 2002
; Müri et al., 2002
) or investigated the offline effects of 1 Hz rTMS in neglect patients (e.g. Brighina et al., 2003
). Previously reported effects on other cognitive functions following 1 Hz rTMS in healthy subjects (515 min application, 60130% motor threshold; for review, see Robertson et al., 2003
) range in magnitude between 10% and 20% changes from baseline (e.g. Kosslyn et al., 1999
; Robertson et al., 2001
; Mottaghy et al., 2002
; Sack et al., 2002
) and outlast the rTMS stimulation by at least half the duration of the stimulation train itself (for review, see Robertson et al., 2003
). Our results are in line with these findings in the literature regarding effect size and duration of the rTMS-effects.
Behaviorally similar phenomena with contralesional impairment and ipsilesional hyperattention have also been demonstrated in patients with parietal damage (Smania et al., 1998). The pattern of rTMS-effects we observed is consistent with the idea that some aspects of neglect might be due to a disinhibition of attention processors within the unimpaired (left) hemisphere caused by damage to structures with equivalent function in the opposite (right) hemisphere (interhemispheric competition model: Kinsbourne 1977
, 1987
). According to this model of neglect, the net attention vector that results from interhemispheric competition is tilted away from the impaired (left) hemifield towards the intact (right) hemifield predicting functional inhibition and release. In our case, the net attentional bias would result from dynamic rather than static attention vectors corresponding to the ability to shift attention left- or rightwards respectively. A similar phenomenology can also be explained by changes in rotation of attention space around the vertical body axis through altered transformation of multisensory afferent inputs into a body-centered reference frame (Karnath, 1997
), particularly with respect to target detection at the eccentricity studied by our design, i.e.
25° (Karnath, 1997
, Karnath et al., 1998
). Hence, our data cannot discriminate between Kinsbourne's and Karnath's model of neglect.
Although performance during leftward orienting was impaired by rTMS, subjects showed a preserved advantage for target detection at cued versus uncued locations. This indicates that rTMS interfered with but did not completely disrupt spatial orienting. Even patients with large parietal lesions can make use of cue information to voluntarily direct attention towards spatial locations independent of visual field, suggesting that these patients are relatively unimpaired regarding this function (Posner et al., 1984; Friedrich et al., 1998
; Smania et al., 1998
). Patients with parietal damage show a gradual increase in the advantage for target detection at cued location with prolonged cuetarget interval (as the focus of attention is converging gradually towards target location), which is symmetrical for both right- and leftward cueing conditions (Posner et al., 1984
). Furthermore, the advantage for target detection increases when the predictability of the side of target presentation is raised and the patients rely more on endogenously controlled attention shifts, an effect that is comparable between the two visual fields (Smania et al., 1998
) even for patients with dorsal PPC lesions (Friedrich et al., 1998
). Finally, free, exploratory saccades under voluntary control do not show any direction-specific deficit in patients with parietal lesion, in contrast to reflexive saccades that are driven by a visual target, i.e. that are not under endogenous control (Niemeier and Karnath, 2000
, 2003
). This is of interest as psychological and neuroimaging studies indicate that covert attention shifts and eye movements (i.e. overt visuospatial orienting) are tightly linked (e.g. Corbetta, 1998
). Taken together, our findings are thus in line with both the neuroimaging studies suggesting PPC involvement in voluntary covert spatial orienting, as well as the clinical evidence that this function is relatively preserved following PPC damage. Our data reconcile these seemingly contradictory results in suggesting that the right PPC is involved, but does not play a pivotal role, in spatial orienting under voluntary control. One possible explanation for the relative preservation is the bilateral organization of the dorsal parts of the frontoparietal network for spatial orienting, which consist of the frontal eye field (FEF) and the IPs (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002
). This bilateral organization might facilitate functional reorganization or compensation following unilateral damage/interference. Another possibility is that there might be a functional specialization within this dorsal network, for instance for displacement of the attention focus (FEF) and the online integration of multisensory afferent inputs to map spatial coordinates for covert orienting (IPs).
PPC and Disengagement of Attention
Finally, we found a rTMS-induced impairment of target detection when participants had to reorient from a right focus of attention to the left (invalid trials, cue R/target L). This is the condition in which patients with right parietal damage show disproportional deficits, interpreted to reflect a difficulty in the disengagement of attention in contralesional direction (Posner et al., 1984, 1987
; Friedrich et al., 1998
; Losier and Klein, 2001
). The difficulty is also regarded as being extinction-like as it occurs only for contralesional targets when an event captures attention in the ipsilesional visual field (Posner et al., 1984
; Friedrich et al., 1998
). However, the finding is generally associated with lesions in the ventral part of the PPC (TPJ) only (Friedrich et al., 1998
). Similarly, stronger TPJ responses are evoked by invalid than valid cuetarget trials as shown by functional magnetic resonance imaging, suggesting that the TPJ is preferentially involved in attentional disengagement and/or reorienting towards unattended locations, while the IPs does not show such differential responses (Corbetta et al., 2000
). Hence, rTMS over our target region surrounding the right IPs (dorsal PPC) would not be expected to affect attentional disengagement processes directly. We thus interpret the rTMS-induced deficit in left-target detection following right cueing to be a consequence of facilitated rightward attention shifts evidenced in the cue R/target R condition, rather than to reflect a disengagement deficit. However, distant rTMS-effects on the TPJ through anatomical connections between the dorsal and ventral parts of the PPC or through direct spread of the rTMS impact cannot be ruled out completely. A result speaking against distant effects on TPJ function, however, is our finding that bilateral target detection was not affected by TMS, thus showing absent left visual extinction with simultaneous, double stimulation in both visual fields. This is relevant because visual extinction has been attributed to circumscribed TPJ damage (Karnath et al., 2003
). In summary, we conclude that our data set is best explained by rTMS interference with voluntary spatial orienting per se.
Concluding Remarks
Although current models implicate the right temporo-parietal junction and/or the right superior and middle temporal gyrus in visuospatial neglect or extinction (e.g. Vallar, 2001; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002
; Karnath et al., 2001
, 2003
), our data suggest that neglect-like phenomena with attentional inhibition and release occurring contra- and ipsi-lesionally can also be induced through rTMS over regions surrounding the right intraparietal sulcus. These phenomena concern the ability to direct attention left- or rightwards respectively. This is consistent with the view that dysfunction of the dorsal, frontoparietal network for attention (FEF-IPs) might possibly contribute to some aspects of neglect (Perry and Zeki, 2000
; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002
). It also provides further support to the findings that spatial attention biases can be modulated through rTMS, which has been proven useful to transiently reduce visual hemispatial neglect (Oliveri et al., 2001
; Brighina et al., 2003
) and suggests dorsal PPC as a possible candidate for stimulation.
![]() |
Acknowledgments |
---|
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Bartolomeo P, Sieroff E, Decaix C, Chokron S (2001) Modulating the attentional bias in unilateral neglect: the effects of the strategic set. Exp Brain Res 137:432444.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Bjoertomt O, Cowey A, Walsh V (2002) Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain 125:20122022.
Boroojerdi B, Prager A, Muellbacher W, Cohen LG (2000) Reduction of human visual cortex excitability using 1-Hz transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 54:15291531.
Boroojerdi B, Meister IG, Foltys H, Sparing R, Cohen LG, Topper R (2002) Visual and motor cortex excitability: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Clin Neurophysiol 113:15011504.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Brighina F, Bisiach E, Piazza A, Oliveri M, La Bua V, Daniele O, Fierro B (2002) Perceptual and response bias in visuospatial neglect due to frontal and parietal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in normal subjects. Neuroreport 13:25712575.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Brighina F, Bisiach E, Oliveri M, Piazza A, La Bua V, Daniele O, Fierro B (2003) 1 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the unaffected hemisphere ameliorates contralesional visuospatial neglect in humans. Neurosci Lett 336:131133.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Chen R, Classen J, Gerloff C, Celnik P, Wassermann EM, Hallett M, Cohen LG (1997) Depression of motor cortex excitability by low-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 48:13981403.[Abstract]
Corbetta M (1998) Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:831838.
Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:201215.[ISI][Medline]
Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Shulman GL, Petersen SE (1993) A PET study of visuospatial attention. J Neurosci 13:12021226.[Abstract]
Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Ollinger JM, McAvoy MP, Shulman GL (2000) Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 3:292297.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Fierro B, Brighina F, Oliveri M, Piazza A, La Bua V, Buffa D, Bisiach E (2000) Contralateral neglect induced by right posterior parietal rTMS in healthy subjects. Neuroreport 11:15191521.[ISI][Medline]
Fierro B, Brighina F, Piazza A, Oliveri M, Bisiach E (2001) Timing of right parietal and frontal cortex activity in visuo-spatial perception: a TMS study in normal individuals. Neuroreport 12:26052607.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Friedrich FJ, Egly R, Rafal RD, Beck D (1998) Spatial attention deficits in humans: a comparison of superior parietal and temporal-parietal junction lesions. Neuropsychology 12:193207.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Halligan PW, Marshall JC (1989) Is neglect (only) lateral? A quadrant analysis of line cancellation. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 11:793798.[ISI][Medline]
Halligan PW, Fink GR, Marshall JC, Vallar G (2003) Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect. Trends Cogn Sci 7:125133.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
He S, Cavanagh P, Intriligator J (1996) Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness. Nature 383:334337.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Hilgetag CC, Theoret H, Pascual-Leone A (2001) Enhanced visual spatial attention ipsilateral to rTMS-induced virtual lesions of human parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 4:953957.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Hopfinger JB, Buonocore MH, Mangun GR (2000) The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nat Neurosci 3:284291.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Husain M, Rorden C (2003) Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:2636.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Intriligator J, Cavanagh P (2001) The spatial resolution of visual attention. Cogn Psychol 43:171216.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Jewell G, McCourt ME (2000) Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia 38:93110.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Kanwisher N, Wojciulik E (2000) Visual attention: insights from brain imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:91100.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Karnath HO (1997) Spatial orientation and the representation of space with parietal lobe lesions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 352:14111419.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Karnath HO, Niemeier M, Dichgans J (1998) Space exploration in neglect. Brain 121:23572367.[Abstract]
Karnath HO, Ferber S, Himmelbach M (2001) Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe. Nature 411:950953.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Karnath HO, Himmelbach M, Kuker W (2003) The cortical substrate of visual extinction. Neuroreport 14:437442.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Kastner S, Pinsk MA, De Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG (1999) Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron 22:751761.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Kinsbourne M (1977) Hemi-neglect and hemisphere rivalry. Adv Neurol 18:4149.[Medline]
Kinsbourne M (1987) Mechanisms of unilateral neglect. In: Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect (Jeannerod M, ed.), pp. 6986. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Kosslyn SM, Pascual-Leone A, Felician O, Camposano S, Keenan JP, Thompson WL, Ganis G, Sukel KE, Alpert NM (1999) The role of area 17 in visual imagery: convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. Science 284:167170.
Ladavas E, Carletti M, Gori G (1994) Automatic and voluntary orienting of attention in patients with visual neglect: horizontal and vertical dimensions. Neuropsychologia 32:11951208.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Losier BJ, Klein RM (2001) A review of the evidence for a disengage deficit following parietal lobe damage. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25:113.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Mesulam MM (1981) A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Ann Neurol 10:309325.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Mottaghy FM, Gangitano M, Sparing R, Krause BJ, Pascual-Leone A (2002) Segregation of areas related to visual working memory in the prefrontal cortex revealed by rTMS. Cereb Cortex 12:369375.
Mounts JRW, Gavett BE (2004) The role of salience in localized attentional interference. Vision Res, in press.
Müri RM, Buhler R, Heinemann D, Mosimann UP, Felblinger J, Schlaepfer TE, Hess CW (2002) Hemispheric asymmetry in visuospatial attention assessed with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Exp Brain Res 143:426430.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Muellbacher W, Ziemann U, Boroojerdi B, Hallett M (2000) Effects of low-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation on motor excitability and basic motor behavior. Clin Neurphysiol 111:10021007.[CrossRef]
Niemeier M, Karnath HO (2000) Exploratory saccades show no direction-specific deficit in neglect. Neurology 54:515518.
Niemeier M, Karnath HO (2003) Stimulus-driven and voluntary saccades are coded in different coordinate systems. Curr Biol 13:585589.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Nobre AC, Sebestyen GN, Gitelman DR, Mesulam MM, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD (1997) Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. Brain 120:515533.[Abstract]
Oliveri M, Bisiach E, Brighina F, Piazza A, La Bua V, Buffa D, Fierro B (2001) rTMS of the unaffected hemisphere transiently reduces contralesional visuospatial hemineglect. Neurology 57:13381340.
Pascual-Leone A, Gomez-Tortosa E, Grafman J, Alway D, Nichelli P, Hallett M (1994) Induction of visual extinction by rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation of parietal lobe. Neurology 44:494498.[Abstract]
Pascual-Leone A, Bartres-Faz D, Keenan JP (1999) Transcranial magnetic stimulation: studying the brain-behaviour relationship by induction of virtual lesions. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 354:12291238.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Perry RJ, Zeki S (2000) The neurology of saccades and covert shifts in spatial attention: an event-related fMRI study. Brain 123:22732288.
Pitzalis S, Spinelli D, Zoccolotti P (1997) Vertical neglect: behavioral and electrophysiological data. Cortex 33:679688.[ISI][Medline]
Pourtois G, Vandermeeren Y, Olivier E, de Gelder B (2001) Event-related TMS over the right posterior parietal cortex induces ipsilateral visuo-spatial interference. Neuroreport 12:23692374.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FJ, Rafal RD (1984) Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. J Neurosci 4:18631874.[Abstract]
Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FA, Rafal RD (1987) How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? Neuropsychologia 25:135145.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Robertson EM, Tormos JM, Maeda F, Pascual-Leone A (2001) The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during sequence learning is specific for spatial information. Cereb Cortex 11:628635.
Robertson EM, Theoret H, Pascual-Leone A (2003) Studies in cognition: The problems solved and created by transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Cogn Neurosci 15:948960.
Romero JR, Anschel D, Sparing R, Gangitano M, Pascual-Leone A (2002) Subthreshold low frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation selectively decreases facilitation in the motor cortex. Clin Neurophysiol 113:101107.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Sack AT, Sperling JM, Prvulovic D, Formisano E, Goebel R, Di Salle F, Dierks T, Linden DE (2002) Tracking the mind's image in the brain II: transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals parietal asymmetry in visuospatial imagery. Neuron 35:195204.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Shapiro KA, Pascual-Leone A, Mottaghy FM, Gangitano M, Caramazza A (2001) Grammatical distinctions in the left frontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 13:713720.
Smania N, Martini MC, Gambina G, Tomelleri G, Palamara A, Natale E, Marzi CA (1998) The spatial distribution of visual attention in hemineglect and extinction patients. Brain 121:17591770.[Abstract]
Stewart LM, Walsh V, Rothwell JC (2001) Motor and phosphene thresholds: a transcranial magnetic stimulation correlation study. Neuropsychologia 39:415419.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Touge T, Gerschlager W, Brown P, Rothwell JC (2001) Are the after-effects of low-frequency rTMS on motor cortex excitability due to changes in the efficacy of cortical synapses? Clin Neurophysiol 112:21382145.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Vallar G (2001) Extrapersonal visual unilateral spatial neglect and its neuroanatomy. Neuroimage 14:S52-58.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Vallar G, Perani D (1986) The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man. Neuropsychologia 24:609622.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Walsh V, Ellison A, Ashbridge E, Cowey A (1999) The role of the parietal cortex in visual attention hemispheric asymmetries and the effects of learning: a magnetic stimulation study. Neuropsychologia 37:245251.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
|