Laboratory for Neural Modeling, Brain Science Institute, RIKEN, Wako-shi, Saitama 351-01, , 1 Department of Physiology, University of Tokyo School of Medicine, Hongo, Tokyo 113-0033, , 2 Department of Physics, Waseda University, , 3 PRESTO, JST, Japan and , 4 Center for Magnetic Resonance Research, University of Minnesota Medical School, MN 55455, USA
![]() |
Abstract |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Although relationships between pinwheel centers and other functional maps have attracted much attention in recent years (Bartfeld and Grinvald, 1992; Obermayer and Blasdel, 1993
; Das and Gilbert, 1997
; Crair et al., 1997a
,b
; Hubener et al., 1997
), there have been very few reports on their arrangement per se, except some studies reporting no evidence for geometric patterns of pinwheel centers in area 17 proper (Obermayer and Blasdel, 1997
). Since theoretical studies have suggested that the areal border has a boundary effect on the pattern formation of functional maps (Swindale, 1980
; Lowel and Singer, 1990
; Wolf et al., 1996
), some regular structure in the arrangement of pinwheel centers might be expected around the areal border.
Cytoarchitectonically, the area 17/18 border in the cat visual cortex is not a sharp border but a 1- to 1.5-mm-wide transition zone (Otsuka and Hassler, 1962; Payne, 1990
). The transition zone represents a strip of the ipsilateral visual field (Payne, 1990
) and is connected to retinotopically corresponding locations in the areas 17 and 18 in the opposite hemisphere via non-mirror- symmetric callosal connections (Payne, 1991
; Payne and Siwek, 1991
; Olavarria, 1996
). Previous studies about functional organization of the transition zone have examined continuity (Orban et al., 1980
; Lowel and Singer, 1987
; Lowel et al., 1987
; Diao et al., 1990
; Bonhoeffer et al., 1995
) and orthogonality (Lowel and Singer, 1987
; Lowel et al., 1987
; Diao et al., 1990
) of functional maps to the transition zone, while the arrangement of pinwheel centers has not been examined yet around the transition zone with discriminating types of pinwheel centers.
In this study, we focused on examining the arrangement of pinwheel centers across the area 17/18 transition zone. To examine the detailed arrangement, we used optical imaging of intrinsic signals because of its high spatial resolution. Our results demonstrate for the first time that the orientation pinwheel centers are arranged to form a unique geometric pattern around the area 17/18 transition zone in the cat visual cortex.
![]() |
Materials and Methods |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Optical Imaging
Intrinsic optical signals were measured to visualize functional maps across the area 17/18 transition zone using standard techniques (Bonhoeffer and Grinvald, 1996). The cortex was illuminated with a 630 nm light and the focal plane was adjusted at 400600 µm below the cortical surface using a tandem-lens macroscope arrangement (Ratzlaff and Grinvald, 1991
). Images were obtained with a CCD video camera (648 x 480 pixels) and digitized by a differential video-enhancement system, Imager 2001 (Optical Imaging, Germantown, NY).
The animals were stimulated with full-screen, high-contrast, moving square-wave gratings displayed on a 20 inch CRT monitor positioned 30 cm in front of the animal. The gratings consisted of two spatial frequencies (0.15 and 0.5 cycles/degree), six orientations separated by 30° and both directions of motion. The temporal frequency was maintained at 2.0 Hz. The responses to each stimulus were summed between 1.0 and 5.0 s after the onset of stimulus movement, while the stimulus was presented 64160 times in a pseudo-random sequence.
Data Analysis
Data analysis was performed using IDL (Research Systems Inc., Boulder, CO). To obtain a single-condition map, the cortical image obtained for one stimulus was divided by the cocktail blank, the sum of the images obtained for all stimuli and band-pass filtered with a Gaussian kernel of 701000 µm radius. Then all the single-condition maps in response to the six orientations of the gratings were summed vectorially, pixel by pixel. The angle of the resultant vector corresponds to the preferred orientation and the length of the vector (magnitude) corresponds to the orientation- specific signal strength of that pixel. The angle map shows only the preferred orientation, by means of color-coding. The polar map shows the preferred orientation as the hue of the color, and the magnitude as the brightness of the color. The positions of the orientation pinwheel centers were detected automatically and checked visually as the points at which the integral of the orientation differences around a pixel was ± 180° (Crair et al., 1997a). The integral paths were always taken in a counterclockwise direction. The point was defined as a counterclockwise pinwheel center when the integral was +180°, and as a clockwise pinwheel center when the integral was 180°. The stability of pinwheel centers was confirmed by using several different band-pass filters.
Delineation of the Area 17/18 Transition Zone
Based on the property that neurons in area 18 prefer visual stimuli with spatial frequencies that are, on average, one-third as high as those preferred by area 17 neurons (Movshon et al., 1978), the functional area 17/18 transition zone can be visualized in vivo (Bonhoeffer et al., 1995
). Figure 1a
shows a polar map obtained with gratings of a high spatial frequency (0.5 cycles/degree, 2 Hz). The activated region was limited to the posteromedial part of the image, which corresponds to area 17. Figure 1b
shows a polar map on the same cortical surface in response to gratings of a low spatial frequency (0.15 cycles/degree, 2 Hz). Although the whole imaged area was activated, the anterolateral part, which corresponds to area 18, showed stronger response than the posteromedial part. Figure 1c
shows a map depicting the ratio of the response strength recorded with the low spatial frequency stimuli (optimal for area 18) to the response strength recorded with the high spatial frequency stimuli (optimal for area 17). The white and black regions indicate preference to the area 17-optimal and area 18-optimal stimuli, respectively. The spatial frequency preference changes smoothly and rapidly at the ~1-mm-wide narrow band-like gray region between the white and black regions (Bonhoeffer et al., 1995
). We delineated this spatial frequency preference transition zone as where the spatial frequency preference changes rapidly. Briefly, the steepness of the change of spatial frequency preference was defined as the length of the gradient vector of the spatial frequency map after smoothing. The region of high steepness was defined as the spatial frequency transition zone. Because the spatial frequency preference transition zone is known to correlate well with the anatomical area 17/18 transition zone (Orban et al., 1980
; Sheth et al., 1996
), we regarded this region as the area 17/18 transition zone. The transition zone defined from the spatial frequency preferences always ran from the anteromedial to the posterolateral part in the lateral gyrus in all the cats examined, which is consistent with the anatomical and retinotopical area 17/18 transition zone (Otsuka and Hassler, 1962
; Tusa et al., 1978
).
|
|
In order to understand the possible mechanism of arrangement of pin- wheel centers, we conducted simulation study. To construct orientation preference maps, we selected bandpass filter model (Rojer and Schwartz, 1990) because of its simplicity and one of its properties: the nearest neighbor pinwheel centers tend to be of opposite types, that is, clockwise and counterclockwise (Obermayer and Blasdel, 1997
; Tal and Schwartz, 1997
). To overcome an unrealistic property (Erwin et al., 1995
) in orientation preference maps produced by this method, we modified it in the following way. We first prepared a pair of two-dimensional white noises which were both convoluted with two-dimensional bandpass filter repeatedly (typically 20 times). A pair of the yielded two-dimensional patterns were regarded as a pair of single condition maps which represent 0°/90° and 45°/135°. An orientation preference map was then calculated as the pixel-by-pixel vectorial summation of two maps in the same manner as described in Data Analysis.
In this model, near-excitatory far-inhibitory lateral interaction is assumed and it is represented as a band-pass filter (Rojer and Schwartz, 1990):
![]() |
We assumed the direction of the areal border to be horizontal. To realize the orthogonality between iso-orientation contours and the areal border, we assumed anisotropy in the lateral interaction (Swindale, 1980; Rojer and Schwartz, 1990
). This is the only way to introduce orthogonality in this model. It seems reasonable also from the experimental observations as follows: in both models, the autocorrelation function of the orientation preference map takes a shape similar to the lateral interactions. Autocorrelation functions of orientation maps around the area 17/18 transition zone obtained from experiments show anisotropy with the long axis orthogonal to the direction of the transition zone. Therefore, the lateral interaction is also expected to have anisotropy.
We used three kinds of anisotropy: (A) only far-inhibitory interaction is elongated parallel to the border (Ex = 140,
Ey = 140,
Ix = 310,
Iy = 288 µm); (B) both near-excitatory and far-inhibitory interactions are elongated orthogonal to the border (
Ex = 112,
Ey = 224,
Ix = 230,
Iy = 460 µm); and (C) only near-excitatory interaction is elongated orthogonal to the border (
Ex = 140,
Ey = 150,
Ix = 310,
Iy = 310 µm). The shapes of these interactions are illustrated in the top part of Figure 5
. With these parameters, the measured degree of orthogonality was obtained (Fig. 5D
). In the experiments, percentage of opposite-type in the nearest-neighbor pinwheel centers was 69.9 ± 9.1% (n = 5; cats AE; mean ± SD). To incorporate this statistic, simulated results outside of a range of 69.9 ± 9.1% were discarded.
|
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Arrangement of Orientation Pinwheel Centers
To examine the arrangement of orientation pinwheel centers, the positions of clockwise and counterclockwise pinwheel centers in the same animal as in Figure 1 were detected using the algorithm described in Materials and Methods (Fig. 3A,B
). Visual inspection of Figure 3B
reveals that the pinwheel centers are arranged in a unique geometric pattern relative to the area 17/18 transition zone: both the clockwise pinwheel centers (filled circles) and the counterclockwise pinwheel centers (open circles) are aligned parallel to the transition zone. The rows composed of clockwise pinwheel centers and those composed of counterclockwise pinwheel centers are arranged alternately, in the direction orthogonal to the border. Similar patterns of orientation pinwheel centers were also observed in all the other animals.
|
To confirm objectively the results of visual inspection, we evaluated the local arrangement of pinwheel centers quantitatively using principal components analysis.
Initially, to define neighboring pinwheel centers, we used the distribution of distances among all the pairs of pinwheel centers as shown in Figure 3C. The first peak appearing at ~600 µm showed unimodal distribution up to 800 µm. Therefore, when two pinwheel centers were located within a distance of 800 µm from each other, they were defined as neighboring pinwheel centers. Pairs of neighboring pinwheel centers are displayed as line segments in Figure 3D
.
Secondly, the relative positions from any pinwheel centers to their neighboring pinwheel centers are plotted in Figure 3E. Red squares, representing pinwheel pairs of the same type, and blue squares, representing pinwheel pairs of opposite types, are separately clustered. Finally, we applied the principal component analysis to the scattergram of the red and blue squares, respectively. The axis of the first principal component (
1stPC) indicates the direction of the clustering of the data points, and the ratio of the first principal component eigenvalue to the second one (
1/
2) reflects the strength of clustering. Pairs of the same type are strongly clustered around the red line, as shown in Figure 3E
, which nearly coincides with the average direction of the area 17/18 transition zone (|
1stPC
TZ| (same type) = 6°). On the other hand, pairs of opposite types are weakly clustered around the blue line, which is approximately orthogonal to the transition zone (|
1stPC
TZ| (opposite type) = 85°). The eigenvalue ratio is 5.59 for pairs of the same type [
1/
2 (same type) = 5.59] and 2.02 for pairs of opposite types [
1/
2 (opposite type) = 2.02], suggesting that the clustering is stronger for the pairs of the same type. These clusterings indicate that pairs of the same type have a strong tendency to be located parallel to the transition zone, while pairs of opposite types have a weak tendency to be located orthogonal to the transition zone. The observed global arrangement in Figure 3B
was, therefore, confirmed objectively by this local arrangement of pinwheel centers (Fig. 4C
). Principal component analysis was also applied to arrangements of pinwheel centers in the other animals and they showed the same tendency (Table 1
).
|
|
To see the statistical significance of the local arrangement of pinwheel centers as revealed by the principal component analysis, we examined the distribution of angles between the line segments linking pairs of neighboring pinwheel centers (Fig. 3D) and the average direction of the area 17/18 transition zone. The average value of the angles between the line segments linking pairs of the same type and the direction of transition zone (|
same
TZ|) was 19°, and significantly smaller than 45° (|
same
TZ| < 45°; P < 5 x 105; two-tailed Student's t-test), indicating that pinwheel pairs of the same type were likely to be aligned parallel to the direction of the transition zone. On the other hand, the average value of the angles between the line segments linking pinwheel pairs of opposite types and the direction of transition zone (|
opposite
TZ|) was 57°, and significantly larger than 45° (|
opposite
TZ| > 45°; P < 0.002; two-tailed Student's t-test), indicating that pairs of opposite types were likely to be aligned orthogonal to the transition zone. The difference between these two angles was significant (|
same
TZ| < |
opposite
TZ|; P < 2 x 107; two-tailed Welch's t-test). The results of statistical analyses in the five animals are summarized in Table 1
. In all the animals examined in this study, the tendency of pinwheel pairs of the same type to be aligned parallel to the area 17/18 transition zone was statistically significant. This result confirms the existence of similar global arrangements in these animals.To see the statistical significance of the local arrangement of pinwheel centers as revealed by the principal component analysis, we examined the distribution of angles between the line segments linking pairs of neighboring pin- wheel centers (Fig. 3D
) and the average direction of the area 17/18 transition zone. The average value of the angles between the line segments linking pairs of the same type and the direction of transition zone (|
same
TZ|) was 19°, and significantly smaller than 45° (|
same
TZ| < 45°; P < 5 x 105; two-tailed Student's t-test), indicating that pinwheel pairs of the same type were likely to be aligned parallel to the direction of the transition zone. On the other hand, the average value of the angles between the line segments linking pinwheel pairs of opposite types and the direction of transition zone (|
opposite
TZ|) was 57°, and significantly larger than 45° (|
opposite
TZ| > 45°; P < 0.002; two-tailed Student's t-test), indicating that pairs of opposite types were likely to be aligned orthogonal to the transition zone. The difference between these two angles was significant (|
same
TZ| < |
opposite
TZ|; P < 2 x 107; two-tailed Welch's t-test). The results of statistical analyses in the five animals are summarized in Table 1
. In all the animals examined in this study, the tendency of pinwheel pairs of the same type to be aligned parallel to the area 17/18 transition zone was statistically significant. This result confirms the existence of similar global arrangements in these animals.
In one animal, we recorded orientation maps around the 17/18 transition zone and inside area 17 simultaneously (cat E in Table 1). A similar systematic arrangement of pinwheel centers was observed around the area 17/18 transition zone but not inside area 17 which is >1 mm away from the transition zone. In the last animal, we recorded orientation maps inside area 17, and systematic arrangement was also not observed (cats F, G and H in Table 1
). In one animal, we recorded orientation maps around the 17/18 transition zone and inside area 17 simultaneously (cat E in Table 1
). A similar systematic arrangement of pinwheel centers was observed around the area 17/18 transition zone but not inside area 17 which is >1 mm away from the transition zone. In the last animal, we recorded orientation maps inside area 17, and systematic arrangement was also not observed (cats F, G and H in Table 1
).
Orthogonality
Finally, we confirmed previous findings about the area 17/18 transition zone. It has been reported that orientation columns in cat visual cortex tend to run across the transition zone at right angles (Lowel et al., 1987; Diao et al., 1990
). Similarly, in our study, the orthogonality between the iso-orientation contours and the direction of area 17/18 transition zone was observed clearly within the transition zone, and weakly even outside the transition zone (Fig. 4A,B
).
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Relationship to Other Features of Pinwheel Centers
Since the finding of orientation pinwheel centers (Bonhoeffer and Grinvald, 1991), two issues have been clarified about their arrangement, namely (i) pinwheel centers tend to be located in the middle of ocular dominance columns (Bartfeld and Grinvald, 1992
; Obermayer and Blasdel, 1993
; Crair et al., 1997a
; Hubener et al., 1997
) and (ii) the nearest neighbor pinwheel centers tend to be of opposite types (Obermayer and Blasdel, 1997
; Tal and Schwartz, 1997
). Although the former might be thought to influence the positions of pinwheel centers, it does not impose any restrictions on the types of pinwheel centers. When the types of pinwheel centers in Figure 3B
were randomly assigned, the probability of obtaining some systematic arrangement was extremely low (<3% in Monte Carlo simulation). The latter property does not restrict pairs of pinwheel centers in specific directions and will not cause pinwheel pairs of the same type to be aligned parallel to the transition zone. Therefore, it is difficult to explain our finding based only on two known properties of orientation pinwheel centers, both of which are not specific to the border region but are also valid in the area 17 proper. We speculate that some property specific to the border region contributes to the systematic arrangement of pinwheel centers.
Boundary Effect on Pattern Formation
For the organization of functional maps in the cerebral cortex, the importance of activity-dependent refinement of neuronal connections has been pointed out (Wiesel and Hubel, 1965; Hubel et al., 1977
; Stryker and Strickland, 1984
; Stryker and Harris, 1986
; Shatz, 1990
; Weliky and Katz, 1997
; Crair et al., 1998
; Sengpiel et al., 1999
). It has also been considered theoretically that these functional maps are self-organized in an activity-dependent manner during development (Linsker, 1986
; Tanaka, 1990
; Miller, 1994
). In self-organizing pattern formation, boundary conditions play an essential role in determining the final pattern. Therefore, in the visual cortex, the boundary condition imposed by the areal border is expected to be one of the important constraints on the layout of orientation columns (Wolf et al., 1996
). The close relationship between the arrangement of pinwheel centers and the direction of the transition zone in our study supports the idea that the arrangement of pinwheel centers is a consequence of the boundary effect of the area 17/18 transition zone on the pattern formation of orientation maps.
Relationship to Orthogonality
One of characteristic features of functional maps often observed around the areal border is a tendency for columns to run perpendicular to the border. In the primate striate cortex, ocular dominance columns run perpendicular to the V1/V2 border (LeVay et al., 1975), as do the thick, thin and pale stripes in primate V2 (Livingstone and Hubel, 1982
; Tootell et al., 1983
) and the orientation columns in the tree shrew striate cortex (Humphrey et al., 1980
; Bosking et al., 1997
). In areas 17 and 18 in cats (Lowel and Singer, 1987
; Lowel et al., 1987
; Diao et al., 1990
) and ferrets (Law et al., 1988
; Chapman et al., 1996
), both the orientation and ocular dominance columns tend to intersect the area 17/18 border at right angles. Such orthogonality of stripe patterns to the boundary is a general feature of a self- organizing pattern formation (Greenside et al., 1982
) and it has been proposed that these orthogonalities are due to the boundary effect imposed by the areal border (Swindale, 1980
; Lowel and Singer, 1990
).
Our results also indicated that iso-orientation contours tend to run orthogonal to the area 17/18 transition zone (Fig. 4A,B). Since iso-orientation contours link adjacent pairs of clockwise and counterclockwise pinwheel centers (Obermayer and Blasdel, 1993
), there might be a mutual constraint between the direction of iso-orientation contours and the arrangement of pinwheel centers. We examined whether the arrangement of pinwheel centers observed in our study is related to the orthogonality of the iso-orientation contours to the transition zone. As described in Results, the orthogonality is observed both inside and outside the transition zone. To satisfy the orthogonality in the wide areas, the configurations shown in Figure 4C,D
, which are similar to our observations, would be preferable to that shown in Figure 4E
, in which some iso-orientation contours run parallel to the border around the transition zone. Therefore, the observed arrangement of pin- wheel centers seems to be related to the orthogonality between iso-orientation contours and the area 17/18 transition zone, and it is speculated that these two properties might have a common origin, that is, a boundary effect. To clarify their relationship and possible underlying mechanisms, we conducted a simulation study.
Simulation Study
The details of the simulation method is described in Materials and Methods. Briefly, we used a modified bandpass filter model (Rojer and Schwartz, 1990) and Swindale's model (Swindale, 1982
) to construct an orientation preference map. In these models, near-excitatory far-inhibitory lateral interaction is assumed (Rojer and Schwartz, 1990
; Swindale, 1982
). To realize the orthogonality between iso-orientation contours and the areal border, we introduced anisotropy in the lateral interaction (Swindale, 1980
; Rojer and Schwartz, 1990
) in three different ways: (A) only far-inhibitory interaction is elongated parallel to the border; (B) both near-excitatory and far-inhibitory interactions are elongated orthogonal to the border; and (C) only near-excitatory interaction is elongated orthogonal to the border. The shapes of these interactions are illustrated in the top part of Figure 5AC
. We conducted simulations 400 times for each of these interactions, by using the bandpass filter model. Some of the results are illustrated in the bottom part of Figure 5AC
. In all of them, the orthogonality was realized to a degree similar to the experimental results (Fig. 5D
). However, in the case of (B), 95.2% of simulations resulted in no significant tendency in the distribution of pinwheel centers (Fig. 5E
). Therefore, orthogonality does not necessarily lead to the systematic arrangement of pinwheel centers.
On the other hand, in the cases of (A) and (C), pinwheel centers of the same type tend to be aligned parallel to the areal border, in a way similar to that observed in our results. In 72.8 and 69.5% of simulations in the cases of (A) and (C) respectively, pinwheel pairs of the same type tend to be aligned parallel to the areal border. In 26.3 and 22.3% of simulations in the cases of (A) and (C), pinwheel pairs of opposite types also tend to be located orthogonal to the border. These results suggest that what is important for the systematic arrangement of pinwheel centers is difference in the shapes of near-excitatory and far-inhibitory interactions (that is, Ey/
Ex >
Iy/
Ix). If such a difference exists, both orthogonality and the systematic arrangement of pinwheel centers will emerge. As neural correlates, two possibilities can be considered corresponding to (A) and (C): (i) truncation of the lateral inhibitory interaction at the areal border, as suggested by Swindale (1980); and (ii) elongation of lateral excitatory interaction orthogonal to the border. These possibilities cannot be discriminated from this study and further anatomical and physiological studies are required.
In the cases of (A) and (C), while pinwheel pairs of the same type tend to be aligned in a row parallel to the border, the alignment often appears in a single row, and alternating rows of pinwheel centers according to their types were observed infrequently. This suggests that some additional condition has to be considered. In the experimental results, the axes of anisotropy were not totally identical for each iso-orientation domain. The differences of these axes were within a range of 20°. When we incorporated such difference in the axes of anisotropy in the cases of (A) and (C), alternating rows of pinwheel centers according to their types often emerged, and the statistical results were also greatly improved (pinwheel pairs of the same type tend to be aligned parallel to the border in 82.8% of simulations, while pinwheel pairs of opposite types tend to be aligned orthogonal to the border in 59.0% of simulations). We succeeded in reproducing all the results by using Swindale's model (Swindale 1982).
To summarize, our simulation study suggests that the orthogonality between iso-orientation contours and the area 17/18 transition zone does not necessarily lead to the systematic arrangement of pinwheel centers, and that both the orthogonality and the systematic arrangement are caused by the same mechanism.
In conclusion, we suggest that the area 17/18 transition zone imposes a profound restriction on the pattern formation of orientation preference maps: the areal border not only makes iso-orientation contours run orthogonal to the border, but also arranges the orientation pinwheel centers to form a unique geometric pattern.
![]() |
Notes |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Address correspondence to Dr Shigeru Tanaka, Laboratory for Neural Modeling, Brain Science Institute, RIKEN, Wako-shi, Saitama 351-0198, Japan. Email: shigeru{at}postman.riken.go.jp.
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Blasdel GG (1992) Orientation selectivity, preference, and continuity in monkey striate cortex. J Neurosci 12:31393161.[Abstract]
Bonhoeffer T, Grinvald A (1991) Iso-orientation domains in cat visual cortex are arranged in pinwheel-like patterns. Nature 353:429431.[ISI][Medline]
Bonhoeffer T, Grinvald A (1996) Optical imaging based on intrinsic signals: the methodology. In: Brain mapping: the methods (Toga A, Mazziotta JC, eds), pp. 5597. San Diego, CA: Academic.
Bonhoeffer T, Kim DS, Malonek D, Shoham D, Grinvald A (1995) Optical imaging of the layout of functional domains in area 17 and across area 17/18 border in cat visual cortex. Eur J Neurosci 7:19731988.[ISI][Medline]
Bosking WH, Zhang Y, Schofield B, Fitzpatrick D (1997) Orientation selectivity and the arrangement of horizontal connections in tree shrew striate cortex. J Neurosci 17:21122127.
Braitenberg V, Braitenberg C (1979) Geometry of orientation columns in the visual cortex. Biol Cybern 33:179186.[ISI][Medline]
Chapman B, Stryker MP, Bonhoeffer T (1996) Development of orientation preference maps in ferret primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 16: 64436453.
Crair MC, Ruthazer ES, Gillespie DC, Stryker MP (1997a) Ocular dominance peaks at pinwheel center singularities of the orientation map in cat visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 77:33813385.
Crair MC, Ruthazer ES, Gillespie DC, Stryker MP (1997b) Relationship between the ocular dominance and orientation maps in visual cortex of monocularly deprived cats. Neuron 19:307318.[ISI][Medline]
Crair MC, Gillespie DC, Stryker MP (1998) The role of visual experience in the development of columns in cat visual cortex. Science 279: 566570.
Das A, Gilbert CD (1997) Distortions of visuotopic map match orientation singularities in primary visual cortex. Nature 387:594598.[ISI][Medline]
Diao YC, Jia WG, Swindale NV, Cynader MS (1990) Functional organization of the cortical 17/18 border region in the cat. Exp Brain Res 79:271282.[ISI][Medline]
Erwin E, Obermayer K, Schulten K (1995) Models of orientation and ocular dominance columns in the visual cortex: a critical comparison. Neural Computat 7:425468.[Abstract]
Gotz KG (1988) Cortical templates for the self-organization of orientation specific d- and l-hypercolumns in monkeys and cats. Biol Cybern 58:213223.[ISI][Medline]
Greenside HS, Coughran WM Jr, Schryer NL (1982) Nonlinear pattern formation near the onset of RayleighBenard convection. Phys Rev Lett 49:726729.[ISI]
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1962) Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. J Physiol 160: 106154.[ISI][Medline]
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1963) Shape and arrangement of columns in cat's striate cortex. J Physiol 165:559568.[ISI]
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN, LeVay S (1977) Plasticity of ocular dominance columns in the monkey striate cortex. Phil Trans R Soc (Lond) B 278:377409.
Hubener M, Shoham D, Grinvald A, Bonhoeffer T (1997) Spatial relationship among three columnar systems in cat area 17. J Neurosci 17:92709284.
Humphrey AL, Skeen LC, Norton TT (1980) Topographic organization of the orientation column system in the striate cortex of the tree shrew (Tupaia glis). II. Deoxyglucose mapping. J Comp Neurol 192: 544566.
Law MI, Zahs KL, Stryker MP (1988) Organization of primary visual cortex (area 17) in the ferret. J Comp Neurol 278:157180.[ISI][Medline]
LeVay S, Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1975) The pattern of ocular dominance columns in macaque visual cortex revealed by a reduced silver stain. J Comp Neurol 159:559576.[ISI][Medline]
Linsker R (1986) From basic network principles to neural architecture: emergence of orientation columns. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 83: 87798783.[Abstract]
Livingstone MS, Hubel DH (1982) Thalamic inputs to cytochrome oxidase-rich regions in monkey visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 79:60986101.[Abstract]
Lowel S, Singer W (1987) The pattern of ocular dominance columns in flat-mounts of the cat visual cortex. Exp Brain Res 68:661666.[ISI][Medline]
Lowel S, Singer W (1990) Tangential intracortical pathways and the development of iso-orientation bands in cat striate cortex. Dev Brain Res 56:99116.[ISI][Medline]
Lowel S, Freeman B, Singer W (1987) Topographic organization of the orientation column system in large flat-mounts of the cat visual cortex: a 2-deoxyglucose study. J Comp Neurol 255:401415.[ISI][Medline]
Miller KD (1994) A model for the development of simple cell receptive fields and the ordered arrangement of orientation columns through activity-dependent competition between ON- and OFF-center inputs. J Neurosci 14:409441.[Abstract]
Movshon JA, Thompson ID, Tolhurst DJ (1978) Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of neurons in areas 17 and 18 of the cat's visual cortex. J Physiol 283:101120.[Abstract]
Obermayer K, Blasdel GG (1993) Geometry of orientation and ocular dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. J Neurosci 13: 41144129.[Abstract]
Obermayer K, Blasdel GG (1997) Singularities in primate orientation maps. Neural Computat 9:555575.[Abstract]
Olavarria JF (1996) Non-mirror-symmetric patterns of callosal linkages in areas 17 and 18 in cat visual cortex. J Comp Neurol 366:643655.[ISI][Medline]
Orban GA, Kennedy H, Maes H (1980) Functional changes across the 1718 border in the cat. Exp Brain Res 39:177186.[ISI][Medline]
Otsuka R, Hassler R (1962) Uber aufbau und gliderung der corticalen sehsphare bei der katze. Arch Psychiatr Z Ges Neurol 203:212234.
Payne BR (1990) Representation of the ipsilateral visual field in the transition zone between areas 17 and 18 of the cat's visual cortex. Vis Neurosci 4:445474.[ISI][Medline]
Payne BR (1991) Visual-field map in the transcallosal sending zone of area 17 in the cat. Vis Neurosci 7:201219.[ISI][Medline]
Payne BR, Siwek DF (1991) Visual-field map in the callosal recipient zone at border between areas 17 and 18 in the cat. Vis Neurosci 7:221236.[ISI][Medline]
Ratzlaff EH, Grinvald A (1991) A tandem-lens epifluorescence macroscope: hundred-fold brightness advantage for wide-field imaging. J Neurosci Methods 36:127137.[ISI][Medline]
Rojer AS, Schwartz EL (1990) Cat and monkey cortical columnar patterns modeled by bandpass-filtered 2D white noise. Biol Cybernet 62:381391.[ISI][Medline]
Sengpiel F, Stawinski P, Bonhoeffer T (1999) Influence of experience on orientation maps in cat visual cortex. Nature Neurosci 2:727732.[ISI][Medline]
Shatz CJ (1990) Impulse activity and the patterning of connections during CNS development. Neuron 5:745756.[ISI][Medline]
Sheth BR, Sharma J, Rao SC, Sur M (1996) Orientation maps of subjective contours in visual cortex. Science 274:21102115.
Stryker MP, Harris W (1986) Binocular impulse blockade prevents the formation of ocular dominance columns in cat visual cortex. J Neurosci 6:21172133.[Abstract]
Stryker MP, Strickland SL (1984) Physiological segregation of ocular dominance columns depends on the pattern of afferent electrical activity. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci (suppl) 25:278.[Abstract]
Swindale NV (1980) A model for the formation of ocular dominance stripes. Proc R Soc Lond B 208:24364.[ISI][Medline]
Swindale NV (1982) A model for the formation of orientation columns. Proc R Soc Lond B 215:211230.[ISI][Medline]
Swindale NV, Matsubara JA, Cynader MS (1987) Surface organization of orientation and direction selectivity in cat area 18. J Neurosci 7: 14141427.[Abstract]
Tal D, Schwartz EL (1997) Topological singularities in cortical orientation maps: the sign theorem correctly predicts orientation column patterns in primate striate cortex. Network 8:229238.[ISI]
Tanaka S (1990) Theory of self-organization of cortical maps: mathematical framework. Neural Networks 3:62540.[ISI]
Tootell RBH, Silverman MS, DeValois RL, Jacobs GH (1983) Functional organization of the second cortical visual area in primates. Science 220:737739.[ISI][Medline]
Tusa RJ, Palmer LA, Rosenquist AC (1978) The retinotopic organization of area 17 (striate cortex) in the cat. J Comp Neurol 177:213236.[ISI][Medline]
Weliky M, Katz LC (1997) Disruption of orientation tuning in visual cortex by artificially correlated neuronal activity. Nature 386: 680685.[ISI][Medline]
Wiesel TN, Hubel DH (1965) Comparison of the effects of unilateral and bilateral eye closure on cortical unit responses in kittens. J Neurophysiol 28:10291040.
Wolf F, Bauer HU, Pawelzik K, Geisel T (1996) Organization of the visual cortex. Nature 382, 306.[ISI][Medline]