Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, MO 63110, USA
![]() |
Abstract |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
![]() |
Structure of the OMPFC |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Rat
The most prominent cytoarchitectonic feature of the rat prefrontal cortex is that it is composed exclusively of agranular cortical areas. Brodmann did not use the term prefrontal cortex, but in primates and carnivores he defined the granular cortex anterior to the motor strip as the frontal region (Brodmann, 1909). His cingulate region includes anterior areas that are included here in the medial prefrontal cortex as well as more caudal areas. Brodmann considered that rodents do not have a comparable frontal cortex, since they lack a rostral granular cortex, although they have an agranular anterior cingulate region. Rose and Woolsey (Rose and Woolsey, 1948a
) proposed that the equivalent areas could be recognized on the basis of similar connections, and specifically that the orbitofrontal cortex of rabbits and cats was similar to the primate prefrontal cortex due to its connections with the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus (MD). Because they were using relatively insensitive retrograde cell degeneration methods to define the connections, they still considered this region separate from the anterior cingulate cortex that was connected to the anteromedial thalamic nucleus (Rose and Woolsey, 1948b
).
More recent studies have shown that MD is reciprocally connected to all parts of the OMPFC in both rats and monkeys, including both orbital and anterior cingulate regions, as well as dorsolateral prefrontal areas (Krettek and Price, 1977; Goldman-Rakic et al., 1985; Groenewegen, 1988
; Giguere and Goldman-Rakic, 1988
; Uylings and Van Eden, 1990
; Ray and Price, 1992
, 1993
). Based on this criterion, the prefrontal cortex includes all of the cortex of the dorsolateral, orbital and medial frontal lobe rostral to the precentral motor cortex. Despite the usefulness of this criterion, the problem that Brodmann raised has not been completely solved. The absence of a granular zone in the rat prefrontal cortex still makes it difficult to establish which areas in the rat are homologous to the primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) or frontal polar cortex. A similar analysis was made by Preuss (Preuss, 1995
).
Although Brodmann did not include a rat map in his monograph on the cerebral cortex, he did map the cortex of a variety of other animals, including ground squirrels and hedgehogs (Brodmann, 1909; Garey, 1994
). Based on these maps and on the cellular structure and lamination of the cortex, the medial prefrontal cortex of rats can be divided into infralimbic, prelimbic, dorsal and ventral anterior cingulate and medial precentral cortical areas (Fig. 1A,B
) (Krettek and Price, 1977
; Ray and Price, 1992
). Previous workers (Zilles, 1995; Paxinos and Watson, 1997
) have used the term frontal area 1 (Fr1) instead of medial precentral area (PrCm) and renamed the dorsal and ventral anterior cingulate areas as areas Cg1 and Cg2 and the prelimbic area as area Cg3, a portion of the anterior cingulate cortex (Fig. 1C
). This difference is almost entirely terminological, and most investigators agree on the boundaries of prefrontal cortex areas (Fig. 1
). The frontal pole of hemisphere in rats has not been extensively studied, but it appears to be different from more caudal areas, and has been subdivided into medial and lateral frontal polar areas (Ray and Price, 1992
). The orbital cortex in rats is situated in the dorsal bank of the rostral rhinal sulcus, and may be subdivided into medial, ventrolateral, lateral and dorsolateral orbital areas. Caudal to these areas, the dorsal bank of the rhinal sulcus is occupied by the ventral and dorsal agranular insular areas (Krettek and Price, 1977
; Ray and Price, 1992
).
|
Monkey
Unlike rats, the prefrontal cortex of monkeys contains dysgranular and granular as well as agranular areas. The posterior orbital and medial areas consist of five-layered agranular cortex, while the central orbital areas have an incipient layer IV and may be termed dysgranular. Finally, the most rostral orbital and medial areas are fully granular and sharply laminated, as are the areas of the DLPFC.
Brodmann's original parcellation of the cortex of the monkey Cercopithecus (guenon) bears witness to the increased complexity of the primate prefrontal cortex by including a number of areas with no obvious homologues in the rat prefrontal cortex (Brodmann, 1909; Garey, 1994
). The areas in the caudal part of the medial wall in Cercopithecus resemble those in the rabbit or ground squirrel map, but other more rostral areas differ. Brodmann did not carry out a detailed study of the orbital cortex.
Based on granularity and other staining criteria, Walker (Walker, 1940) divided the OMFPC of the Macaca fascicularis into several different areas (Fig. 2A
). He recognized an area 10 on the frontal pole and area 11 on the rostral orbital surface. Areas 12 and 13 occupied the lateral and medial orbital surface respectively, and area 14 was on the ventromedial convexity. Many researchers have used this classification or extended it based on new data (Porrino et al., 1981
; Amaral and Price, 1984
; Preuss and Goldman-Rakic, 1991
) (Fig. 2B
). On the medial wall, Walker also described several areas that he labeled with the same numbers that Brodmann used, although areas with the same number do not have corresponding locations in Brodmann's and Walker's maps. In particular, in Walker's map area 24 lies caudal to area 25, and extends to the ventral surface, while in Brodmann's map and most other maps area 25 occupies the most caudal and ventral part of the medial wall (Figs 2, 3
).
|
|
In a recent study on the structure of the OMPFC, Carmichael and Price used nine different histochemical and immunohistochemical stains in an effort to identify individual areas (Carmichael and Price, 1994). As a working definition, each recognized area was required to exhibit typical staining patterns in a consistent location with at least three stains, have distinct connections and show little variation across brains. This analysis subdivided many of the areas originally described by Walker (Walker, 1940
) and resulted in the identification of >20 areas in the OMPFC (Fig. 2D
). For example, Walker's rostral granular areas 10 and 11 were subdivided into areas 10m, 10o, 11m and 11l. Of these, 10m extends primarily onto the medial surface of the hemisphere where areas 25 and 32 were also recognized. Area 24 on the anterior cingulate cortex was subdivided into areas 24a, 24b and 24c. On the orbital surface, Walker's areas 12, 13 and 14 were subdivided into areas 12r, 12l, 12m, 12o, 13a, 13b, 13m, 13l, 14r and 14c. Finally, five areas (Iam, Iai, Ial, Iapm and Iapl) were recognized within the rostral extension of the agranular insula into the posterior orbital cortex. Each of these areas represents a cortical module characterized by consistent staining patterns and specific input/output relations, similar to the large number of areas described within the visual-processing stream. Studies of the anatomical connections of these individual areas revealed a wealth of information about the OMPFC (Carmichael and Price, 1995a
,b
, 1996
).
Human
Brodmann's human map resembles his monkey and other maps (Fig. 3A), although he remarked that his monkey and human maps are not identical (Brodmann, 1909
; Garey, 1994
). He particularly noted that the homologies between human and monkey areas are unclear in the frontal (prefrontal) and cingulate regions. In the human map the medial prefrontal cortex is occupied mainly by areas 24, 25, 32 and 10, although Brodmann particularly stated that area 32 is not homologous to area 32 in the monkey map. In one map that has been republished in several subsequent reviews (Petrides and Pandya, 1994
), he included an area 12 on the medial wall anterior to the corpus callosum, but this is not found in his monograph (Brodmann, 1909
) or preceding journal articles (Brodmann, 1905
, 1908
). Where illustrated, this area 12 is clearly different from area 12 in Walker's monkey map (Walker, 1940
). The lateral orbital area that is usually denoted as area 12 in monkeys appears to correspond to area 47 in Brodmann's human map (Petrides and Pandya, 1994
). Brodmann did not illustrate the rest of the orbital surface in detail, but most of it appears to have been denoted as area 11.
Several investigators have proposed modifications to Brodmann's original parcellation of the human OMPFC. A report by Beck recognized the cortex of the human gyrus rectus as area 14 (Beck, 1949). The same report also noted the agranular to granular gradient on the orbital surface and designated the posterior, dysgranular orbital cortex as area 13. Both of these modifications brought Brodmann's map into closer accord with that of Walker (Walker, 1940
). Semendeferi and co-workers also recognized area 13 on the posterior orbital surface of several different primate species, including those of macaque monkeys and humans (Semendeferi et al., 1998
). Petrides and Pandya have made an attempt at formally reconciling the Brodmann and Walker labeling schemes in human and monkey prefrontal cortex (Fig. 3B
) (Petrides and Pandya, 1994
). They use the term area 47/12 to refer to the lateral orbital areas in both humans and monkeys, preserve area 32 anterior to the genu of the corpus callosum, area 24 on the anterior cingulate cortex and area 25 on the posterior medial wall. More recent studies have elaborated on similar themes even though they do not always use similar nomenclature. Using immunohistochemical as well as cytoarchitectonic evidence, Hof and co-workers divided the orbital cortex into eight distinct areas, although they denoted the areas by letters that indicate their position (Fig. 3C
) (Hof et al., 1995
). Because of this, it is difficult to compare their map with those of other workers.
There is now general agreement (Petrides and Pandya, 1994; Semendeferi, 1998) that the cytoarchitecture of the orbital cortex is similar in humans and non-human primates and the differences in Brodmann's and Walker's maps have been largely reconciled. The broad parallels in the organization of the OMPFC in monkeys and humans suggest that cytoarchitectonic areas that are identified anatomically and physiologically in the macaque brain may have counterparts in the human as well. Lacking anatomical connectivity data, the only avenue to define such areas in humans is to study staining patterns in post-mortem material just as was done in monkeys.
Recently we have analyzed sections of the human OMPFC prepared with five different stains in order to apply the architectonic analysis that was done in monkeys by Carmichael and Price (Carmichael and Price, 1994) (Fig. 4
; D. Öngür and J.L. Price, unpublished observations). Almost all of the areas that were recognized in monkeys were also identified in the human, although there are some changes in the relative size of many of the areas. Because area 32 in Brodmann's human map does not correspond to area 32 in the monkey, two areas were distinguished in the medial prefrontal cortex. Area 32h corresponds to the area in the human map, dorsal and rostral to the corpus callosum. In contrast, area 32m, corresponding to the area in monkeys, is recognized in the caudoventral part of the medial wall, adjacent to area 25. The granular area 10m appears to have expanded so greatly, occupying most of the medial wall, that it has been divided into area 10r rostrally and 10m caudally.
|
![]() |
Anatomical Connections of the OMPFC |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Intrinsic Corticocortical Connections
The intrinsic corticocortical connections of the OMPFC have been analyzed in a number of experiments with small injections of retrograde and anterograde axonal tracers that were restricted to single architectonic areas (Carmichael and Price, 1996). This analysis indicated that the areas of the OMPFC can be divided into two groups or networks, one restricted to the orbital cortex and the other involving the medial cortex and some orbital areas (Fig. 5
). Tracer injections in most orbital areas labeled many cells or axons in other orbital areas but only a few in the medial wall. Within the orbital cortex, a few areas (Iai, 12o, 13a and 11m) were more weakly labeled than other orbital areas, and in some cases were conspicuously lacking in label. In contrast, injections in the medial wall areas labeled cells or axons not only in medial areas 24, 25, 32 and 10m, and in the ventromedial areas 14r and 14c, but also in the orbital areas Iai, 12o, 13a and 11m. Furthermore, injections in these select orbital areas produced label in the medial wall areas.
|
Although the two OMPFC networks are clearly distinct, a number of pathways provide communication between them. For example medial network areas 14r, 14c and 12o also have connections with areas of the orbital network. Area 11m, which projects mainly to medial network areas, receives a substantial input from adjacent area 11l, which in turn is innervated by posterior orbital areas that are part of the orbital network (Carmichael and Price, 1996). Thus the sensory-receptive orbital network is connected to the visceromotor medial network, providing a basis for sensorymotor linkage.
Sensory Inputs to the OMPFC
In the words of Nauta, all major sensoria find some form of representation in the prefrontal cortex, however abstract or compounded (Nauta, 1971). Indeed, visual, auditory, somatosensory, olfactory, and gustatory inputs to the OMPFC have been reported in macaque monkeys (Pandya and Kuypers, 1969
; Jones and Powell, 1970
; Chavis and Pandya, 1976
; Mesulam and Mufson, 1982
; Kawamura and Naito, 1984
; Petrides and Pandya, 1988
; Barbas, 1988
; Seltzer and Pandya, 1989
; Rolls et al., 1989; Carmichael et al., 1994
; Hackett et al., 1999
; Romanski et al., 1999
). Similar inputs from olfactory, gustatory, somatosensory and visual sensory areas have also been reported in rats (Guldin and Markowitsch, 1983
; Miller and Vogt, 1984
; Turner and Zimmer, 1984
; Price et al., 1991a
; Conde et al., 1995
; Datiche and Cattarelli, 1996
; Reep et al., 1996
).
Experiments with injections in single OMPFC areas in macaques show that projections from different sensory modalities terminate in restricted areas of the orbital network, especially in the caudal and lateral orbital cortex: visual inputs in area 12l, somatosensory inputs in area 12m, visceral inputs in areas Ial and Iapm, olfactory inputs in areas Iam and Iapm, and gustatory inputs in the primary gustatory cortex (G) (Fig. 5) (Carmichael and Price, 1995a
). All of these areas are in the caudal and lateral part of the orbital network, and projections within this network converge on more rostral areas, providing for integration between several sensory modalities, and with limbic influences (see below) (Carmichael and Price, 1995a
). The central orbital cortex is the first part of the central nervous system where olfactory and gustatory inputs converge, and thus it is probably involved in generating the sensation of flavor associated with different foods. Indeed, many of the other sensory inputs, such as the visceral afferents that parallel the gustatory pathway through the parvicellular part of the ventral posterior medial thalamic nucleus, also appear to be related to food or feeding. Even the somatic sensory inputs appear to arise from the hand and face regions of the somatic sensory cortical areas (Carmichael and Price, 1995a
). In contrast to these sensory inputs to the orbital network, there are very few direct sensory inputs to the medial network.
Studies in both rats and monkeys show not only that cells in the OMPFC respond to multisensory stimulation (Rolls, 1997; Rolls et al., 1999
), but also that the responses are often related to the affective value of the stimulus as well as its sensory properties. For example, the ensemble firing of neurons in the rat orbital cortex during an associative learning task codes better for the reward properties of a smell than for the identity of the smells (Schoenbaum and Eichenbaum, 1995
). Neurons with firing properties such as these are found in the macaque orbital cortex in response to visual and flavor stimulation as well (Rolls, 1984
). Rolls and colleagues have shown that neurons that fire strongly at the sight of a desirable food item while the animal is hungry have much less response when the animal is satiated for the same food item (Critchley et al., 1996). Many of these neurons are still responsive to other appetizing food items. The work of Schultz and colleagues indicates that the OMPFC is part of a wider circuit including limbic structures as well as the ventral striatum and midbrain dopamine cells that respond to reward associations of sensory stimuli. However, the OMPFC is unique among these areas in its coding of the identity of the sensory stimuli as well as their rewarding properties (Schultz et al., 1998
).
Connections with Limbic Structures
In addition to receiving processed sensory inputs, the OMPFC is also intimately connected with limbic forebrain areas (Fig. 6). In rats and cats, as well as monkeys, the OMPFC has largely reciprocal connections with the basal, accessory basal, and lateral nuclei of the amygdala, the subiculum, and the entorhinal and perirhinal cortex (Krettek and Price, 1977
; Swanson, 1981
; Ray and Price, 1992
; Barbas and Blatt, 1995
; Carmichael and Price, 1995b
). In monkeys, these connections as a whole are not selective for either the medial or the orbital network. The projection from the basal amygdaloid nucleus, however, is divided between the two networks in an organized manner (Carmichael and Price, 1995b
). The medial network (including the orbital areas Iai and 12o) is primarily connected to the ventrolateral part of the basal nucleus, while areas of the orbital network are connected to the ventromedial part of the nucleus. The entorhinal, perirhinal and temporal polar cortices have extensive projections, especially within the orbital cortex, although no organization has been discerned within these systems relative to the two networks. The projection from the subiculum arises primarily from the rostral subiculum, especially from the region at the border between the subiculum and CA1. The fibers terminate in areas associated with the medial prefrontal network, specifically in the caudal part of the medial wall and the medial part of the orbital cortex (Barbas and Blatt, 1995
; Carmichael and Price, 1995b
).
|
Studies of the projections from the OMPFC to the striatum have shown that distinct regions of the prefrontal cortex project to distinct portions of the ventromedial striatum (Fig. 7). In rats, the OMPFC as a whole projects to the ventromedial part of the striatum, including the nucleus accumbens, the medial part of the caudate nucleus and the ventral part of the putamen (Philipson and Griffiths, 1985; Berendse et al., 1992
; Brog et al., 1993
). Within the OMPFC, however, different areas project to different striatal regions. For example, the infralimbic area projects strongly to the shell of the nucleus accumbens, while the prelimbic area projects to the core of the nucleus accumbens and the medial part of the caudate nucleus. Lateral areas, such as the agranular insular cortex, project to a contiguous ventrolateral part of the striatum, including the ventral putamen. Notably, the lateral orbital area, in the middle of the orbital cortex, has been reported to project to the central part of the caudate/putamen, out of the sequence of projections from adjacent areas (Berendse et al., 1992
).
|
The ventromedial striatal regions that receive fibers from the OMPFC in turn project to the ventral pallidum, the ventral and rostral extension of the globus pallidus. The ventral pallidum then projects to the medial part of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus (MDm), which is reciprocally interconnected with the OMPFC (Fig. 7). Remarkably, the distinction between the orbital and medial networks is also seen in the connections with MDm, such that areas within the two networks are reciprocally related to different subdivisions of this nucleus. Thus, areas 32, 24, 14, 13a and Iai, all belonging to the medial network, are primarily connected to the paramedian edge and caudodorsal part of the medial part of MDm (Ray and Price, 1993
). In contrast, areas of the orbital network are all primarily connected to the fibrous part of MDm that is more centrally located within MD as a whole.
Although all of the projections have not yet been analyzed in sufficient detail, it appears possible, therefore, that there are distinct striato-pallido-thalamo-cortical circuits related to the medial and orbital networks. The ventral striato-pallidal circuit is comparable to the parallel dorsal striato-pallido-thalamo-cortical circuit that involves the dorsolateral striatum, globus pallidus, ventroanterior and ventrolateral thalamic nuclei and the DLPFC. However, where the dorsal circuit has been implicated in motor functions, it is likely that the ventral circuit is involved in reward-guided choice behavior (Schultz et al., 1998).
It should be noted that the intralaminar nuclei of the thalamus, especially the medial and midline nuclei, also have extensive connections with both the ventromedial striatum and the OMPFC (Berendse and Groenewegen, 1990, 1991
). As yet, however, these have not been analyzed in relation to the medial or orbital prefrontal networks.
Connections with the Hypothalamus and Brainstem
It has been recognized since the last decades of the 19th century that the cerebral cortex exerts an influence over visceral function and the autonomic nervous system in particular. A large number of studies have described cortical control over cardiovascular, gastric and respiratory systems in monkeys, dogs, cats, rabbits and rats (Hoff and Green, 1936; Smith, 1945
; Fulton, 1949
; Penfield and Rasmussen, 1950
; Kaada, 1951
; Wall and Davis, 1951
). Specifically, a region of cerebral cortex including the anterior cingulate gyrus, the medial wall ventral to the corpus callosum, the posterior orbital surface, the agranular insula and the temporal polar cortex was shown affect a wide variety of physiological systems under autonomic control (Kaada, 1960
). As discussed below, most of this output arises in areas of the medial prefrontal network, which can be considered a visceromotor system.
In his review of the literature on cortical control of the autonomic nervous system, Neafsey noted that there is not a simple relationship between cortical activity and the indices being monitored (Neafsey, 1990). Depending on the location of the electrode and the current intensity, cortical stimulation produces variable changes in heart rate or blood pressure, and some recent studies even report intermingled pressor and depressor sites (Kaada et al., 1949
; Lofving, 1961
; Terreberry and Neafsey, 1987
; Fisk and Wyss, 1997
). It is probable that these physiological studies reveal distinct output pathways from closely positioned areas of cingulate, subcallosal and posterior orbital cortex.
The major autonomic related structures that receive fibers from the OMPFC are the hypothalamus and periaqueductal gray (PAG) (Hardy and Holmes, 1988; Cechetto and Chen, 1990
), although in rats there also appear to be cortical projections to more caudal brainstem structures (Terreberry and Neafsey, 1987
; Neafsey 1990
; Hurley et al., 1991
). Within these structures, different nuclei or columns (in the PAG) have been associated with distinct functions as diverse as aggression, autonomic control, feeding, drinking, sexual behavior, thermoregulation and anti-nociception (Swanson, 1987
). Such functions may occur separately, but they are often evoked together as parts of coordinated coping strategies. For example, stimulation of the lateral column of the PAG by excitatory amino acids generates tachycardia, hypertension and non-opioid analgesia as part of an overall confrontational behavioral stance. In contrast, stimulation of the ventrolateral column of the PAG generates bradycardia, hypotension and opioid analgesia within a quiescent behavioral stance (Bandler and Shipley, 1994
). By its influence on such responses the OMPFC goes well beyond reflexive effects to evoke a coordinated array of responses that define coping strategies in response to a challenge.
It has been known for some time that in rats there are anatomical projections to the hypothalamus and brainstem from both medial and lateral (orbital) prefrontal areas (Terreberry and Neafsey, 1987; Neafsey 1990
; Hurley et al., 1991
; Price et al., 1991b
; Bandler and Shipley, 1994
). In monkeys, it has recently been shown that the projections to the hypothalamus and PAG arise almost entirely from areas in the medial prefrontal network (Fig. 5
) (An et al. 1998
; Öngür et al., 1998a
). Cells labeled by retrograde tracer injections into the PAG are almost entirely restricted to the medial network and closely related areas (e.g. area 9 in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex). Cells that project to the hypothalamus are also primarily located in the medial network, although there is also a light projection to the posterior hypothalamus from agranular insular areas that are otherwise considered part of the orbital network. Recent observations in rats indicate that the cortical projections to the PAG arise in the medial wall and in the far lateral dorsolateral orbital area and dorsal agranular insular area, but not from intervening orbital areas in the dorsal bank of the rhinal sulcus (N. Floyd, J.L. Price, K. Keay and R. Bandler, unpublished observations). These areas appear to be homologous to the medial network defined in monkeys. Outside the OMPFC, the only cortical areas in monkeys that project to the hypothalamus and PAG are parts of the temporal pole and dysgranular insula. Similar cortical regions also provide the cortical efferents to the dopaminergic, serotonergic and noradrenergic brainstem nuclei in rats and monkeys (Nauta, 1971
; Sesack et al., 1989
).
The medial prefrontal network can be subdivided into several regions that have distinctive projections to different portions of the hypothalamus and PAG (Fig. 5) (An et al. 1998
; Öngür et al., 1998a
). Anterograde tracer injections in medial wall areas 25 and 32 result in strong label in most hypothalamic regions, including in particular the medial hypothalamus. The same areas, along with medial area 10m, project strongly to the dorsolateral column of the PAG. Dorsomedial areas 24b and 9 project strongly to the dorsal hypothalamus and to the lateral PAG column, while orbital areas 13a, 12o and Iai project to the lateral hypothalamus and the ventrolateral column of the PAG. Similar subdivisions can be recognized in rats (N. Floyd et al., unpublished observations). There are also substantial ascending and descending connections between the hypothalamus and PAG, and it is probable that the portions of the hypothalamus and PAG that receive similar cortical inputs are interconnected (Beitz, 1981; Venning et al., 1991). Hypothalamic and PAG projections to the parabrachial nucleus and other brainstem regions may also be organized along the same lines (Krout et al., 1998
).
![]() |
Functions of the OMPFC |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
In his synthesis of the physiological studies with anatomical data from the rat, Neafsey proposed that prefrontal cortex can be divided into visceral sensory areas on the ventrolateral (orbital) surface and visceral motor areas on the medial wall (Neafsey, 1990). These are situated on either side of the dorsal somatic sensory and motor cortical areas. This concept is useful in interpreting the anatomical data on the macaque OMPFC as well since the sensory inputs into the OMPFC mainly reach the orbital network while the subcortical outputs to visceromotor structures mainly arise from the medial network. (The sensory motor distinction appears not to lie along a simple mediolateral axis, however, since the medial network areas Iai and 12o lie on the lateral orbital surface.) It is important to note that while the two networks are distinct, there are a number of points of interaction by which sensory information may be transferred from the orbital to the medial network. Many of these pathways are through areas at the ventromedial corner of the frontal lobe, especially areas 13a, 14r, 14c and 11m.
Fuster proposed a similar sensory-motor transfer system through the DLPFC (Fuster, 1997). In this scheme, highly processed information from sensory association areas in the parietal and temporal cortex converges onto the DLPFC. The DLPFC then produces behaviors based on this sensory input via projections to premotor and motor areas. The difference with the OMPFC is that the output is primarily to visceromotor control. Interestingly, this system also appears to have a major role in the guidance of behavior.
Guidance of Behavior by the OMPFC
Behavioral studies suggest that in addition to linking sensory and visceromotor activity, the OMPFC also participates in the guidance of emotional behavior. As discussed below, these two functions appear to be closely linked. Monkeys with lesions of the OMPFC show abnormal food preferences (Ursin et al., 1969; Butter et al., 1969
; Baylis and Gaffan, 1991
) and also have altered aggressive and aversive responses to threatening stimuli (Butter and Snyder, 1972
). Many of the behavioral effects of OMPFC lesions are remarkably similar to those seen following amygdala lesions, indicating that both of these structures are involved in circuits controlling emotional and social behaviors (Kling, 1972
). For example, in free-ranging settings animals with prefrontal cortex lesions, like those with amygdala lesions, lose their position in the social hierarchy (Butter and Snyder, 1972
), or fall out of the troop altogether and die in solitude (Myers et al., 1973
).
These behavioral studies have remained somewhat segregated in the literature from studies on the visceral functions of the OMPFC, but this may be deceiving. In a review full of remarkable insights, Nauta posited that the prefrontal cortex is informed about most sensory stimuli impinging on the sensory organs of the body, and that the OMPFC uses this information to obtain the most desirable and rewarding outcomes for the organism (Nauta, 1971). Nauta emphasized that the visceral sensory inputs and visceromotor outputs are particularly important in this regard since they provide information about the status of the internal milieu and generate bodily reactions to alter that status. These bodily reactions are then relayed to the prefrontal cortex as new information on the state of the body, forming a loop where the OMPFC is constantly monitoring and altering that state. Based on these ideas, Nauta suggested that animals with lesions of the prefrontal cortex experience an interoceptive agnosia', an inability to determine how the body is reacting when faced with contrasting behavioral options. Since the organism is deprived of the navigational markers' it would normally use to decide which action is the most desirable, this agnosia then leads to a variety of emotional, social and cognitive deficits.
These ideas that Nauta distilled from anatomical studies have received important support from clinical studies in humans. Observations about lesions of the OMPFC have a long history, starting with Harlow's report on his patient Phineas Gage, a respected railroad construction foreman who after a work accident sustained damage to most of the medial wall and orbital surface of the frontal lobe (Harlow, 1868). After the accident, Gage underwent personality changes and started showing impulsive, socially inappropriate behavior. Despite apparently possessing all of his intellectual faculties, he lost his job, strained his family ties and was left a changed person. In another early study, Welt (Welt, 1888) described a patient with a penetrating frontal wound who developed personality changes very similar to those suffered by Gage, although they later resolved (Benton, 1991
). In her study, Welt also reviewed and synthesized many other reports of behavioral change following frontal lobe injury and concluded that the most consistent locations of damage associated with disinhibited, dysfunctional behavior were the orbitofrontal gyri. In this century, frontal leukotomies performed on psychiatric patients to decrease anxiety, depression and a host of other problems generated renewed interest in frontal lobe lesions, although they were of uncertain clinical value. The clinical picture following this procedure was a loss of emotional regulation that manifested itself in various ways. Some patients showed apathy toward most things that would elicit emotional reactions from normals, while others became disinhibited and inappropriately outgoing (Valenstein, 1986
).
More recently, Damasio and colleagues showed in a series of studies that patients with specific OMPFC damage give deficient visceral responses to disturbing or exciting images, as demonstrated with galvanic skin responses (GSRs). Particularly in their prototypical patient E.V.R., this lack of visceral/emotional response is coupled with an inability to make appropriate life decisions (Damasio et al., 1990). Bechara and colleagues demonstrated this inability in a card game intended to model daily-life decision making (Bechara et al., 1996
, 1997
). In this game, which uses play money, normal controls quickly learn to navigate their way without losing money, even before they develop conscious awareness of this fact and before they employ explicit strategies for winning. This quick adaptation phase is characterized by anticipatory GSRs prior to each unfavorable move. E.V.R. and other OMPFC-lesioned patients display a different pattern: they are deficient in their anticipatory GSRs and keep making bad moves that look to them to be lucrative in the short term. Much like in real life, the patients eventually go bankrupt.
In an effort to understand this complex behavioral deviation, Damasio has proposed a somatic marker hypothesis' (Damasio, 1994). This hypothesis echoes earlier theories by William James, who equated emotional experience with the visceral reactions of the body (James, 1890
); by Schachter and Singer, who suggested that the dissonance between cognitive states and visceral reactions may trigger conscious perceptions of emotion (Schachter and Singer, 1962
); and most specifically by Nauta, who suggested that navigational markers produced by the prefrontal cortex guide behavior (Nauta, 1971
). Damasio suggests that CNS-generated changes in the internal milieu of the organism serve to color our daily experiences and tag certain situations or outcomes as positive or negative. The OMPFC may serve to evaluate external events and to generate autonomic or somatic changes in the body, thus signaling to the organism that a given behavioral option is risky or undesirable. Damasio suggests that the inability of E.V.R. and other patients with damage to the OMPFC to generate these kinds of internal reactions renders them dysfunctional in making many small and large decisions on a daily basis.
Mood Disorders and the OMPFC
Several lines of evidence indicate a role for the prefrontal cortex in emotional behavior. Disinhibited or apathetic behavior has been reported in monkeys and humans following damage to the ventral prefrontal cortex. The OMPFC is related to fearand reward-related circuits through its connections with the amygdala and other medial temporal lobe structures and the ventral striatum. The OMPFC also projects to sites in the diencephalon and brainstem that mediate the endocrine, visceral and somatic changes accompanying emotional behavior. However, the relationship between emotional behavior and the setting of mood is not always clear. Mood represents the longer-term internal experience of an organism and it colors one's sense of self and relation to the external world. It is influenced by but distinct from fleeting emotional or motivational experiences. It is difficult to study mood states in experimental animals but the OMPFC has been implicated in mood disorders in humans.
Major depressive disorder (MDD) and bipolar disorder (BD), together known as mood disorders, are the most common pathological alterations of mood and emotion in humans. The use of in vivo neuroimaging techniques has helped identify a circuit of forebrain areas that is altered in activity and structure in mood disorders. Within this circuit, the OMPFC, mediodorsal thalamus and amygdala were found to have increased blood flow in patients with familial pure depressive disorder (Drevets et al., 1992). This classification denotes a subset of MDD patients with positive family history for MDD or BD, no family history for alcoholism and no history of psychotic or manic episodes.
Morphological changes in the depressed brain have been difficult to identify but they may provide indications of the neural deficit underlying mood disorders. One such change has been found in a small area in the subgenual prefrontal cortex. Unlike other parts of the OMPFC, this area shows a relative decrease in blood flow in patients with MDD and BD (Drevets et al., 1997). It corresponds to the first gyrus ventral to the genu of the corpus callosum [the subgenual part of Brodmann's area 24 (area sg24)]. On magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)-based volumetric measurements, the subgenual prefrontal cortex is actually 40% smaller in depressed patients and this at least partially accounts for the observed decrease in blood flow.
Although the basis for the tissue shrinkage in area sg24 is not clear, examination of autopsy brains from subjects with MDD or BD has indicated that there is a specific cellular difference in this area and possibly in other parts of the OMPFC. Cell counts in these areas have shown that the number of neurons is approximately normal, but that there is a specific reduction in the number and density of glial cells (Fig. 8) (Öngür et al., 1998b
; Rajkowska et al., 1999
). The reduction is especially prominent in subjects with a family history of MDD or BD. A reduction in glia was not found in the primary somatosensory cortex in the post-central gyrus in the same cases. Recently, however, a similar reduction has been found in the amygdala of depressed subjects (M. Bowley, D. Öngür and J. Price, unpublished observations).
|
![]() |
Notes |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Address correspondence to Joseph L. Price, Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Campus Box 8108, Washington University School of Medicine, 660 S. Euclid Avenue, St Louis, MO 63110, USA. Email: pricej{at}thalamus.wustl.edu.
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Amaral, DG, Price, JL (1984) Amygdalo-cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca fascicularis). J Comp Neurol 230:465496.[ISI][Medline]
An X, Öngür D, Price JL (1997) Prefrontostriatal projections in relation to cortico-cortical networks in the macaque monkey. Soc Neurosci Abstr 23:901.
An X, Bandler R, Öngür D, Price JL (1998) Prefrontal cortical projections to longitudinal columns in the midbrain periaqueductal gray in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 401:455479.[ISI][Medline]
Bandler R, Shipley MT (1994) Columnar organization in the midbrain periaqueductal gray: modules for emotional expression? Trends Neurosci 17:379389.[ISI][Medline]
Barbas H (1988) Anatomic organization of basoventral and mediodorsal visual recipient prefrontal regions in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 276:313342.[ISI][Medline]
Barbas H, Pandya DN (1989) Architecture and intrinsic connections of the prefrontal cortex in the Rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 286:353375.[ISI][Medline]
Barbas H, Blatt GJ (1995) Topographically specific hippocampal projections target functionally distinct prefrontal areas in the rhesus monkey. Hippocampus 5:511533.[ISI][Medline]
Baylis LL, Gaffan D (1991) Amygdalectomy and ventromedial prefrontal ablation produce similar deficits in food choice and in simple object discrimination learning for an unseen reward. Exp Brain Res 86:617622.[ISI][Medline]
Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR (1996) Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 6:215225.[Abstract]
Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR (1997) Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 275:12931295.
Beck, E (1949) A cytoarchitectural investigation into the boundaries of cortical areas 13 and 14 in the human brain. J Anat 83:147157.[ISI]
Beitz, AJ (1982) The organization of afferent projections to the midbrain periaqueductal gray of the rat. Neuroscience 7:133159.[ISI][Medline]
Benton, AL (1991) The prefrontal region: its early history. In: Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (Levin HS, Eisenberg HM, Benton AL, eds), pp. 334. New York: Oxford University Press.
Berendse HW, Groenewegen HJ (1990) Organization of the thalamostriatal projections in the rat, with special emphasis on the ventral striatum. J Comp Neurol 299:187228.[ISI][Medline]
Berendse HW, Groenewegen HJ (1991) Restricted cortical termination fields of the midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei in the rat. Neuroscience 42:73102.[ISI][Medline]
Berendse HW, Galis-de Graaf Y, Groenewegen HJ (1992) Topographical organization and relationship with ventral striatal compartments of prefrontal corticostriatal projections in the rat. J Comp Neurol 316:314347.[ISI][Medline]
Bonin, G, Bailey P (1947) The neocortex of Macaca mulatta. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
Brodmann, K (1905) Beitraege zur histologischen Lokalisation der Grosshirnrinde. III Mitteilung. Die Rindenfelder der niederen Affen. J Psychol Neurol 4:177226.
Brodmann, K (1908) Beitraege zur histologischen Lokalisation der Grosshirnrinde. VI Mitteilung. Die Cortexgliederung des Menschen. J Psychol Neurol 10:231246.
Brodmann, K (1909) Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues. Leipzig: JA Barth.
Brog JS, Salyapongse A, Deutch AY, Zahm DS (1993) The patterns of afferent innervation of the core and shell in the accumbens' part of the rat ventral striatum: immunohistochemical detection of retrogradely transported fluoro-gold. J Comp Neurol 338:255278.[ISI][Medline]
Butter CM, McDonald JA, Snyder DR (1969) Orality, preference behavior and reinforcement value of non-food objects in monkeys with orbital frontal lesions. Science 164:13061307.[ISI][Medline]
Butter, CM, Snyder DR (1972) Alterations in aversive and aggressive behaviors following orbital frontal lesions in Rhesus monkeys. Acta Neurobiol Exp 32:525565.[Medline]
Carmichael, ST, Price JL (1994) Architectonic subdivision of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 346: 366402.[ISI][Medline]
Carmichael, ST, Price JL (1995a) Limbic connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 363: 615641.[ISI][Medline]
Carmichael, ST, Price JL (1995b) Sensory and premotor connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex. J Comp Neurol 363:642664.[ISI][Medline]
Carmichael, ST, Price JL (1996) Connectional networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 371:179207.[ISI][Medline]
Carmichael, ST, Clugnet M-C, Price JL (1994) Central olfactory connections in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 346:403434.[ISI][Medline]
Cechetto, DF, Chen SJ (1990) Subcortical sites mediate sympathetic responses from insular cortex. Am J Physiol 258:R245255.
Chavis DA, Pandya DN (1976) Further observations on corticofrontal connections in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 117:369386.[ISI][Medline]
Conde F, Maire-Lepoivre E, Audinal E, Crepel F (1995) Afferent connections of the medial frontal cortex of the rat. II. Cortical and subcortical afferents. J Comp Neurol 352:567593.[ISI][Medline]
Critchley, HD, Rolls ET (1996) Hunger and satiety modify the responses of olfactory and visual neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 75:16731686.
Damasio, AR (1994) Descartes' error. New York: Avon.
Damasio, AR, Tranel D, Damasio H (1990) Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behav Brain Res 41:8194.[ISI][Medline]
Datiche, F, Cattarelli M (1996) Reciprocal and topographic connections between the piriform and prefrontal cortices in the rat: a tracing study using the B subunit of the cholera toxin. Brain Res Bull 41:391398.[ISI][Medline]
Drevets WC, Videen TO, Price JL, Preskorn SH, Carmichael ST, Raichle ME (1992) A functional anatomical study of unipolar depression. J Neurosci 12:36283641.[Abstract]
Drevets WC, Price JL, Simpson JR, Todd RD, Vannier M, Raichle ME (1997) Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders. Nature 386:824827.[ISI][Medline]
Fisk GD, Wyss JM (1997) Pressor and depressor sites are intermingled in the cingulate cortex of the rat. Brain Res 754:204212.[ISI][Medline]
Fulton JF (1949) Cerebral cortex: the orbitofrontal and cingulate regions. In: Physiology of the nervous system, 3rd edn, pp. 447467. New York: Oxford University Press.
Fuster JM (1997) The prefrontal cortex. New York: Raven Press.
Garey LJ (1994) Brodmann's Localisation in the cerebral cortex. London: Smith-Gordon & Co.
Giguere M, Goldman-Rakic PS (1988) Mediodorsal nucleus: areal, laminar, and tangential distribution of afferents and efferents in the frontal lobe of rhesus monkeys. J Comp Neurol 277:195213.[ISI][Medline]
Glassman AH, Shapiro PA (1998) Depression and the course of coronary artery disease. Am J Psychol 155:411.
Goldman-Rakic PS, Porrino LJ (1985) The primate mediodorsal (MD) nucleus and its projection to the frontal lobe. J Comp Neurol 242:535560.[ISI][Medline]
Groenewegen HJ (1988) Organization of the afferent connections of the medio-dorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to mediodorsal prefrontal topography. Neuroscience 24:379431.[ISI][Medline]
Guldin WO, Markowitsch HJ (1983) Cortical and thalamic afferent connections of the insular and adjacent cortex of the rat. J Comp Neurol 215:135153.[ISI][Medline]
Haber SN, Kunishio K, Mizobuchi M, Lynd-Balta E (1995) The orbital and medial prefrontal circuit through the primate basal ganglia. J Neurosci 7:48514867.
Hackett TA, Stepniewska I, Kaas JH (1999) Prefrontal connections of the parabelt auditory cortex in macaque monkeys. Brain Res 817:4558.[ISI][Medline]
Hardy SGP, Holmes DE (1988) Prefrontal stimulus-produced hypotension in the rat. Exp Brain Res 73:249255.[ISI][Medline]
Harlow JM (1968) Recovery of the passage of an iron bar through the head. Pub Mass Med Soc 2:327347.
Hof PR, Mufson EJ, Morrison JH (1995) Human orbitofrontal cortex: cytoarchitecture and quantitative immunohistochemical parcellation. J Comp Neurol 359:4868.[ISI][Medline]
Hoff EC, Green HD (1936) Cardiovascular reactions induced by electrical stimulation of the cerebral cortex. Am J Physiol 117:411422.
Hurley KM, Herbert H, Moga MM, Saper CB (1991) Efferent projections of the infralimbic cortex of the rat. J Comp Neurol 308:249276.[ISI][Medline]
James W (1890) The principles of psychology, vols 1 and 2 [reprinted 1950]. New York: Dover.
Jones EG, Powell TPS (1970) An anatomical study of converging sensory pathways within the cerebral cortex of the monkey. Brain 93: 793820.[Medline]
Kaada BR, Pribram KH, Epstein JA (1949) Respiratory and vascular responses in monkeys from temporal pole, insula, orbital surface and cingulate gyrus. J Neurophysiol 12:347356.
Kaada, BR (1951) Somato-motor, autonomic and electrocardiographic responses to electrical stimulation of rhinencephalic and other structures in primate, cat and dog. Acta Physiol Scand 24(suppl): 83:285.
Kaada BR (1960) Cingulate, posterior orbital, anterior insular and temporal pole cortex. In: Handbook of physiology, Section I: Neurophysiology, Vol. II (Magoun HW, ed.), pp. 13451372. Washington DC: American Physiological Society.
Kawamura K, Naito J (1984) Corticocortical projections to the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey investigated with horseradish peroxidase techniques. Neurosci Res 1:89103.[Medline]
Kling A (1972) Effects of amygdalectomy on social-affective behavior in non-human primates. In: The neurobiology of the amygdala (Eleftheriou BE, ed.), pp. 511536. New York: Plenum Press.
Krettek JE, Price JL (1977) Projections from the amygdaloid complex to the cerebral cortex and thalamus in the rat and cat. J Comp Neurol 172:687722.[ISI][Medline]
Krout KE, Jansen AS, Loewy AD (1998) Periaqueductal gray matter projection to the parabrachial nucleus in rat. J Comp Neurol 401:437454.[ISI][Medline]
Lofving B (1961) Cardiovascular adjustments induced from the rostral cingulate gyrus. Acta Physiol Scand 53(suppl):184:182.
Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ (1982) Insula of the old world monkey. III: Efferent cortical output and comments on function. J Comp Neurol 212:3852.[ISI][Medline]
Miller MW, Vogt BA (1984) Direct connections of rat visual cortex with sensory, motor and association cortices. J Comp Neurol 226:184202.[ISI][Medline]
Myers RE, Swett C, Miller M (1973) Loss of social group affinity following prefrontal lesions in free-ranging macaques. Brain Res 64:257269.[ISI][Medline]
Nauta WJH (1971) The problem of the frontal lobe: a reinterpretation. J Psychiat Res 8:167187.[ISI][Medline]
Neafsey EJ (1990) Prefrontal cortical control of the autonomic nervous system: anatomical and physiological observations. Progr Brain Res 85:147166.[Medline]
Öngür D, An X, Price JL (1998a) Prefrontal cortical projections to the hypothalamus in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 401:480505.[ISI][Medline]
Öngür D, Drevets WD, Price JL (1998b) Glial reduction in the subgenual prefrontal cortex in mood disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:1329013295.
Pandya DN, Kuypers HG (1969) Cortico-cortical connections in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 13:1336.[ISI][Medline]
Paxinos G, Watson C (1997) The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego: Academic Press.
Penfield W, Rasmussen TB (1950) The cerebral cortex of man. New York: Macmillan.
Petrides M, Pandya DN (1988) Association fiber pathways to the frontal cortex from the superior temporal region in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 273:5266.[ISI][Medline]
Petrides M, Pandya DN (1994) Comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis of the human and the macaque frontal cortex. In: Handbook of neuropsychology, Vol. 9 (Boller F, Grafman J, eds), pp. 1758. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Phillipson OT, Griffiths AC (1985) The topographic order of inputs to nucleus accumbens in the rat. Neuroscience 16:275296.[ISI][Medline]
Porrino LJ, Crane AM, Goldman-Rakic PS (1981) Direct and indirect pathways from the amygdala to the frontal lobe in rhesus monkeys. J Comp Neurol 198:121136.[ISI][Medline]
Preuss TM (1995) Do rats have prefrontal cortex? The RoseWoolsey Akert program reconsidered. J Cogn Neurosci 7:124.[ISI]
Preuss TM, Goldman-Rakic PS (1991) Myeloand cytoarchitecture of the granular frontal cortex and surrounding regions in the strepsirhine primate Galago and the anthropoid primate Macaca. J Comp Neurol 198:121136.
Price JL, Carmichael ST, Carnes KM, Clugnet M-C, Kuroda M, Ray JP (1991a) Olfactory input to the prefrontal cortex. In: Olfaction: a model for computational neuroscience (Davis, JL, Eichenbaum H, eds), pp. 101120. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Price JL, Slotnick BM, Revial M-F (1991b) Olfactory projections to the hypothalamus. J Comp Neurol 306:447461.[ISI][Medline]
Rajkowska G, Miguel-Hidalgo JJ, Wei J, Dilley G, Pittman SD, Meltzer HY, Overholser JC, Roth BL, Stockmeier CA (1999) Morphometric evidence for neuronal and glial prefrontal cell pathology in major depression. Biol Psychiat 45:10851098.[ISI][Medline]
Ray JP, Price JL (1992) The organization of the thalamocortical connections of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to the ventral forebrainprefrontal cortex topography. J Comp Neurol 323:167197.[ISI][Medline]
Ray JP, Price JL (1993) The organization of projections from the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus to orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 337:131.[ISI][Medline]
Reep RL, Corwin JV, King V (1996) Neuronal connections of orbital cortex in rats: topography of cortical and thalamic afferents. Exp Brain Res 111:215232.[ISI][Medline]
Rolls ET (1984) The neurophysiology of feeding. Int J Obesity 8(suppl 1):139150.
Rolls ET (1989) Information processing in the taste system of primates. J Exp Biol 146:141164.[Abstract]
Rolls ET (1997) Taste and olfactory processing in the brain and its relation to the control of eating. Crit Rev Neurobiol 11:263287[ISI][Medline]
Rolls ET, Critchley HD, Browning AS, Hernadi I, Lenard L (1999) Responses to the sensory properties of fat of neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci 19:15321540
Romanski LM, Bates JF, Goldman-Rakic PS (1999) Auditory belt and parabelt projections to the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 403:141157.[ISI][Medline]
Rose JE, Woolsey CN (1948a) The orbitofrontal cortex and its connections with the mediodorsal nucleus in rabbit, sheep and cat. Res Publ Ass Nerv Ment Dis 27:210232.[ISI]
Rose JE, Woolsey CN (1948b) Structure and relations of limbic cortex and anterior thalamic nuclei in rabbit and cat. J Comp Neurol 77:61130.
Russchen FT, Price JL (1984) Amygdalostriatal projections in the rat. Topographical organization and fiber morphology shown using the lectin PHA-L as an anterograde tracer. Neurosci Lett 47:1522.[ISI][Medline]
Russchen FT, Bakst I, Amaral DG, Price JL (1985) The amygdalostriatal projections in the monkey. An anterograde tracing study. Brain Res 329:241257.[ISI][Medline]
Sanides F (1970) Functional architecture of motor and sensory cortices in primates in the light of a new concept of neocortex evolution. In: The primate brain: advances in primatology (Noback CR, Montagna W, eds), pp. 137205. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Schachter S, Singer J (1962) Cognitive, social and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychol Rev 69:379399.[ISI]
Schoenbaum G, Eichenbaum H (1995) Information coding in the rodent prefrontal cortex. II. Ensemble activity in orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 74:751762.
Schultz W, Tremblay L, Hollerman JR (1998) Reward prediction in primate basal ganglia and frontal cortex. Neuropharmacology 37: 421429.[ISI][Medline]
Seltzer B, Pandya DN (1989) Frontal lobe connections of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 281:97113.[ISI][Medline]
Semendeferi K, Armstrong E, Schleicher A, Zilles K, Van Hoesen GW (1998) Limbic frontal cortex in hominoids: a comparative study of area 13. Am J Physiol Anthropol 106:129155.[ISI][Medline]
Sesack SR, Deutch AY, Roth RH, Bunney BS (1989) Topographical organization of the efferent projections of the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: an anterograde tract-tracing study with Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. J Comp Neurol 290:213242.[ISI][Medline]
Smith WK (1945) The functional significance of the rostral cingular cortex as revealed by its responses to electrical stimulation. J Neurophysiol 8:241255.
Swanson LW (1981) A direct projection from Ammon's horn to prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Res 217:150154.[ISI][Medline]
Swanson LW (1987) The Hypothalamus. In: Handbook of chemical neuroanatomy, Vol. 5 (Bjorklund A, Hokfelt T, Swanson LW, eds), pp. 1124. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Terreberry RR, Neafsey EJ (1987) The rat medial frontal cortex projects directly to autonomic regions of the brainstem. Brain Res Bull 19:639649.[ISI][Medline]
Turner BH, Zimmer J (1984) The architecture and some of the interconnections of the rat's amygdala and lateral periallocortex. J Comp Neurol 227:540557.[ISI][Medline]
Ursin H, Linck P, McCleary RA (1969) Spatial differentiation of avoidance deficit following septal and cingulate lesions. J Comp Physiol Psychol 68:7479.[ISI][Medline]
Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG (1990) Qualitative and quantitative comparison of the prefrontal cortex in rat and in primates, including humans. Progr Brain Res 85:3162.[Medline]
Valenstein ES (1986) Great and desperate cures: the rise and decline of psychosurgery and other radical treatments for mental illness. New York: Basic Books.
Veening J, Buma P, Ter Horst GJ, Roeling TAP, Luiten PGM, Niewenhuys R (1991) Hypothalamic projections to the PAG in the rat: topographical, immuno-electronmicroscopical and functional aspects. In: The midbrain periaqueductal gray matter (Depaulis A, Bandler R, eds), pp. 387414. New York: Plenum Press..
Walker AE (1940) A cytoarchitectural study of the prefrontal area of the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol 73:5986.
Wall PD, Davis GD (1951) Three cerebral cortical systems affecting autonomic function. J Neurophysiol 14:507517.
Welt L (1888) Über Charakterveranderungen des Menschen infolge von Läisionen des Stirnhirns. Deutsche Arch Klin Med 31:339390.
Zilles K, Wree A (1995) Cortex: areal and laminar structure. In: The rat nervous system (Paxinos G, ed.), pp. 649685. San Diego: Academic Press.