1 Neuropsychology, Department of Psychology, University of Freiburg, Germany, 2 Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, UK, 3 Department of Neuroradiology, University of Freiburg, Germany, 4 Department of Nuclear Medicine, University of Ulm, Germany and 5 Department of Nuclear Medicine, University of Düsseldorf, Germany
![]() |
Abstract |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Key Words: individual performance neuroimaging planning problem solving Tower of London trial correctness
![]() |
Introduction |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
This possible confound of planning-related and performance- and item-related processes becomes apparent when considering the literature on neuronal processes elicited by errors in speeded-response tasks (e.g. Go-NoGo or flanker tasks; Falkenstein et al., 1991; Gehring et al., 1993
). Recent fMRI studies showed that performance errors are accompanied by a characteristic pattern of activity in the rostral anterior and the posterior cingulate, the insular cortex, and the intraparietal sulcus (Menon et al., 2001
; Ullsperger and von Cramon, 2001
). Although the tasks used in such studies usually require rapid motor responses, and can thus not be directly compared to problem solving tasks, the question remains whether error-related processes at least partly contribute to the overall patterns of brain activation observed during higher-order cognitive tasks.
In the present study, we thus examined the influence of both problem-solving ability (between-subjects) and error-related processing (within-subject) on brain activation elicited by problem solving. Participants were recruited ranging from very poor to very good problem-solvers, based on scores in pilot testing. The problem set used was constructed to ensure that good problem-solvers would commit at least some errors, and that poor problem-solvers would still be able to solve a substantial number of trials correctly. As planning task we chose the Tower of London (ToL; Shallice, 1982), to obtain optimal comparability with previous studies. This task allows the precise determination of a cut-off value where participants start to fail finding a correct solution. Moreover, the ToL task consists of a mental preplanning and a movement execution phase. The use of an MRI-compatible computer mouse allowed us to examine cognitive processing during the motor realization of problem solving for the first time ever in an fMRI-study.
![]() |
Materials and Methods |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Two months before the imaging session, 59 students were assessed regarding their performance in the ToL task. Selection of participants for the imaging study was based on this testing in order to ensure a wide and equally distributed variety of individual performance. Another criterion for selection was that both good and poor performers were able to solve six move problems in a defined predetermined time window, without significant differences in planning time for good and poor performers. The mean ± SD preplanning time for good performers was 13451 ± 4790 ms, and for poor performers 15864 ± 2590 ms. These times did not differ reliably (P = 0.17).
Finally, 22 right-handed students (12 male, 10 female; aged 22.5 ± 3.5 years) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision (contact lenses) and normal audition participated in the imaging experiment. None of the volunteers had any history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, or of significant drug abuse. All subjects gave informed consent prior to participation in the study, and all procedures complied with both university and hospital ethical approval (research project no. 35/2001). Participants were paid 25.
MRI-compatible Computer Mouse
This special, wedge-shaped mouse (ESINOMED GmbH, Prutting) has a small joystick which is operated with the thumb to move the cursor across the screen. At the front side of the mouse, a touch-sensitive button allows the selection of a ball with the index finger, while a second touch uncouples the ball from the cursor.
Cognitive Paradigm
Participants were administered a computerized version of the Tower of London (ToL; Shallice, 1982). In this version, the goal state is presented in the upper field of the screen, while participants operate on the start state in the lower half of the screen by moving the balls with the computer mouse. Participants were told to transform the start state into the goal state while following three rules: (i) only one ball may be moved at a time; (ii) a ball cannot be moved when another ball is lying on top of it; and (iii) three balls may be placed on the tallest peg, two balls on the middle peg, and one ball on the shortest peg. For all problems, the minimum number of moves was indicated on the screen. In addition to the description of the ToL, participants received instructions to (i) always plan ahead the optimal solution to a given problem first and (ii) only thereafter to start moving the balls. The problem set employed in the present imaging study (see Appendix) ranged from four- to six-move problems. Problems differed from those used in the preliminary testing session. Concerning problem structure, only structurally unique problems (Berg and Byrd, 2002
) with a single path for achieving an optimal solution were selected. Across the levels of minimum number of moves, ToL problems were counter-balanced according to the following global problem parameters: the ambiguity of goal hierarchy and the patterns of subgoal generation (Kaller et al., 2004
). In addition, existence and number of suboptimal alternatives were also controlled for. Furthermore, problems featuring specific parameters (e.g. counterintuitive subgoal moves, alternative goal moves) were not included in the problem set. Controlled selection of an individual iso-form (Berg and Byrd, 2002
) for each selected structurally unique problem ensured for all problems that within the sequences of the first as well as the last three moves, 95.8 and 97.9% of the inherent two-move sequences were unique, respectively. This was done in order to prevent potential learning of state-move contingencies across problems.
The final problem set comprised 24 ToL problems (eight four-, five-, and six-move problems each). Since task difficulty and cognitive demands also vary within the particular levels of minimum number of moves according to structural problem parameters (Kaller et al., 2004), the systematic item selection applied in the present study allowed for a controlled increase of problem difficulty ranging from very easy four- to very difficult six-move problems.
Regarding the performance measure, empirical problem difficulty was estimated for each problem and found to reflect closely the variety in problem difficulty especially for problems with the same minimum number of moves. For every participant, individual performance was then calculated on the basis of the empirical problem difficulty of the correctly solved trials.
Experimental Design
Four stimulus conditions were applied in a blocked design: a problem solving condition and three different baseline conditions (rest, visual and motor).
The problem solving condition consisted of an initial planning phase and a subsequent movement execution phase. Participants were instructed first to solve the ToL problem mentally and to repeat the trial in mind if they found a solution before the end of the planning phase which lasted 24 s and was followed by a movement execution phase of 32 s. Within the latter, the cursor appeared in the middle of the screen and participants were asked to execute the movements according to their preplanned solution.
In the rest condition, participants were instructed to look at a black cross in the middle of a gray screen. The visual reference condition was implemented to control for the effects of visual stimulation by presenting two identical tower structures in the upper and lower half of the screen. Participants simply had to match for slight differences in the brightness of the color of the balls without performing any response. In the motor reference condition, subjects were requested to assemble and disassemble the tower structures in the lower half of the screen in a simple, predetermined way avoiding any self-generated planning strategies. Each of the three baseline conditions was presented eight times, with each presentation lasting 24 s.
During the imaging session, the arrangement of the four conditions was pseudo-randomized according to the following restrictions: (i) a problem solving condition must not be followed by another problem solving condition with the equal minimum number of moves; (ii) a baseline condition must not be followed by the same baseline condition. For every participant, the assignment and order of the 24 ToL problems was randomized. However, to avoid differences of general task difficulty between participants due to unequally distributed sequences of particularly demanding problems, the frequencies of four-, five-, and six-move problems were counter-balanced within the session. For the first six problems, two four-, five-, and six-move problems each were randomly selected from the item pool; for the second six problems, another two four-, five-, and six-move problems each were randomly selected from the pool of the remaining 18 problems, and so on.
Experimental Procedure and Imaging Techniques
Prior to the neuroimaging experiment, experimental procedures were practised outside the scanner in order to ensure that participants were familiar with the fMRI-compatible computer mouse and the experimental conditions. First, subjects practised with the mouse on three-move ToL problems for approximately half an hour. Afterwards, participants were introduced to the four different experimental conditions and practiced for 10 min.
Imaging was performed on a 1.5 T whole-body MR scanner (MAGNETOM Vision, Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany). Using a magnet-compatible video beamer, stimuli were projected onto a screen mounted on the rear of the scanner bore. Visual images were viewed via a mirror system placed on the standard head coil. To avoid excessive motion, neck and side pillows were used to fix participants' heads in the coil. During scanning, participants responded with their right hand using the fMRI compatible computer mouse.
Functional images were collected with a gradient-recalled echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence, allowing the sampling of 30 parallel slices effectively covering the whole brain (TR = 4.12 s, TE = 60 ms, FOV = 256 x 256 mm, FA = 90°, 4 x 4 mm in-plane resolution, 4 mm slice thickness, 0.4 mm interslice gap). A total of 484 functional image volumes were collected lasting 33 min. Four rest scans were collected at the beginning in order for T1-effects to stabilize, and these images were excluded from further analysis. At the end of the experiment, a sagittal T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient echo (MP-RAGE) image of the entire brain (160 slices, TR = 40 ms, TE = 6 ms, FA = 40°, FOV = 256 x 256 mm, voxel size = 1 x 1 x 1 mm) was acquired for purposes of co-registration and normalization during image preprocessing.
Image Preprocessing
All fMRI data were processed and analyzed using the SPM99 software (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). Functional and anatomical images were reoriented in such a way that the anterior commissure corresponded to the origin of the three-dimensional standard coordinate system used in SPM99. The functional images were realigned with respect to the first image of each run, using the realignment procedure of SPM99. The mean image and all other functional images were then co-registered with the anatomical image volume collected for each subject. Parameters for spatial normalization were determined from the anatomical image volumes and applied to the respective subject's co-registered functional image volumes. A new set of normalized functional images was created for which the original spatial resolution of the EPI volumes was interpolated from 4 x 4 x 4 mm to a voxel size of 2 x 2 x 2 mm. The realigned and normalized images were finally smoothed with a 6 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. Areas were labeled using the nomenclature of Talairach and Tournoux (1988) and Brodmann (1909)
by use of the Talairach Daemon (Lancaster et al., 2000
).
Model Specification and Statistical Inference
Single-subject data were modelled with a box-car function convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function, removing low-frequency noise with a high-pass filter. The function contained the appropriately placed models of the hemodynamic response for the three baseline conditions, and for four-, five-, and six-move problems in the planning and movement execution phases. For analysis of trial-correctness, six-move problems were further differentiated into correctly and erroneously solved trials. The six sets of realignment parameters were included in the model to account for undesired effects of head movements. Single-subject contrast images were then set up for all planning and movement execution conditions, using the respective four-move planning or execution phase as reference.
For group analyses, the single subject contrast images were entered into SPM99 random effects models, using simple t-tests for categorical comparisons and simple regression for assessing the correlation of individual performance scores and change in hemodynamic activity. Given the engagement of many different processes and the extended time-scale of measurement in the blocked design, all inferences were performed at cluster level (P < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons across the whole brain; Friston et al., 1996).
![]() |
Results |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Inside the scanner, participants solved 94% (range 78) of four-, 77% (range 48) of five-, and 57% (range 27) of six-move problems. The numbers in parentheses indicate the number of correctly solved trials out of eight possible. Subjects' overall performance ranged from 24 to 81% on the weighted performance score. To ensure that participants in the pilot study showed similar performances compared to those tested in the scanner, a correlation analysis was carried out, which revealed highly reliable results (r = 0.68; P < 0.001). There was no significant difference in movement execution time between good (20 682 ± 1829 ms) and poor problem solvers (21 861 ± 1981 ms; P = 0.163).
Planning-related Brain Activations
To limit large-scale brain activation to units relevant for complex planning, and to compare different levels of difficulty, we analysed the brain activity elicited by complex six- versus easy four-move problems. On average, 50% of the six-move problems were solved correctly, providing an adequate number of trials for examination of individual performance level, trial correctness and their interaction. In contrast, four-move problems were solved by nearly all participants and thus serve as an adequate reference condition.
Figure 1a shows the overall brain activation of six- versus four-move problems during the planning phase. Significant bilateral activations can be seen in the prefrontal cortex, in lateral and mesial parietal regions, in the cingulate cortex, the basal ganglia, thalamus, and cerebellum.
|
Regression analyses revealed a linear positive relationship between individual performance level and strength of activation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 9, 10), with the main extension in the right middle gyrus (Fig. 2a; Table 1). Better problem-solvers also showed increased activation in right superior temporal and inferior parietal regions (BA 22, 40). No regions showed a significant negative relationship with performance.
|
|
No significant voxels were found when comparing the brain activation elicited by incorrectly and correctly solved problems, in both directions. We did not find any significant activations at a threshold of 0.001 uncorrected, either. Additional direct comparisons of correctly and erroneously solved six-move problems (without being contrasted to four-move problems) also failed to reach significance.
Combining Individual Performance Level and Trial-correctness
Analyses were performed using the following models: Correctly and erroneously solved problems were separately correlated with performance level, to test for either positive or negative relationships with brain activation. During erroneously solved trials, we found a strong positive relationship of performance level and activation of the anterior cingulate cortex bilaterally (BA 32) and the medial wall of the prefrontal cortex bilaterally. These activations were slightly more pronounced in the right hemisphere, and extended into the dorsal prefrontal cortex (BA 9; Fig. 2b, Table 2). No negative relationship between performance level and brain activation was found. For correctly solved trials, performance level was significantly positively correlated with activation in the right posterior thalamus, more precisely in the pulvinar, while no negative relationships were observed.
|
The overall activation patterns of movement execution are shown in Figure 1b. As expected, in comparison to the planning condition, additional activations were detected in the left precentral gyrus and in the midbrain. In analogy to the planning conditions, all subsequent analyses were carried out on the activations elicited by six- versus four-move problems.
Individual Performance Level
When correlating performance level with brain activation during the execution phase, significant activation peaks emerged in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 9; Fig. 2c, Table 3) and in the right parahippocampal and lingual cortex (BA 30, 19). No significant negative relationships were observed.
|
When subtracting incorrect from correct solutions during movement execution, a significant difference in activation maxima (cluster size 326 voxels) was found in the border between the posterior cingulate and the precuneus (x = 0, y = 42, z = 34; BA 23, BA 2931). In contrast erroneously solved trials elicited more activation than correctly solved trials in the prefrontal cortices (BA 6, 8, 9, 46) bilaterally and in the left precuneus (BA 31), extending from the superior occipital and middle temporal gyrus to the inferior parietal cortex (BA 19, 39; Fig. 2d, Table 4).
|
During erroneously solved trials, we found a significant positive relationship of performance level with activation of the right medial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 9, 10), and of posterior cortical areas including the cerebellum and the lingual and parahippocampal gyri (BA 19, 30; Table 5). No significant negative relationships were detected. We found no significant positive or negative relationships of performance level and brain activation for correctly solved trials.
|
![]() |
Discussion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Individual Performance Level
In the planning condition, better problem-solvers showed an increased activation in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) as well as in the right superior temporal and inferior parietal regions. The activation in the frontal cortex reached its maximum at the mid-dorsolateral part of the prefrontal cortex (BA 9), even though the activation extended into the medial wall. This pattern is well in line with previous neuroimaging (Owen et al., 1996), patient (Burgess et al., 2000
), animal lesion (Petrides, 1994
), and computational modeling studies (Newman et al., 2003
), which suggest a strong role of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the generation of plans for complex visuo-spatial tasks (Dagher et al., 1999
; van den Heuvel et al., 2003
). As direct evidence for this claim, our study shows a relation of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity and performance level during planning, supporting the view that right DLPFC function is a critical determinant of planning efficiency. Note that simple subtraction analysis of difficult and easy problem-solving trials revealed bilateral DLPFC activation. Thus, the left prefrontal cortex seems to be generally involved in planning, but does not seem to determine the planning performance in healthy adults. Cazalis et al. (2003)
reported that superior performers on the Tower of London task showed a significantly more spatially extended activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex than did standard performers. When we checked for the left prefrontal dorsolateral brain activation, we detected a cluster (cluster size 16; x = 32; y = 36; z = 32) that was the homologue to the right dorsolateral activation in the regression analyses. However, this cluster did not reach significance even at an uncorrected level (P = 0.19).
The central role of right DLPFC activation for planning performance is further underlined by our finding of a significant relationship to performance level during movement execution. The activation maximum for this condition is comparable to the right prefrontal activation in the planning condition, but the cluster extends into the medial wall of BA 9 and BA 10. Note that previous studies have never assessed brain activation during the execution of pre-planned movements; however, computational models suggested that left prefrontal cortex should be primarily involved in the execution of a problem (Newman et al., 2003).
In addition to right DLPFC, other regions showed a context-specific positive correlation with performance level, pointing to a more specific involvement in processes necessitated during phases of problem solving. These regions included right inferior parietal and temporal cortices during planning, and right parahippocampal and lingual gyri during movement execution. It has been proposed that during planning, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex monitors and manipulates information stored in posterior parietal areas, given the extensive connections of both areas (Petrides, 1994; Dagher et al., 1999
). In contrast, other theorists state that the right parietal cortex is mostly involved in attention processes, while the left homologue is supposed to be more important for mental imagery processing (Newman et al., 2003
). The planning-related increase in right inferior parietal activation in the present study is consistent with both these interpretations, and may reflect more pronounced visuospatial attentional processing, a more intensive recourse on stored visual information needed for the task, or a combination of both (Jonides et al., 1993
; Formisano et al., 2002
; Sack et al., 2002
). In contrast, the engagement of right parahippocampal and lingual gyri during movement execution probably reflects these increased demands on spatial working memory, when the successive spatial positions of single balls have to be recalled and integrated into the overall movement plan. This interpretation is supported by findings that lesions in these structures cause marked spatial memory deficits (Ploner et al., 2000
), and that right parahippocampal and lingual gyri seem to respond preferentially to stimuli that depict spatial and topographical information about the environment (Gorno-Tempini and Price, 2001
). Moreover, a recent fMRI study found activation of right parahippocampal regions during a comparable high-level cognitive task, reasoning about spatial relations between geometrical objects (Ruff et al., 2003
).
Trial-correctness
In our analysis of planning-related brain activation, we did not find any significant differences for correctly and incorrectly solved problems across participants. While this pattern is certainly surprising, it underlines that the relationship observed between performance level and brain activation during planning is not an artefact of trial-related error-processing, as has been possible in previous studies.
However, during the movement execution phase, incorrectly solved trials elicited more widespread activations than correctly solved problems in bilateral premotor and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, as well as in the left precuneus and the temporo-parieto-occipital region. These activations resemble the pattern observed during the overall planning condition (Fig. 1a), raising the suspicion that participants simply began to re-plan once they recognised they could not solve a problem correctly. Such re-planning should result in longer movement execution times for incorrect than for correct trials, which were confirmed in post hoc analyses (correctly solved trials, mean = 14.2 s; erroneously solved trials, mean = 16.5 s; P = 0.003). However, a striking difference to planning-related activation during erroneous movement execution trials was that no significant activations were detected in the right posterior hemisphere, but in the left inferior posterior parietal cortex instead. This may indicate that during execution and re-planning of erroneous trials, increasingly verbal strategies are applied to solve the problem.
Combining Trial-correctness and Individual Performance Level
The combined analysis of individual and item-related performance level revealed that for planning during erroneous trials only, there was a positive relationship between individual performance level and bilateral activation of the anterior cingulate cortex and the medial wall of the prefrontal cortex. Note that during the execution phase, in contrast, we observed similar correlations between performance level and brain activity as in the analysis of all movement execution trials, underlining the specifity of the ACC activation for planning.
Prominent theories (Bush et al., 2000; Botvinick et al., 2001
) summarize the functions of the anterior cingulate as relating to response override, underdetermined responding, and error commission. Interestingly, these authors describe the anterior cingulate activation found for planning tasks as being related to competition or conflict among alternative actions. Competition between response alternatives should thus be a key feature of correctly and incorrectly solved trials, and should be present for both superior and inferior problem solvers. This is supported by the results of the comparison of difficult six-move and easy four-move problems. However, our data also suggest that the cingulate additionally plays a crucial role during planning for erroneous trials, specifically for good problem-solvers.
One might argue that better problem-solvers are aware of their disability to solve a problem, and try to resolve this conflict with special strategies expressed in the additional activation of the anterior cingulate. Interestingly, the awareness of the existing conflict and the additional effort to solve the problem does not seem to succeed, since cingulate activity was only observed in erroneously solved trials. Alternatively, the activation pattern we observed might not be directly associated with the notion of an error, but rather might reflect the ongoing efforts to correct it by re-planning. In this context, the ACC may either signal the need for further resource allocation, or participate in the resolution of a conflict between response alternatives when faced with uncertainty in the problem-space (Bush et al., 2000; Botvinick et al., 2001
; Ruff et al., 2001
).
Irrespective of these hypotheses for why only good problem solvers show additional activation of the ACC during erroneous planning, it is interesting to note that the location of ACC activation found in the present study is different from those locations commonly found in studies of error processing. In speeded motor response paradigms, error-related activation is mostly observed in the motor cingulate area (BA 24c'; Ullsperger and von Cramon, 2001), while the cingulate activity during erroneous trials in our planning condition was located more dorsally, and adjacent to the anterior prefrontal cortex including BA 9. It is well-known that the ACC can be subdivided into several different regions according to preferential processing of emotional, motor or cognitive information (Bush et al., 2000
). Error-related processing during higher cognitive functions may thus take place in the segments of the ACC anatomically related to the cortical regions involved in the specific cognitive processes carried out (Stephan et al., 2003
). The performance-related activations of the ACC subunit and the right lateral PFC in the present study support such a process-specific model.
![]() |
Conclusion |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Interestingly, no significant negative relationships were obtained when correlating brain activation with performance level. One might argue that there is only one alternative to solve a given problem correctly, while there are many ways to bring the problem to an end in a suboptimal manner.
![]() |
Appendix The Tower of London Problem Set |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|
Address correspondence to Dr J.M. Unterrainer, Neuropsychology, Department of Psychology, University of Freiburg, Engelbergerstraße 41, D-79085 Freiburg i. Br., Germany. Email: josef.unterrainer{at}psychologie.uni-freiburg.de.
![]() |
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS |
---|
![]() |
References |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Berg KW, Byrd D (2002) The Tower of London spatial problem-solving task: enhancing clinical and research implementation. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 24:586604.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD (2001) Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev 108:624652.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Brodmann K (1909) Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Großhirnrinde. Leipzig: Barth.
Burgess PW, Veitch E, de Lacy CoA, Shallice T (2000) The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking. Neuropsychologia 38:848863.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI (2000) Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 4:215222.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Cazalis F, Valabregue R, Pelegrini-Issac M, Asloun S, Robbins TW, Granon S (2003) Individual differences in prefrontal cortical activation on the Tower of London planning task: implication for effortful processing. Eur J Neurosci 17:22192225.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Chen X, Zhang D, Zhang X, Li Z, Meng X, He S, Hu X (2003) A functional MRI study of high-level cognition. II. The game of GO. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 16:3237.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Christoff K, Prabhakaran V, Dorfman J, Zhao Z, Kroger JK, Holyoak KJ, Gabrieli JD (2001) Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relational integration during reasoning. Neuroimage 14:11361149.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Dagher A, Owen AM, Boecker H, Brooks DJ (1999) Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. Brain 122:19731987.
Falkenstein M, Hohnsbein J, Hoormann J, Blanke L (1991) Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 78:447455.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Formisano E, Linden DE, Di Salle F, Trojano L, Esposito F, Sack AT, Grossi D, Zanella FE, Goebel R (2002) Tracking the mind's image in the brain I: time-resolved fMRI during visuospatial mental imagery. Neuron 35:185194.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Friston KJ, Holmes A, Poline JB, Price CJ, Frith CD (1996) Detecting activations in PET and fMRI: levels of inference and power. Neuroimage 4:223235.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Gehring WJ, Goss B, Coles MGH, Meyer DE, Donchin E (1993) A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychol Sci 4:385390.[ISI]
Gorno-Tempini ML, Price CJ (2001) Identification of famous faces and buildings: a functional neuroimaging study of semantically unique items. Brain 124:20872097.
Jonides J, Smith EE, Koeppe RA, Awh E, Minoshima S, Mintun MA (1993) Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET. Nature 363:623625.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Kaller CP, Unterrainer JM, Rahm B, Halsband U (2004) The impact of problem structure on planning: insights from the Tower of London task. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res (in press).
Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, Kochunov PV, Nickerson D, Mikiten SA, Fox PT (2000) Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hum Brain Mapp 10:120131.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Lazeron RH, Rombouts SA, Machielsen WC, Scheltens P, Witter MP, Uylings HB, Barkhof F (2000) Visualizing brain activation during planning: the Tower of London test adapted for functional MR imaging. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 21:14071414.
Menon V, Adleman NE, White CD, Glover GH, Reiss AL (2001) Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Hum Brain Mapp 12:131143.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Newman SD, Carpenter PA, Varma S, Just MA (2003) Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia 41:16681682.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Nichelli P, Grafman J, Pietrini P, Alway D, Carton JC, Miletich R (1994) Brain activity in chess playing. Nature 369:191.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Owen AM, Doyon J, Petrides M, Evans AC (1996) Planning and spatial working memory: a positron emission tomography study in humans. Eur J Neurosci 8:353364.[ISI][Medline]
Petrides M (1994) Frontal lobes and working memory: evidence from investigations of the effects of cortical excisions in nonhuman primates. In: Handbook of neuropsychology (Boller F, Grafman J, eds), pp. 5982. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Ploner CJ, Gaymard BM, Rivaud-Pechoux S, Baulac M, Clemenceau S, Samson S, Pierrot-Deseilligny C (2000) Lesions affecting the parahippocampal cortex yield spatial memory deficits in humans. Cereb Cortex 10:12111216.
Prabhakaran V, Smith JA, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD (1997) Neural substrates of fluid reasoning: an fMRI study of neocortical activation during performance of the Raven's Progressive Matrices Test. Cognit Psychol 33:4363.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Prabhakaran V, Rypma B, Gabrieli JD (2001) Neural substrates of mathematical reasoning: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of neocortical activation during performance of the necessary arithmetic operations test. Neuropsychology 15:115127.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Rowe JB, Owen AM, Johnsrude IS, Passingham RE (2001) Imaging the mental components of a planning task. Neuropsychologia 39:315327.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Ruff CC, Woodward TS, Laurens KR, Liddle PF (2001) The role of the anterior cingulate cortex in conflict processing: evidence from reverse stroop interference. Neuroimage 14:11501158.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Ruff CC, Knauff M, Fangmeier T, Spreer J (2003) Reasoning and working memory: common and distinct neuronal processes. Neuropsychologia 41:12411253.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Sack AT, Sperling JM, Prvulovic D, Formisano E, Goebel R, Di Salle F, Dierks T, Linden DE (2002) Tracking the mind's image in the brain. II. Transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals parietal asymmetry in visuospatial imagery. Neuron 35:195204.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Schall U, Johnston P, Lagopoulos J, Juptner M, Jentzen W, Thienel R, Dittmann-Balcar A, Bender S, Ward PB (2003) Functional brain maps of Tower of London performance: a positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage 20:11541161.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Shallice T (1982) Specific impairments of planning. Phil Trans R Soc Lond 298:199209.[ISI][Medline]
Spitz HH, Webster NA, Borys SV (1982) Further studies of the Tower of Hanoi problem-solving performance of retarded young adults and nonretarded children. Dev Psychol 18:922930.[CrossRef][ISI]
Stephan KE, Marshall JC, Friston KJ, Rowe JB, Ritzl A, Zilles K, Fink GR (2003) Lateralized cognitive processes and lateralized task control in the human brain. Science 301:384386.
Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Stuttgart: Thieme-Verlag.
Ullsperger M, von Cramon DY (2001) Subprocesses of performance monitoring: a dissociation of error processing and response competition revealed by event-related fMRI and ERPs. Neuroimage 14:13871401.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
van den Heuvel OA, Groenewegen HJ, Barkhof F, Lazeron RH, van Dyck R, Veltman DJ (2003) Frontostriatal system in planning complexity: a parametric functional magnetic resonance version of Tower of London task. Neuroimage 18:367374.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
|