MRC Cyclotron Unit, Hammersmith Hospital, Du Cane Road, London W12 0NN
With regard to Professor Crow's stimulating commentary (Crow, 2000) on our recent paper (Spence et al, 2000), we write to correct some errors of fact and interpretation which appeared in his article.
In our paper we demonstrated that during word generation people with schizophrenia exhibited a functional disconnection between neural activity in dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices relative to controls, as stated by Crow. People with schizophrenia also exhibited relative overactivity of the precuneus compared to controls and those at genetic risk (not only to controls, as stated by Crow).
Crow criticised our choice of an a priori hypothesis, based as it was on previous studies of word generation (summarised in Table 1 of our paper). We examined the hypothesis that focal or distributed brain dysfunction might provide a trait marker for schizophrenia, specifically implicating the left superior temporal gyrus, and a proposed frontotemporal disconnection (Friston et al, 1995). Crow suggested that we should have specified a dysfunction of right prefrontal cortex, despite the absence of such a finding from previous studies or statistical confirmation of such dysfunction in our data-set. Indeed, despite many papers on the relevance of language to schizophrenia, Crow has never previously hypothesised such a specific region of dysfunction. In fact, although he has provided diagrams of how language must be organised in the brain (e.g. Crow, 1998), it is notable that they exist in isolation from contemporary cognitive neurobiological accounts of the functional anatomy of language; and despite his emphasis on modelling the first rank symptoms of schizophrenia, he has ignored those studies which have specifically addressed the neural correlates of these phenomena (e.g. Spence et al, 1997).
Failure to address contemporary developments exposes Professor Crow's theories to the risk of appearing increasingly anachronistic.
When neuroimaging studies are published in psychiatric journals there is a particular responsibility incumbent on referees to be cautious in their interpretation of these data (Brodie, 1996) and on authors to be rigorous in their application of statistics, lest a false impression be given to clinicians not used to examining such analyses (Spence, 1999). Such responsibilities should also extend to those invited to comment on others' work.
REFERENCES
Brodie, J. D. (1996) Imaging for the clinical psychiatrist: facts, fantasies, and other musings. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 145-149.[Medline]
Crow, T. J. (1998) Nuclear schizophrenic symptoms as a window on the relationship between thought and speech. British Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 303-309.[Abstract]
Crow, T. J. (2000) Invited commentary on:
Functional anatomy of verbal fluency in people with schizophrenia and those at
genetic risk. The genetics of asymmetry and psychosis. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 176,
61-63.
Friston, K. J., Herold, S., Fletcher, P., et al (1995) Abnormal fronto-temporal interactions in schizophrenia. In Biology of Schizophrenia and Affective Diseases (ed. S. J. Watson), pp. 449-481. New York: Raven.
Spence, S. A. (1999) More stringent threshold
needed. American Journal of Psychiatry,
156,
803-804.
Spence, S. A., Brooks, D. J., Hirsch, S. R., et al (1997) A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). Brain, 120, 1997-2011.[Abstract]
Spence, S. A., Liddle, P. F., Stefan, M. D., et al
(2000) Functional anatomy of verbal fluency in people with
schizophrenia and those at genetic risk. Focal dysfunction and distributed
disconnectivity reappraised. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 176,
52-60.
HOME | HELP | FEEDBACK | SUBSCRIPTIONS | ARCHIVE | SEARCH | TABLE OF CONTENTS |
Psychiatric Bulletin | Advances in Psychiatric Treatment | All RCPsych Journals |