Invited commentary on: Abusive experiences and psychiatric morbidity in women primary care attenders{dagger}

Paul E. Mullen, DSc, Professor of Forensic Psychiatry

Monash University, Clinical Director, Victorian Institute of Forensic Mental Health, Thomas Embling Hospital, Locked Bag 10, Fairfield, Victoria 3078, Australia.

Correspondence: Tel: 3 9495 9136; fax: 3 9495 9195; e-mail: paul.mullen{at}dhs.vic.gov.au

{dagger} See pp. 332–339, this issue. Back

Child sexual abuse has come to occupy a position of pre-eminence among the various forms of childhood disadvantage. This priority has had useful social and political consequences in galvanising public concern and driving attempts both to protect children from sexual exploitation and to assist those who fall victim to such abuse. The precedence accorded child sexual abuse has also had negative consequences, socially and scientifically, particularly in distracting attention away from physical and emotional abuse, and from the whole context of disadvantage and neglect in which the sexual abuse often occurs. The research of Jeremy Coid and colleagues (Coid et al, 2003, this issue) deserves praise for contributing to what is now a substantial body of research which has, over the past decade, attempted to return child sexual abuse to its appropriate context as one, not the, form of childhood adversity (Mullen et al, 1993; Finkelhor & Dzinba-Leatherman, 1994; Fergusson et al, 1997; Fleming et al, 1997; Molnar et al, 2001). Their study concludes that a history of child sexual abuse has few associations with adult mental health measures, and specifically no association with depressive and anxiety disorders. This conclusion is in stark contrast to the vast majority of studies on the impact of sexual abuse in childhood on adult mental health, and specifically to studies that have also incorporated statistical controls to take account of other forms of abuse, as well as the family and social background of victims (Mullen et al, 1996; Fleming et al, 1999; Zuravin & Fontanella, 1999; Molnar et al, 2001). If in science in general, and epidemiology in particular, you keep asking the same question over and over again eventually you will, purely from the play of chance, obtain an answer at variance with previous answers. This, however, is probably not a sufficient explanation of these aberrant findings. The question that needs addressing is why this study has failed to go where so many have gone before. The answer is almost certainly to be found in the methodology.

In this area of epidemiological research a rough hierarchy can be constructed of the methods of sampling in ascending order of sophistication and reliability. Case reports are at the bottom, followed by case series, convenience samples and random community samples, with birth cohort and twin studies vying for top spot. Prospective methodologies trump retrospective ones but in this area they are a great rarity (Calam et al, 1998; Horwitz et al, 2001). Studies that measure relevant family and social factors together with other abuse variables, and employ multivariate analysis, are to be preferred. An excellent example of such a study is that of Salter and colleagues, who disentangled the effects of multiple confounding variables on the relationship between child sexual abuse in boys and subsequent sexually aggressive behaviours (Salter et al, 2003). The study of Coid and colleagues falls into the category of a non-random convenience sample, investigated retrospectively, with a range of relevant factors evaluated and entered into a multivariate analysis. This is an acceptable, if not leading-edge, methodology. Caution should, however, be exercised in rejecting on the basis of such a study the conclusions of both more sophisticated studies, and more rigorous studies employing a similar methodology (Hill et al, 2000).

The researchers’ response rate (55%), calculated after excluding 15% of the original sample, was acceptable, if not impressive. The ascertainment of abuse experiences does not appear to be based on any of the established, and standardised, instruments or approaches. The use of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale may not impress readers as an appropriate instrument for eliciting psychopathological symptoms in this population. The reported levels of abuse experiences during adult life, specifically domestic violence (41%), rape (8%) and sexual assault (9%), were unusually high if compared with random community samples. It is in this latter aspect of the study that the difficulty in establishing an association between child sexual abuse and most mental health outcomes may in part reside.

There is no doubt that coexisting abuse and disadvantage may confound the association between child sexual abuse and adverse mental health outcomes. That domestic violence has an even stronger association with mental disorder than does sexual abuse in childhood, and that child and adult abuse interact, has long been documented (Mullen et al, 1988; Follette et al, 1996; Fleming et al, 1999). This creates a particular problem in a population in which over half have a history of domestic violence or adult sexual assault. In such a population one would expect bivariate analysis to demonstrate a significant association between child sexual abuse and depressive and anxiety disorders. This, the authors indicate, was indeed the case, although they do not document the level of that association. When it comes to multivariate analysis the problems of variance stealing, and in particular the influence of the 41% of participants with a history of domestic violence, may well have drowned the still small voice of child sexual abuse. No amount of sophistication in a multivariate analysis will, in my opinion, stop the effects of sexual abuse in childhood being obscured by the presence of so much severe adult abuse.

The paper by Coid et al contains the claim that previous studies have either not measured the confounding effects of childhood physical abuse, or have not included it in a logistic regression model. This is simply not true. Our group was not the first, and certainly not the last, to investigate this. For example, in our 1996 study on a random community sample we demonstrated that it was physical abuse that ceased to retain a significant association with mental health variables in the logistic regression in which child sexual abuse, physical abuse and emotional abuse were all entered. Childhood sexual abuse, however, continued to be associated with being a psychiatric case on the Present State Examination (OR=2.0, 95% CI 1.4–2.9) and with depression in particular (OR=1.8, 95% CI 1.3–2.5). In that study we warned that the long-term effect of physical abuse could have been obscured by variance stealing related to the close relationship of physical abuse to other forms of abuse and adversity in the developmental histories of the participants (Mullen et al, 1996). This becomes an even greater problem when variables related to abuse in adult life are added to the mix. Perhaps a similar warning would have been appropriate with regard to the apparent absence of an effect of child sexual abuse on depressive and anxiety disorders in this study.

In summary, we should welcome studies such as this which examine child sexual abuse in the context of other forms of abuse and adversity, during both childhood and adult life. The weight accorded to any particular conclusion or claim must, however, depend on the methodological sophistication of the study on which it is based in relation to the existing literature. Nobody should conclude on the basis of this paper that a real doubt has now been cast over the association between child sexual abuse and subsequent adverse mental health outcomes. Anyone in any doubt over how substantial the evidence is for this association should consult the report of Andrews and his colleagues to the World Health Organization (Andrews et al, 2002). There are still those who would obscure and obfuscate the dreadful consequences of childhood abuse in general, and child sexual abuse in particular, and it would be shameful if such people were encouraged, however inadvertently, by this study.


   REFERENCES
 TOP
 REFERENCES
 
Andrews, G., Corry, J., Slade, T., et al (2002) Comparative Risk Assessment: Child Sexual Abuse. Final Report. http://www.crufad.com/research/report.pdf/

Calam, R., Horne, L., Glasgow, D., et al (1998) Psychological disturbance and child sexual abuse: a follow up study. Child Abuse and Neglect, 22, 901 –913.[CrossRef][Medline]

Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J. & Lynskey, M. T. (1997) Childhood sexual abuse, adolescent sexual behaviors and sexual revictimization. Child Abuse and Neglect, 21, 789 –803.[CrossRef][Medline]

Finkelhor, D. & Dzinba-Leatherman, J. (1994) Children as victims of violence: a national survey. Paediatrics, 94, 410 –420.[Medline]

Fleming, J., Mullen, P. & Bammer, G. (1997) A study of potential risk factors for sexual abuse in childhood. Child Abuse and Neglect, 23, 791 –802.[CrossRef]

Fleming, J., Mullen, P., Sibthorpe, B., et al (1999) The long-term impact of child sexual abuse in Australian women. Child Abuse and Neglect, 23, 145 –159.[CrossRef][Medline]

Follette, V. M., Polusny, M. A., Bechtle, A. E., et al (1996) Cumulative trauma: the impact of child sexual abuse, adult sexual assault and spouse abuse. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 9, 25 –35.[Medline]

Hill, J., Davis, R., Byatt, M., et al (2000) Childhood sexual abuse and affective symptoms in women: a general population study. Psychological Medicine, 30, 1283 –1291.[CrossRef][Medline]

Horwitz, A.V., Widom, C. S., McLaughlin, J., et al (2001) The impact of childhood abuse and neglect on adult mental health: a prospective study. Journal of Health and Social Behaviour, 42, 184 –201.[Medline]

Molnar, B. E., Berkman, L. F. & Buka, S. L. (2001) Psychopathology, childhood sexual abuse and other childhood adversities: relative links to subsequent suicidal behaviour. Psychological Medicine, 31, 965 –977.[CrossRef][Medline]

Mullen, P. E., Romans-Clarkson, S. E., Walton, V., et al (1988) Impact of sexual and physical abuse on women’s mental health. Lancet, i, 841 –846.

Mullen, P. E., Martin, J. L., Anderson, J. C., et al (1993) Childhood sexual abuse and mental health in adult life. British Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 721 –732.[Abstract]

Mullen, P. E., Martin, J. L., Anderson, J. C., et al (1996) The long-term impact of the physical, emotional, and sexual abuse of children: a community study. Child Abuse and Neglect, 20, 7 –21.[CrossRef][Medline]

Salter, D., McMillan, D., Richards, M., et al (2003) Development of sexually abusive behaviour in sexually victimised males: a longitudinal study. Lancet, 361, 471 –476.[CrossRef][Medline]

Zuravin, S. J. & Fontanella, C. (1999) The relationship between child sexual abuse and major depression among low-income women: a function of growing up experiences? Child Maltreatment: Journal of the American Professional Society on the Abuse of Children, 3, 4 –12.


Related articles in BJP:

Highlights of this issue
ELIZABETH WALSH
BJP 2003 183: 273. [Full Text]  

Abusive experiences and psychiatric morbidity in women primary care attenders
JEREMY COID, ANN PETRUCKEVITCH, WAI-SHAN CHUNG, JO RICHARDSON, STIRLING MOOREY, and GENE FEDER
BJP 2003 183: 332-339. [Abstract] [Full Text]  




This Article
Full Text (PDF)
Submit a response
Alert me when this article is cited
Alert me when eLetters are posted
Alert me if a correction is posted
Services
Email this article to a friend
Related articles in BJP
Similar articles in this journal
Similar articles in PubMed
Alert me to new issues of the journal
Download to citation manager
Google Scholar
Articles by Mullen, P. E.
Articles citing this Article
PubMed
PubMed Citation
Articles by Mullen, P. E.


HOME HELP FEEDBACK SUBSCRIPTIONS ARCHIVE SEARCH TABLE OF CONTENTS
Psychiatric Bulletin Advances in Psychiatric Treatment All RCPsych Journals