University Departments of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurology (Neuropathology), Oxford
Department of Neuropathology, Institute of Psychiatry, London
![]() |
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Correspondence: Dr P.J. Harrison, Neurosciences Building, University Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, Oxford OX37JX
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Aims To investigate synaptophysin gene expression in the cerebral cortex in schizophrenia.
Method The dorsolateral prefrontal (Brodmann area [BA] 9/46), anterior cingulate (BA 24), superior temporal (BA 22) and occipital (BA 17) cortex were studied in two series of brains, totalling 19 cases and 19 controls. Synaptophysin was measured by immunoautoradiography and immunoblotting. Synaptophysin messenger RNA (mRNA) was measured using in situ hybridisation.
Results Synaptophysin was unchanged in schizophrenia, except for a reduction in BA 17 of one brain series. Synaptophysin mRNA was decreased in BA17, and in BA 22 in the women with schizophrenia. No alterations were seen in BA 9/46.
Conclusions Synaptophysin expression is decreased in some cortical areas in schizophrenia. The alterations affect the mRNA more than the protein, and have an unexpected regional distribution. The characteristics of the implied synaptic pathology remain to be determined.
![]() |
INTRODUCTION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
![]() |
METHOD |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|
Tissue collection and processing
From the Oxford series, blocks were taken from the left dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex (Brodmann area (BA) 9/46), primary visual (striate) cortex
(BA 17), superior temporal cortex (BA 22), and anterior cingulate cortex (BA
24), and stored at -70°C. These regions were chosen because three of them
(BA 22, 24 and 9/46) are sites of presumed functional or structural
abnormality in schizophrenia, whereas BA 17 is not. Brains in the London
series were coronally sliced, snap-frozen and stored at -70°C. Blocks from
BA 17, 22 and 9/46 were subsequently dissected from the slices at -20°C,
randomly from either hemisphere, surrounded with embedding compound, and
stored at -70°C. All material was coded and experiments carried out blind
to diagnosis.
From both series, 18-µm cryostat sections were cut for in situ hybridisation (ISH) (to measure synaptophysin mRNA) and for immunoautoradiography (IAR) and immunoblotting (to measure synaptophysin). From BA 9/46 of the Oxford series, autoclaved formalin-fixed wax-embedded sections were also taken, because their better morphology facilitates detailed per cell ISH analysis (Eastwood & Harrison, 1999).
Immunoautoradiography and in situ hybridisation
Methods for IAR (Eastwood &
Harrison, 1995) and ISH (Eastwood et al,
1994,
1995) have been described.
Brief details and modifications are given here.
For IAR, sections were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde. Non-specific binding sites were blocked by incubating the slides for 30 min with 10% normal sheep serum in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) containing 0.3% Triton X-100. Sections were incubated overnight at 4°C with mouse antisynaptophysin antibody (SY38; DAKO, High Wycombe, UK) diluted 1:100 with PBS containing 0.3% Triton X-100 and 1% normal sheep serum. After washing in PBS, sections were incubated for 1 h with 0.2 µCi/ml 35S-labelled sheep anti-mouse immunoglobulin (the secondary antibody) in PBS containing 0.3% Triton X-100 and 1% normal sheep serum. Omission of the primary antibody was used as an experimental control. Sections were apposed to film for 10 days alongside 14C microscales.
The ISH probe was a 39-base oligodeoxynucleotide, 3'-labelled with
-[35S]-deoxyadenosine triphosphate (~ 1200 Ci/mmol).
Triplicate sections were incubated overnight at 34°C in 150 µl of
hybridisation buffer containing 1 x 106 counts/min of
labelled probe and 20 mM dithiothreitol. Post-hybridisation washes were in 150
mmol sodium chloride/15 mmol sodium citrate for 3 x 20 min at 58°C
followed by 2 x 60 min washes at room temperature. Slides were apposed
to Hyperfilm betamax (Amersham) for 21 days together with 14C
microscales. The wax-embedded sections of BA 9/46 were dipped in nuclear track
emulsion, exposed for 3 weeks, developed and stained with cresyl violet. ISH
using the synaptophysin probe in the sense orientation was used as an
experimental control, in addition to previous demonstrations of the probe's
specificity under these conditions (Eastwood et al,
1994,
1995).
Immunoblotting
Immunoblotting (western blotting) was carried out on the BA 9/46 samples of
the London series, to complement the IAR measurements. Twenty cryostat
sections were collected and homogenised directly in 500 µl of suspension
buffer (100 mM sodium chloride, 10 mM Tris-HCl (pH 7.6), 1 mM ethylene diamine
tetraacetate (EDTA)) containing 1 µg/ml aprotinin and 100 µg/ml
phenylmethyl-sulphonyl fluoride and centrifuged for 5 min at 13 000
g. The resulting pellet was re-suspended in the above buffer
containing 1% w/v sodium dodecyl sulphate, boiled for 5 min, spun at 13 000
g for 5 min, and the supernatant collected. The protein concentration
of the resulting crude synaptosomal extracts was determined using the Bradford
method. Pilot studies found a linear relationship between the amount of
protein loaded (2-4.5 µg) and the signal obtained. Hence, 3 µg was
loaded for the main analyses. Protein samples were fractionated by
electrophoresis on a 10% sodium dodecyl sulphate/polyacrylamide gel for 2 h
and transferred onto a polyvinyl difluoride membrane. After blocking of
nonspecific binding sites with 2% bovine serum albumin in PBS for 30 min at
room temperature, membranes were incubated for 1 h with the SY38 antibody
(1:1000) in PBS containing 1% normal sheep serum. Membranes were given three
rinses in PBS/0.3% Triton-X100 before incubation for 1 h with
35S-labelled sheep anti-mouse immunoglobulin at 0.2 µCi/ml in
PBS. After three washes in PBS with 0.3% Triton-X100, membranes were apposed
to film for 48 h alongside 14C microscales. Samples were run in
quadruplicate.
Image and data analysis
For ISH and IAR, film autoradiographic measurements were taken through
three representative strips of the grey matter of each cortical region; for
the immunoblots, the intensity of the 38 kDa band was measured. Values were
corrected for background (defined as the sense probe for ISH, and omission of
primary antibody for IAR) and converted to nCi/g tissue equivalents by
calibrating to the microscales. For analysis of the ISH dipped sections in BA
9/46, grain densities were measured using dark-field microscopy
(Eastwood et al, 1995)
over pyramidal neurons in laminae III, V and VI. The main synaptic targets of
these neurons are, respectively, other cortical areas, subcortical nuclei, and
mediodorsal thalamus.
Statistical analysis
Brain pH (an index of agonal state), postmortem interval, and age can
influence detection of synaptophysin and its mRNA (Eastwood et al,
1994,
1995;
Eastwood & Harrison, 1999).
Their effects were investigated using multiple regression. Analysis of
variance, with pH, post-mortem interval and age as covariates, was used to
test for effects of diagnosis and diagnosis-by-region interactions. Analyses
were carried out for the two series separately, followed by analysis of the
combined data set.
![]() |
RESULTS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|
The regression analyses (Table 2) showed that increasing age, lower pH and longer post-mortem interval were associated with decreased synaptophysin mRNA. In the London series, the abundance of synaptophysin was inversely related to pH and positively related to the post-mortem interval; these correlations were opposite to what had been predicted and are probably chance findings. Nevertheless, the pH and the post-mortem interval were retained as covariates for the IAR analyses.
|
The abundance of synaptophysin mRNA
(Table 3) and synaptophysin
(Table 5) was significantly
higher in the Oxford series than the London series, despite their comparable
demographic and storage characteristics
(Table 1, and data not shown).
Because of this, we computed Z-scores for analysis of the combined series.
(The difference may be explained by other perimortem or tissue processing
variables. Whatever the reason, it illustrates the potential pitfalls if
tissue from cases and controls come from separate sources.)
|
|
|
Synaptophysin mRNA
In the Oxford series there was an overall effect of diagnosis (P
<0.001), and a diagnosis-by-region interaction (P=0.047).
Synaptophysin mRNA was decreased in BA 17 (P=0.002) and BA 22
(P=0.043), with a trend in BA 24 (P=0.06)
(Table 3; compare Figs
1A and
1B). No change was seen in BA
9/46 overall (Table 3), nor in
any of the constituent pyramidal neuron populations measured therein using the
dipped sections (Table 4). In
the London series there was an effect of diagnosis (P=0.019) but no
interaction with region (P=0.2), reflecting the smaller sample size
and absence of BA 24. The decrease of synaptophysin mRNA from BA 17, though of
similar magnitude to that in the Oxford series, was not significant
(P=0.056). In the combined sample there was an effect of diagnosis
(P <0.001), with less synaptophysin mRNA in schizophrenia in BA 17
(P <0.001) and in BA 22 (P=0.012).
In a post hoc analysis there was a diagnosis-by-gender interaction, significant in the Oxford series (P=0.029) and in the combined sample (P=0.006). In BA 22, synaptophysin mRNA was decreased in the brains of women with schizophrenia (P <0.01) but not in the brains of the men, with a similar trend in BA 9/46 (P=0.058; Table 3). In contrast, the effects of schizophrenia on synaptophysin mRNA in BA 17 and BA 24 were similar in both genders.
There were no significant (P <0.01) correlations of synaptophysin mRNA with duration of illness (after partialling out the effect of age), age at onset, or extent of antipsychotic exposure (rated on a threepoint scale).
Synaptophysin
Synaptophysin measured by IAR did not differ between diagnostic groups in
the Oxford (P=0.15) or London (P=0.09) series
(Table 5; compare Figs
1C and
1D). Neither were there
diagnosis-by-region interactions (Oxford: P=0.3; London:
P=0.9). Nevertheless, we examined the data for BA 17 and BA 22
separately because of the loss of the mRNA in those areas. A modest reduction
was seen in BA 17 in the Oxford series (P <0.05) but not in the
London series nor in the combined sample. There were no changes in BA 22.
Immunoblotting corroborated the IAR results, showing synaptophysin to be
unchanged in schizophrenia in BA 9/46 (cases: 686 (178) nCi/g; controls: 680
(95) nCi/g).
No diagnosis-by-gender interactions were seen for synaptophysin. Nor did its abundance correlate with duration of illness, age at onset or medication history.
![]() |
DISCUSSION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
The main positive finding of this study is that synaptophysin mRNA is decreased in some regions of the cerebral cortex (Table 3). The reduction is statistically robust and seen in two series of brains. Antipsychotic treatment is unlikely to have confounded the results, since chronic administration of the drugs to rats leaves the expression of synaptophysin either unaffected or modestly increased (see Harrison, 1999b), and exposure to medication did not correlate with synaptophysin expression here or in previous studies. As such, the basic hypothesis that there is a synaptic abnormality in the cerebral cortex in schizophrenia is supported. Timing and aetiology cannot be determined from a post-mortem study, but the presence of synaptic pathology is consistent with an anomaly in synaptogenesis or synaptic pruning during neurodevelopment (Keshavan et al, 1994; Eastwood et al, 1995). Similarly, despite the relatively large sample size, this present study is underpowered (and limited by the retrospective nature of assessments) to investigate clinicopathological correlations of potential interest, such as between the expression of synaptophysin and the severity of illness or cognitive functioning. The reduced expression of synaptophysin in schizophrenia may well reflect decreased synaptic density, just as in neurodegenerative disorders (Masliah & Terry, 1993; Heffernan et al, 1998), but alternative explanations are possible (e.g. the synapses are smaller, or contain fewer vesicles). Either way, the changes seem likely to be associated with impaired synaptic functioning.
Other aspects of the results were unexpected: first, the distribution of changes, which were most striking in BA 17 (visual cortex) and did not occur in BA 9/46; and second, the mRNA decrements were not accompanied by equivalent reductions by synaptophysin (Table 5). These related anatomical and molecular issues are explored below, since they determine the overall interpretation of the findings.
Synaptophysin expression in the cortex in schizophrenia
Decreased synaptophysin has been reported in BA 9/46 and adjacent
prefrontal regions in schizophrenia in three studies
(Perrone-Bizzozero et al,
1996; Glantz & Lewis,
1997; Karson et al,
1999), but results of a fourth study were negative
(Gabriel et al,
1997), and Honer et al
(1999) could only find a
reduction when subjects dying by suicide were omitted. We failed to find a
change in synaptophysin in BA 9/46 in either brain series, using IAR or
immunoblotting, so that the matter remains unresolved. There are no obvious
characteristics which distinguish the studies which found reductions from
those which did not. The discrepancy is reminiscent of, and might indeed be
related to, the continuing controversy regarding hypofrontality in
schizophrenia (Weinberger & Berman,
1996). There is more consistency regarding synaptophysin mRNA,
since two other studies of prefrontal cortex also found no change
(Rodriguez et al,
1998; Karson et al,
1999). These measurements did not, however, rule out the
possibility of a localised reduction from a sub-population of constituent
neurons. The cellular analysis (Table
4) is informative in this respect, showing that synaptophysin mRNA
expression is maintained in BA 9/46 in schizophrenia in the three major
pyramidal neuron groups.
Synaptophysin and its mRNA were also unchanged in BA 24, in agreement with the findings of Honer et al (1997), but not with the finding of a 25% elevation in an elderly sample (Gabriel et al, 1997). The latter may reflect ongoing age-associated changes in this region in schizophrenia.
Visual symptoms are not a characteristic feature of schizophrenia, and BA 17 was initially included in our studies as an internal control region. In the event, the reduction of synaptophysin expression was greatest and most robust in BA 17 (Tables 3 and 5). The implication that this region is not entirely normal, at least histologically, in schizophrenia is supported by neuronal morphometric data (see Selemon & Goldman-Rakic, 1999) and one previous synaptophysin study (Perrone-Bizzozero et al, 1996). Moreover, we have found other mRNAs to be reduced in BA 17 (Burnet et al, 1996; Eastwood & Harrison, 1998), with a trend for the total mRNA content to be decreased (Harrison et al, 1997), implying that gene expression in this region is non-specifically impaired in schizophrenia. The origin and consequences of this are obscure.
The reduction in synaptophysin mRNA in BA 22 in brains of schizophrenia cases was limited to women. As this was not predicted, the observation must be replicated before suggesting that there are gender differences in synaptic pathology in schizophrenia. However, we note that diagnosis-by-gender interactions have been reported in other recent morphometric studies of schizophrenia (Highley et al, 1998), and this should be investigated further.
Interpreting differential changes in synaptophysin mRNA and
protein
Interpreting the significance of a loss of mRNA when the level of encoded
protein is preserved is not straightforward. It might be due to an altered
balance of transcriptional v. translational gene regulation (see
Burnet et al, 1996).
For example, only a small fraction of an mRNA is translationally active and
being used for protein synthesis. Perhaps the proportion of
active synaptophysin mRNA is increased in schizophrenia,
compensating for the decreased total amount. Or it may be that the methods
used are more sensitive in detecting changes in abundance of an mRNA than of a
protein. A further possibility that the mRNA levels are simply more
labile and susceptible to confounding variables (e.g. pH or post-mortem
interval) is not likely, given that these factors were matched between
groups and covaried for in the analyses.
For pre-synaptic proteins there is an additional issue, first raised by a similar finding of a greater decrease of mRNA than protein in the hippocampus (Eastwood & Harrison, 1995; Eastwood et al, 1995). An mRNA is found only in the neuronal cell body, whereas the protein is in the axon terminals (Fig. 2). In the case of cortical pyramidal neurons, for example, these are located elsewhere, in other cortical regions, the thalamus, striatum and other subcortical nuclei (Peters & Jones, 1984), where there may be losses of synaptophysin to accompany the decreases in its mRNA identified here. Equally, neurons projecting into one of the measured areas might be overexpressing synaptophysin, compensating for decreased expression by intrinsic neurons. In other words, our mRNA data suggest that neurons in BA 17 and BA 22 are forming fewer, or otherwise aberrant, synaptic connections, but we have not located the affected synapses. Though important, this can only be a partial explanation, since many cortical synapses do arise from interneurons and from axon collaterals of adjacent pyramidal neurons. Further studies are needed to distinguish molecular from anatomical explanations for the greater loss of synaptophysin mRNA than synaptophysin.
|
Further characterisation of synaptic pathology in schizophrenia
Because of the complexities and discrepancies discussed here, it would be
premature to conclude that synaptic abnormalities are a feature of
schizophrenia, or that the characteristics of any synaptic pathology are
known. There is, however, enough evidence to warrant further studies.
Since synaptophysin is present in virtually all synapses, its expression cannot reveal whether subtypes defined by phenotype (e.g. glutamatergic v. GABA-ergic synapses) or by subcellular location (e.g. axo-spinous v. axo-somatic synapses) are differentially affected in a disease. Neither does the expression of unchanged synaptophysin preclude the existence of discrete alterations in a sub-population of synapses. Indeed, there are already suggestions that in schizophrenia the synaptic pathology in the hippocampus (Harrison & Eastwood, 1998; Young et al, 1998) differs from that in the prefrontal cortex (Aganova & Uranova, 1992; Honer et al, 1997; Thompson et al, 1998; Woo et al, 1998). Molecular dissection as well as regional delineation will, therefore, be needed if the existence and nature of synaptic pathology in the disorder are to be revealed.
![]() |
Clinical Implications and Limitations |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
LIMITATIONS
![]() |
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS |
---|
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn, revised) (DSMIIIR). Washington, DC: APA.
Browning, M. D., Dudek, E. M., Rapier, J. L., et al (1993) Significant reductions in synapsin but not synaptophysin specific activity in the brains of some schizophrenics. Biological Psychiatry, 34, 529-535.[CrossRef][Medline]
Burnet, P.W.J., Eastwood, S. L. & Harrison, P. J. (1996) 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptor mRNAs and binding site densities are differentially altered in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 15, 442-445.[CrossRef][Medline]
Eastwood, S. L. & Harrison, P.J. (1995) Decreased synaptophysin in the medial temporal lobe in schizophrenia demonstrated using immunoautoradiography. Neuroscience, 69, 339-343.[CrossRef][Medline]
Eastwood, S. L. & Harrison, P.J. (1998) Hippocampal and cortical growth-associated protein-43 messenger RNA in schizophrenia. Neuroscience, 86, 437-448.[CrossRef][Medline]
Eastwood, S. L. & Harrison, P. J. (1999) Detection and quantification of hippocampal synaptophysin messenger RNA in schizophrenia using autoclaved, formalin-fixed, paraffin wax-embedded sections. Neuroscience, 93, 99-106.[CrossRef][Medline]
Eastwood, S. L., Burnet, P.W.J., McDonald, B., et al (1994) Synaptophysin gene expression in human brain: A quantitative in situ hybridization and immunocytochemical study. Neuroscience, 59, 881-892.[CrossRef][Medline]
Eastwood, S. L., Burnet, P.W.J., & Harrison, P.J. (1995) Altered synaptophysin expression as a marker of synaptic pathology in schizophrenia. Neuroscience, 66, 309-319.[CrossRef][Medline]
Gabriel, S. M., Haroutunian, V., Powchik, P., et al (1997) Increased concentrations of presynaptic proteins in the cingulate cortex of subjects with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 559-566.[Abstract]
Glantz, L. A. & Lewis, D. A. (1997) Reduction of synaptophysin immunoreactivity in the prefrontal cortex of subjects with schizophrenia regional and diagnostic specificity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 943-952.[Abstract]
Harrison, P. J. (1999a) The
neuropathology of schizophrenia. A critical review of the data and their
interpretation. Brain,
122,
593-624.
Harrison, P.J. (1999b) The neuropathological effects of antipsychotic drugs. Schizophrenia Research, 40, 87-99.[CrossRef][Medline]
Harrison, P.J. & Eastwood, S. L. (1998) Preferential involvement of excitatory neurons in medial temporal lobe in schizophrenia. Lancet, 352, 1669-1673.[CrossRef][Medline]
Harrison, P.J., Heath, P.R., Eastwood, S. L., et al (1995) The relative importance of premortem acidosis and postmortem interval for human brain gene expression studies: Selective mRNA vulnerability and comparison with their encoded proteins. Neuroscience Letters, 200, 151-154.[CrossRef][Medline]
Harrison, P.J., Burnet, P.W.J., Falkai, P., et al (1997) Gene expression and neuronal activity in schizophrenia: a study of polyadenylated mRNA in the hippocampal formation and cerebral cortex. Schizophrenia Research, 26, 93-102.[CrossRef][Medline]
Heffernan, J. M., Eastwood, S. L., Nagy, Z., et al (1998) Temporal cortex synaptophysin mRNA is reduced in Alzheimer's disease and is negatively correlated with the severity of dementia. Experimental Neurology, 150, 235-239.[CrossRef][Medline]
Highley, J. R., Esiri, M. M., McDonald, B., et al (1998) Anomalies of cerebral asymmetry in schizophrenia interact with gender and age of onset: a post-mortem study. Schizophrenia Research, 34, 13-25.[CrossRef][Medline]
Honer, W. G., Falkai, P., Young, C., et al (1997) Cingulate cortex synaptic terminal proteins and neural cell adhesion molecule in schizophrenia. Neuroscience, 78, 99-110.[CrossRef][Medline]
Honer, W. G., Falkai, P., Chen, C., et al (1999) Synaptic and plasticity-associated proteins in anterior frontal cortex in severe mental illness. Neuroscience, 91, 1247-1255.[CrossRef][Medline]
Karson, C. N., Mrak, R. E., Schluterman, K. O., et al (1999) Alterations in synaptic proteins and their encoding mRNAs in prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia: a possible neurochemical basis for hypofrontality. Molecular Psychiatry, 4, 39-45.[CrossRef][Medline]
Keshavan, M. S., Anderson, S. & Pettegrew, J. W. (1994) Is schizophrenia due to excessive synaptic pruning in the prefrontal cortex the Feinberg hypothesis revisited. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 28, 239-265.[CrossRef][Medline]
Lawrie, S. M. & Abukmeil, S. S. (1998) Brain abnormality in schizophrenia. A systematic and quantitative review of volumetric magnetic resonance imaging studies. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 110-120.[Abstract]
Masliah, E. & Terry, R. D. (1993) The role of synaptic proteins in the pathogenesis of disorders of the central nervous system. Brain Pathology, 3, 77-85.[Medline]
Perrone-Bizzozero, N. I., Sower, A. C., Bird, E. D., et
al (1996) Levels of the growth-associated protein GAP-43
are selectively increased in association cortices in schizophrenia.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 93,
14182-14187.
Peters, A. & Jones, E. G. (1984) Cerebral Cortex. Vol. 1. Cellular Components of the Cerebral Cortex. New York: Plenum Press.
Rodriguez, R. M., Weickert, C. S., Herman, M. M., et al (1998) Synaptophysin mRNA in normal postnatal prefrontal cortex development and in schizophrenic versus normal control. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 24, 987.
Selemon, L. D. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1999) The reduced neuropil hypothesis: a circuit based model of schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 17-25.[CrossRef][Medline]
Tcherepanov, A. A. & Sokolov, B. P. (1997) Age-related abnormalities in expression of mRNAs encoding synapsin 1A, synapsin 1B, and synaptophysin in the temporal cortex of schizophrenics. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 49, 639-644.[CrossRef][Medline]
Thompson, P. M., Sower, A. C. & Perrone-Bizzozero, N. I. (1998) Altered levels of the synaptosomal associated protein SNAP-25 in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 43, 239-243.[CrossRef][Medline]
Vawter, M. P., Howard, A. L., Hyde, T. M., et al (1999) Alterations of hippocampal secreted N-CAM in bipolar disorder and synaptophysin in schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry, 4, 467-475.[CrossRef][Medline]
Weinberger, D. R. & Berman, K. F. (1996) Prefrontal function in schizophrenia: confounds and controversies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 351, 1495-1503.[Medline]
Woo, T. U., Whitehead, R. E., Melchitzky, D. S., et al
(1998) A subclass of prefrontal gamma-aminobutyric acid axon
terminals are selectively altered in schizophrenia. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
95,
5341-5346.
World Health Organization (1992) The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Geneva: WHO.
Young, C. E., Arima, K., Xie, J., et al (1998) SNAP-25 deficit and hippocampal connectivity in schizophrenia. Cerebral Cortex, 8, 261-268.[Abstract]
Received for publication June 28, 1999. Revision received September 28, 1999. Accepted for publication September 28, 1999.