School of Psychology, University of Birmingham
Early Intervention Service, Northern Birmingham Mental Health NHS Trust and School of Psychology, University of Birmingham
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
Correspondence: Lynda Tait, School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK. E-mail: lat726{at}bham.ac.uk
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Aims We examined whether insight, psychotic symptoms or individuals' recovery style (integration v. sealing-over) predicts service engagement.
Method Fifty patients with schizophrenia were assessed during acute psychosis and at 3-month and 6-month follow-ups. Measures included recovery style, psychosis symptoms, insight and service engagement.
Results Sealing-over at 3 months following onset of an episode of psychosis predicted low service engagement at 6 months. Neither insight nor symptoms predicted engagement. The clear shift from integration to sealing-over within the first 3 months was independent of changes in symptoms or insight. Sealing-over between 3 and 6 months was associated with improvement in psychosis symptoms.
Conclusions Recovery style contributed more to engagement than did insight, appears to be dynamic in the short term and is orthogonal to insight. Overall, this study demonstrated the importance of addressing psychological adjustment to psychosis as well as illness status when investigating treatment engagement in people with psychosis.
![]() |
INTRODUCTION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Background
Impaired insight might reflect, in part, a psychological process of denial
as a way of coping with the negative consequences of illness
(Johnson & Orrell, 1996).
Denial of the need for services and refusal to participate in treatment may
represent an avoidance strategy as a means of dealing with the stigmatising
consequences of psychosis. One such coping strategy or recovery style is
sealing-over (McGlashan
et al, 1977), which is a way of coping by minimising the
significance of symptoms and the impact of psychosis and showing a lack of
curiosity about the experience. However, sealing-over has been viewed as
either an epiphenomenon of lack of insight or as synonymous with it (e.g.
Amador et al, 1991;
Dixon et al, 1998).
One study that has addressed this issue found no relationship between insight
and recovery style (Drayton et al,
1998); however, it is possible that recovery style and insight do
have a clinically meaningful relationship. Furthermore, the conceptualisation
of integration and sealing-over suggests that they represent enduring
personality trait characteristics
(McGlashan, 1987), which
implies that they are more or less stable over time. This is a challenge for
therapeutic interventions that aim to help individuals to progress from
sealing-over to integration (e.g. Jackson
et al, 1998). In this study, we aim to determine to what
extent engagement with services arises from impaired insight, symptoms of
psychosis or recovery style; and, in relation to recovery style, whether this
is a stable trait and related to insight.
Hypotheses
![]() |
METHOD |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Design
This was a prospective longitudinal study in which three assessments were
undertaken: during an acute episode of psychosis and at 3 and 6 months
following the initial interview.
Assessments
Recovery style
Recovery style was measured with the Recovery Style Questionnaire (RSQ;
Drayton et al, 1998).
The RSQ is a 39-item self-report measure specifically designed as an
alternative to the lengthy interview measure of recovery style pioneered by
McGlashan et al
(1977). As described by
McGlashan et al, the concept of an integration recovery
style is characterised by acknowledgement of, and curiosity about, the
significance of psychosis and by active attempts to cope in managing the
illness. By contrast, a sealing-over recovery style is
characterised by cognitive and behavioural avoidance of the diagnosis and
experience of psychosis.
The RSQ consists of 13 sub-scales, each containing three questions designed to reflect categories consistent with those developed by McGlashan et al. The 13 sub-scales of the RSQ are: (1) Curiosity, to investigate if the person has curiosity about psychotic experiences and is invested in understanding them; (2) Education, to investigate if the individual treats the psychosis as a source of new information about himself/herself; (3) Optimism, to identify if the individual is able to see positive aspects to having had a psychotic episode; (4) Impact, to evaluate the person's reaction to the psychotic experience having had a strong impact on life; (5) Fear, to identify the individual's attitude toward mental illness; (6) Liking, to identify if the individual liked some of the unusual feelings and ideas experienced during psychosis; (7) Continuity, to analyse the awareness of continuity between thoughts and feelings during the psychosis and emotional conflicts prior and subsequent to the psychotic episode; (8) Ownership, to investigate if the psychosis is experienced by the individual as personal, rather than as alien; (9) Responsibility, to identify if the individual feels responsible for psychotic thoughts and acts; (10) Help-seeking, to evaluate if the individual enlists the help of others in mastering the conflicts related to psychosis; (11) Blame, to investigate if the individual blames the illness on own difficulties in coping with life; (12) Cause, to evaluate if the individual views himself/herself as having had an emotional or nervous breakdown; (13) Satisfaction, to evaluate the person's views that psychosis helped in obtaining satisfaction in life.
Scoring of the RSQ allows the classification of four recovery styles: (1) integration; (2) mixed picture in which integration predominates; (3) mixed picture in which sealing-over predominates; and (4) sealing-over. Higher scores represent sealing-over. The RSQ has been shown to have excellent psychometric properties and was validated against the interview version (Drayton et al, 1998).
Service engagement
Service engagement was measured with the Service Engagement Scale
(Tait et al, 2002).
No special training is required to use this brief measure. The Service
Engagement Scale is a 14-item measure consisting of statements that assess
client engagement with services, which case managers rate on a four-point
Likert scale from not at all or rarely to most of the
time. The total score ranges from a minimum of zero to a maximum of 42.
Higher scores indicate lower engagement. Four sub-scales assess availability
(When a visit is arranged, the client is available),
collaboration (The client actively participates in managing his/her
illness), help-seeking (The client seeks help to prevent a
crisis) and treatment adherence (The client refuses to cooperate
with treatment). The scale has high internal consistency and retest
reliability, including discrimination between criterion groups, in an
assertive outreach team (Tait et
al, 2002).
Insight
Insight was measured with the Insight Scale
(Birchwood et al,
1994). This 8-item self-report scale was designed to be sensitive
to changes in levels of insight, and captures each of three widely accepted
dimensions of insight: perceived need for treatment, awareness of illness and
relabelling of symptoms as pathological. Higher scores indicate greater levels
of insight. The psychometric properties of the scale are excellent and it is
widely used in psychosis research.
Psychotic symptoms
Psychotic symptoms were measured with the SCIPANSS
(Kay et al, 1987). The 30-item SCIPANSS is a widely used, valid and reliable measure for
assessing mental state.
The RSQ, Insight Scale and SCIPANSS were administered at each of the three assessments. Case managers completed the Service Engagement Scale at the 6-month assessment.
Data analysis
One-way ANOVAs, with planned comparisons, were used to assess recovery
style group differences in mean sub-scale and total Service Engagement Scale
scores and mean total insight and PANSS scores at each time point. Pearson
correlations were used to examine associations between engagement scores,
insight and symptoms of psychosis, and Spearman correlations were used to
examine associations between recovery style, insight and PANSS change scores.
For the second hypothesis, repeated-measures ANOVAs were used to determine the
stability of recovery style longitudinally, using the multivariate analysis of
variance (MANOVA) approach (Tabachnick
& Fidell, 2001). Repeated-measures one-way MANOVAs with linear
and quadratic trends over time were performed to examine the patterns of
change in the levels of insight and symptoms of psychosis longitudinally.
![]() |
RESULTS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
No significant differences were found for comparison of those who completed all three assessments with those who were lost to the study on gender, ethnic group, age, living situation or recovery style scores. There were no significant differences in any of the clinically relevant baseline measures between participants and those lost to follow-up (duration of illness, PANSS total scores, insight).
The mean age of the participants was 33.8 years (s.d.=12.0); the group comprised 19 (38%) females and 31 (62%) males. They were predominantly single (70%) and living alone (58%). The mean duration of illness was 7.2 years (s.d.=9.71), with a wide range of 2 days to 36 years, and 68% of the sample had been ill for 5 years or less.
Engagement, recovery style, insight and symptoms
The minimum possible Service Engagement Scale score of zero was scored by
three participants (8.6%); 60% scored above the criterion for
low engagement (11 points or above;
Tait et al,
2002).
To test the first hypothesis that the sealing-over recovery style would predict lower engagement than the integration recovery style a set of one-way between-group ANOVAs with pre-planned contrasts was performed on the Service Engagement Scale. Recovery style scores at the 3-month follow-up were used to predict engagement assessed at the 6-month follow-up. For this analysis, planned comparisons involved comparing the means of the two integration groups combined (integration and mixed-integration) with that of the two sealing-over groups combined (sealing-over and mixed sealing-over). These data are presented in Table 1. It should be noted that high scores are indicative of low engagement. Contrasting individuals with integration recovery styles v. individuals with sealing-over recovery styles on each of the four sub-scales and total scale scores of the Service Engagement Scale indicated significantly lower engagement in the sealing-over recovery style groups relative to the integration recovery style groups (see Table 1).
|
There were significant between-group differences for total Service
Engagement Scale ratings (F(3,31)=8.04, P<0.001,
2=0.44). Thus, recovery style at 3 months after acute
psychosis predicted the level of engagement with services at 6 months. Among
those with sealing-over recovery styles, 6.3% were rated as high
engagement compared with 52.6% of those with integration recovery styles.
Insight measured at 3 months showed no significant correlation with the Service Engagement Scale scores measured at 6 months (r=-0.09, NS). Similarly, the PANSS negative (r=0.16, NS), PANSS positive (r=0.23, NS) and PANSS general psychopathology (r=0.07, NS) scores measured at 3 months showed no significant correlations with the Service Engagement Scale scores measured at 6 months. Thus, Insight Scale and PANSS scores at 3 months did not predict the Service Engagement Scale scores at 6 months. There was also no correlation between Service Engagement Scale scores and Insight Scale and PANSS scores at 6 months or changes in Insight Scale and PANSS scores during the follow-up.
Are changes in insight and symptoms of psychosis linked to recovery
style?
In order to understand further the nature of recovery style, changes in
insight and symptoms of psychosis over time were examined. According to
Birchwood et al
(1994), an insight score of
9-12 represents high insight. During the acute episode, 48% of
participants scored 9-12 on the Insight Scale and the majority of participants
at 3 months (63%) were in the 9-12 range of scores, indicating that level of
insight at each of the data points was high for many individuals.
Mean insight scores at baseline and at 3- and 6-month follow-ups were 7.8
(s.d.=3.2), 9.2 (s.d.=2.0) and 8.8 (s.d.=2.0). Mean total insight scores were
compared across three assessments by one-way repeated-measures MANOVA. The
results revealed a significant main effect for time (Wilks' =0.84,
F(2,40)=3.95, P=0.03,
2=0.17). A quadratic
trend was significant (F(1,41)=7.78, P=0.008,
2=0.16), indicating an increase in insight scores between
baseline and 3 months but then showing a slight mean decrease between 3 and 6
months.
Mean PANSS scores at baseline and at 3- and 6-month follow-ups were 54.2
(s.d.=11.5), 38.8 (s.d.=7.5) and 34.5 (s.d.=4.6). Mean total PANSS scores were
compared across three assessments by one-way repeated-measures MANOVA,
revealing a significant main effect for time (Wilks' =0.24,
F(2,40)=62.38, P=0.001,
2=0.76). There were
also significant linear (F(1,41)=124.74, P=0.001,
2=0.75) and quadratic (F(1,41)=22.08,
P=0.001,
2=0.35) trends, indicating a sharp drop in
symptoms of psychosis between baseline and 3 months, followed by a more
moderate decrease over time between 3 and 6 months, in line with expectations
concerning clinical recovery.
One-way ANOVAs were conducted on the total scores for the PANSS and the Insight Scale in order to test for differences between recovery style groups at each time point. No significant differences emerged between the four recovery style groups for either of the two variables at any time point, with means for each of the recovery style groups being very similar.
No significant correlations emerged when changes in recovery style were plotted against changes in insight at 3 and 6 months, or in PANSS change scores at 3 months. However, the change in psychotic symptoms between 3 and 6 months was correlated (r=-0.34, P=0.03) with the RSQ change scores at 6 months, indicating that improvement in psychotic symptoms was associated with an increased tendency towards sealing-over. These results suggest that insight is independent of recovery style. Thus, no evidence was found to support the notion that sealing-over is synonymous with lack of insight.
Stability of recovery style over time
The most widely used coping styles at baseline were the two integration
recovery styles (72%) but by the 6-month follow-up the most widely used were
the two sealing-over recovery styles (59.5%). To test the second hypothesis,
mean recovery style scores were compared across time by one-way
repeated-measures MANOVA. The mean (s.d.) recovery style scores longitudinally
were: baseline=2.98 (s.d.=1.22), 3 months=3.52 (s.d.=1.38) and 6 months=3.69
(s.d.=1.51). The results of the analysis support the prediction by showing a
large significant main effect of time (Wilks' =0.80,
F(2,40)=5.07, P=0.011,
2=0.20). The results
of the polynomial trend analysis support the prediction by showing that the
linear trend was significant (F(1,41)=10.39, P=0.002,
2=0.20), indicating that recovery style scores increased
across time towards sealing-over. However, very little change in recovery
style occurred between 3 and 6 months.
![]() |
DISCUSSION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Engagement, recovery style, insight and symptoms
The sealing-over recovery styles were associated with considerably lower
service engagement than the integration recovery styles, with 46% more
engagement in the integration groups. Furthermore, recovery style at 3 months
predicted service engagement at 6 months, and there was no relationship
between service engagement and either psychotic symptoms or insight. To recap,
those who endorsed sealing-over recovery styles were rated by their case
managers as more likely to have difficulty in arranging and keeping
appointments, in developing a collaborative relationship with mental health
services staff, in seeking help in a crisis and in adhering to prescribed
treatments. These results support the proposition that coping and adjustment
to psychosis are among the main influences on how service users interact with
statutory services. Those individuals who have avoidance-type coping
behaviours (i.e. sealing-over) in the context of psychosis are more likely
also to avoid or withdraw from care provided by mental health professionals,
whether insight is high or otherwise. Understanding the relationship between
recovery style and engagement will be a step forward in designing
interventions to enhance engagement with services.
Recovery style and insight
There was no evidence to support the notion that sealing-over is synonymous
with lack of insight, consistent with the finding of Drayton et al
(1998). It is possible,
therefore, for patients to seal-over with or without insight, e.g. I
know I am ill but it was just one of those things and I want to forget about
it and move on. One client, for example, had high insight at 6 months
(Insight Scale score=12) but used a sealing-over recovery style (recovery
style score=6) and reported that the illness had little effect on his life.
This client was rated by the case manager as rarely seeking help in a crisis
or taking an active role in managing his illness, as sometimes avoiding
contact and as someone who maintained a low level of adherence to treatment
throughout the study period.
Adjustment to psychosis
The results support the second hypothesis in suggesting that, for a
subgroup, recovery style has a distinct capacity for change, supporting
earlier research that for some people with psychosis the recovery style
appears to measure the psychological adjustment to the illness
(Jackson et al, 1998)
and is inconsistent with earlier suggestions that recovery style may be solely
a trait characteristic (McGlashan,
1987). Recovery style showed clear evidence of moving towards
sealing-over during the early phase of adjustment to an episode of psychosis.
It is important to emphasise that the temporal trend for recovery style was in
the opposite direction to that observed for insight and symptoms (insight and
symptoms improved with time). It is possible that when insight and symptoms
improve, individuals become more aware of the impact and negative consequences
of psychosis, motivating sealing-over. The finding that many individuals adopt
integrating recovery styles initially during active treatment
may reflect attempts to cope constructively with the challenge of the
situation. However, during the course of clinical recovery, the opportunity
for reflection begins, which may explain the clear shift towards
sealing-over.
In previous research (Birchwood et al, 2000a,b) we have demonstrated that individuals' appraisals of psychosis and the implications for their future and identity are critical for recovery and that they predict later depression, particularly the appraisal of loss (I will never be the person I was) or shame (If people find out, I will be humiliated). It is not yet understood why some clients use sealing instead of integrating strategies, although we believe that patients who seal are those who see greater loss and shame in their psychosis, motivating denial (Birchwood et al, 2000b). In previous work, sealers were found to have vulnerable self-esteem and a history of insecure attachment, seemingly unable to face up to the personal impact of a psychosis (Drayton et al, 1998). We are currently exploring this possibility further and how it might interact with parallel processes in relatives.
Methodological issues
The participants in the present study were located in mental health
facilities within urban and inner-city settings. Although there is no a
priori reason to suppose that such settings are different from rural
settings, ethnicity and social disadvantage could, conceivably, influence the
process of adaptation to psychosis. It is important also to bear in mind that
this sample was, in 7/10 cases, within 5 years of onset of illness and the
experimental sampling method identified patients in acute crisis, which will
engage a significant group of multiple relapsers. Although this is
representative of the trajectories of psychosis in general
(Harrison et al,
2001), undoubtedly there will be interactions between the course
of psychosis and individual processes of adaptation; the process of adaptation
begins at the first episode or even earlier. In a nutshell, what we have
observed is a 6-month snapshot of a group of patients who are prone to relapse
where sealing has become an endemic part of their attitude to their illness,
leading to service disengagement and treatment reluctance.
The researcher who conducted the PANSS assessments was not blind to two of the assessments insight and recovery style and this may be a source of potential bias; however, both insight and recovery style were assessed by self-report measures, which were not scored until the conclusion of the study, and this limited the potential for bias.
In general, the rate of attrition in this study was low (16%) and there were no notable differences in demographic, recovery style or clinical data between those who dropped out and those who completed all three assessments. This suggests that little specific bias was introduced by those who dropped out posing a threat to the internal validity of the study. However, it is acknowledged that the 12 individuals who refused to participate might have used sealing-over recovery styles and thus introduced a degree of bias into the final sample studied. We would emphasise, however, that the study included a high percentage of poor engagers and individuals who used sealing-over recovery styles. Thus, we believe that the overall conclusions are valid and that the internal validity of the study was not threatened.
Clinical implications
First and foremost, these data suggest that sealing/integration and not
insight/symptoms lie at the heart of service disengagement and treatment
reluctance. Compliance therapy
(Kemp et al, 1996)
a motivational approach to improving drug adherence may have
achieved its results not through modifying insight but by intervening at the
level of the individual's psychological defences to treatment, which we
believe to be synonymous with the concept of sealing. Notable also in the Kemp
et al study was that the sample characteristics were similar to ours
and samples were recruited at the acute episode.
Second, the naturalistic changes that we have observed in recovery style suggest that providing appropriate support at an early stage may help to maintain an integrating response in recovering from psychosis, and that initial sealing-over might be amenable to change. The evidence from this study and cognitive interventions (Jackson et al, 1998) provides support for this possibility. However, it is likely that multiple factors influence the subjective impact of psychosis and the person's psychological adjustment to it (Jackson et al, 1999). In addition to cognitive appraisal (Birchwood et al, 2000a,b), it is likely that the family response (Patterson et al, 2000) and psychological vulnerabilities (Drayton et al, 1998) influence how well individuals will adjust to psychosis over time.
We are currently examining the relationship between recovery style and several interpersonal and psychological vulnerability variables that may underpin or maintain recovery style. The findings of the current study provide support for the development of interventions such as compliance therapy and cognitive therapy, designed to promote psychological adjustment to psychosis. Efforts to enhance engagement with mental health services may also benefit from an assessment of a person's recovery style. Clinicians may be able to tailor their interventions with patients according to their current recovery style. In this way, engagement, effective coping strategies and successful psychological adjustment may be promoted. Finally, the finding that recovery style is independent of insight suggests that interventions to promote psychological adjustment to psychosis need not wait for full insight to be achieved or for complete remission of symptoms.
![]() |
Clinical Implications and Limitations |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
LIMITATIONS
![]() |
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Birchwood, M., Smith, J., Drury, V., et al (1994) A self-report insight scale for psychosis: reliability, validity and sensitivity to change. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 89, 62-67.[Medline]
Birchwood, M., Iqbal, Z., Chadwick, P., et al
(2000a) Cognitive approach to depression and
suicidal thinking in psychosis. I: Ontogeny of post-psychotic depression.
British Journal of Psychiatry,
177,
516-521.
Birchwood, M., Iqbal, Z., Chadwick, P., et al
(2000b) Cognitive approach to depression and
suicidal thinking in psychosis. 2: Testing the validity of a social ranking
model. British Journal of Psychiatry,
177,
522-528.
Corrigan, P.W., Liberman, R. P. & Engel, J. D. (1990) From non-compliance to collaboration in the treatment of schizophrenia. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 41, 1203-1211.[Medline]
Department of Health (2000) The NHS Plan: a Plan for Investment, a Plan for Reform. London: Department of Health.
Dixon, M., King, S. & Steiger, H. (1998) The contribution of depression and denial towards understanding the unawareness of symptoms in schizophrenic out-patients. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 71, 85-97.[Medline]
Drayton, M., Birchwood, M. & Trower, P. (1998) Early attachment experience and recovery from psychosis. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, 269-284.[Medline]
Ghaemi, S. N. & Pope, H. G. (1994) Lack of insight in psychotic and affective disorders: a review of empirical studies. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 2, 22-33.[Medline]
Harrison, G., Hopper, K., Craig, T., et al
(2001) Recovery from psychotic illness: a 15- and 25-year
international follow-up study. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 178,
506-517.
Jackson, H., McGorry, P., Edwards, J., et al (1998) Cognitively-oriented psychotherapy for early psychosis (COPE). Preliminary results. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172 (suppl. 33), 93-100.
Jackson, H. J., Edwards, J., Hulbert, C., et al (1999) Recovery from psychosis: psychological interventions. In The Recognition and Management of Early Psychosis: a Preventive Approach (eds P. D. McGorry & H. J. Jackson), pp. 265-307. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Johnson, S. & Orrell, M. (1996) Insight, psychosis and ethnicity: a case-note study. Psychological Medicine, 26, 1081-1084.[Medline]
Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A. & Opler, L. A. (1987) The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13, 261-276.[Medline]
Kemp, R., Hayward, P., Applewhaite, G., et al
(1996) Compliance therapy in psychotic patients: randomised
controlled trial. BMJ,
312,
345-349.
McGlashan, T. H. (1987) Recovery style from mental illness and long-term outcome. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 681-685.[Medline]
McGlashan, T. H. & Levy, S. T. (1977) Sealing-over in a therapeutic community. Psychiatry, 40, 55-65.[Medline]
McGlashan, T. H., Wadeson, H. S., Carpenter, W. T., et al (1977) Art and recovery style from psychosis. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 164, 182-190.[Medline]
Patterson, P., Birchwood, M. & Cochrane, R. (2000) Preventing the entrenchment of high expressed emotion in first episode psychosis: early developmental attachment pathways. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 34 (suppl.), S191-S197.[CrossRef][Medline]
Sainsbury Centre for Mental Health (1998) Keys to Engagement: Review of Care for People with Severe Mental Illness who are Hard to Engage with Services. London: Sainsbury Centre Publications.
Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (2001) Using Multivariate Statistics. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Tait, L., Birchwood, M. & Trower, P. (2002) A new scale (SES) to measure engagement with community mental health services. Journal of Mental Health, 11, 191-198.
Trauer, T. & Sacks, T. (2000) The relationship between insight and medication adherence in severely mentally ill clients treated in the community. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 102, 211-216.[CrossRef][Medline]
World Health Organization (1992) The ICD-10 International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (Tenth Revision). Geneva: WHO.
Received for publication May 2, 2002. Revision received October 3, 2002. Accepted for publication October 10, 2002.
Related articles in BJP: