London School of Economics, London
![]() |
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Correspondence: Dr Eileen Munro, London School of Economics, Houghton Street, London WC2A 2AE
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Aims To examine the findings of public inquiries into homicides by people with mental illness to see if they support the claim that better risk assessment would have averted the tragedy.
Method Analysis was made of the findings of the public inquiries between 1988 and 1997 in relation to the predictability and preventability of the homicides.
Results Of the homicides considered by the inquiry panels, 27.5% were judged to have been predictable, 65% preventable and 60% of the patients had a long-term history containing violence or substantial risk factors for violence.
Conclusions Improved risk assessment has only a limited role in reducing homicides. More deaths could be prevented by improved mental health care irrespective of the risk of violence. If services become biased towards those assessed as high risk, then ethical concerns arise about the care of both violent and non-violent patients.
![]() |
INTRODUCTION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
![]() |
METHOD |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
The media attention given to these reports has helped to make violence a central issue in policy debates, but our study questioned to what extent the detailed findings of these inquiries support the current changes in policy. In particular, the analysis focused on the findings relating to whether the homicides were deemed to have been either predictable or preventable and what recommendations were made on reducing violence.
Analysis was made of the findings of the inquiries into homicides by mentally disordered offenders in Britain. Because no official list of inquiries existed, we used information supplied by the Zito Trust, who have been trying to keep a comprehensive record. The sample comprised all reports published between 1988 and 1997: a total of 40 reports. One other report was identified but efforts to obtain a copy were unsuccessful. Only five of these reports were published before 1994 when there was a sharp increase because inquiries after homicides became mandatory to protect the public and patients (NHS Executive, 1994). A large number of inquiries set up since 1994 had still failed to publish a report by 1997, hence our total sample was far lower than the number of homicides by mentally disordered offenders during that period.
As recommended in the guidance from the NHS Executive (1994), all the inquiries examined the following topics:
The inquiry panels varied in the balance of professionals represented, particularly between psychiatry and law. The reports also varied in length and detail and, it might be argued, in quality. However, this study did not re-examine the evidence or criticise the conclusions, but analysed the published findings because it is these, however fallible, that have been influencing policy makers. Analysis was made of the judgements about whether the homicides had been predictable and/or preventable. Inquiries generally made a clear statement about the predictability of the violence. For example, "the inquiry records that there was no evidence to indicate that Mr B was a risk to persons other than himself" (Chapman et al, 1996) or "this relapse was predictable, as was the increased risk of violence that accompanied it" (Crawford et al, 1997).
Most reports also made a judgement about whether the homicide could have been avoided. This included most of the cases where the violence could have been predicted but also some cases where the inquiries concluded that, although the violence was unpredictable, the professionals could have predicted that the patient would become ill again and that if he or she had received a reasonable standard of treatment the homicide would have been prevented. For instance, "we are sympathetic to the fact that Kenneth Grey killing his mother could not be predicted but are convinced that steps could have been taken which might have prevented it from happening" (Mishcon et al, 1995). Reports where the homicide was not judged preventable fell into two categories: those where there was a judgement that they were not preventable and those where there was no clear comment on the matter. For the purpose of our analysis, where the focus was on those considered preventable, these were grouped together and were classified as not judged preventable.
In analysing predictions of risk, attention was also paid to whether the predictive factors were long-standing or recent, because this has relevance for case management. Any risk assessment is related to a particular point in time and patients' ratings will vary over time. So a patient who is classified as very dangerous at one time may, after treatment or changes in his or her environment, be considered a much lower risk. In examining the statements about the predictability of the violence in the reports, there were clear differences in the patients' histories: some had a fluctuating history in which there had been episodes when they were considered high risk; others had never caused significant concern on this point until immediately before the homicide was committed; and some patients had never given any indication that they might be violent, even immediately prior to the homicide. Christopher Clunis, for instance (Ritchie et al, 1994), clearly fitted the first category, with a history of violence and a relapsing illness, whereas Philip McFadden had never been assessed as dangerous in the past but displayed acute indicators of violence in the days preceding the homicide (Aronson & Dyer, 1995). James Stemp, on the other hand, is an example of the third category. He, at no time, presented "as someone who posed a significant risk either to himself or to others" (Adams et al, 1997).
In our analysis, we classified a history of violence or high-risk assessments as long-term indicators of violence, and the signs and symptoms displayed immediately prior to the violence as imminent indicators.
We used the inquiry panel's judgement of whether the patient displayed long-term and/or imminent indicators of violence. This usually coincided with the judgement of professionals involved in the case, but if there was any disagreement it was in that inquiries found more risk indicators.
A characteristic of risk assessments is that they involve estimating a probability a degree of risk. Patients are not classified into two distinct categories of safe and dangerous. A patient who has been extremely violent may never be classified as a zero risk but may be considered safe enough to be released into the community. The Sinclair Report (Lingham et al, 1996) uses the graphic image of a ladder of dangerousness up and down which a patient may move. So, when, in our analysis, a report is classified as having concluded that the homicide was predictable, it means that the inquiry panel thought that the probability of violence, at that time, was high enough to warrant action by professionals to try to avert it.
![]() |
FINDINGS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|
In three of the cases that were considered predictable, the patients had shown signs and symptoms indicative of risk only in the days preceding the homicide. Until that point, there had been no serious concern about their dangerousness in their history. Thirteen patients showed neither long-term nor imminent indicators of risk.
Preventability of the homicide
Twenty-six cases (65%) were considered to have been preventable (see
Fig. 2). This included nine of
the 11 cases deemed to have been predictable. In the other two cases
considered predictable, the high risk of violence had, in fact, been
recognised. In one case (Richardson et
al, 1997), the inquiry agreed with the professionals that
they had no legal power to intervene because the patient did not have a mental
disorder that made him detainable under the Mental Health Act. In the other
case (Aronson & Dyer,
1995), police were sent to the house to take the patient to
hospital and he killed one of them.
|
There was, however, a large group of seventeen cases where inquiries concluded that the homicides had been preventable, but this was not through better risk assessment but through a better level of psychiatric care in general. The violence itself was not predictable but a relapse could have been and, with a reasonable standard of care, should have been predicted so that the patient would not have been in the community in such a disordered state at the time of the homicide. The Norman Dunn inquiry report provides a classic example of this category:
"Two questions arise. The first is could anyone have foreseen that Norman would have been so violent that he caused his mother to die? The answer, the Inquiry is quite certain, must be no, never. The second, however, is could this tragedy have been prevented? The answer, the Inquiry is equally certain, must be yes, most definitely. Those who were professionally responsible and directly concerned in the case of Norman in the community had not noticed the deterioration observed by others. Had they done so, there is little doubt that he would have been admitted to hospital and the symptoms of his illness corrected. Norman would have been spared the further deterioration that occurred, his symptoms would have been more easily resolved, his family would have been spared the burden of coping with him in his ill state, and the attack on Mrs M would never have occurred." (Keating et al, 1997)
Correlating preventability with patient history, 16 (66%) cases of those with long-term indicators and nine (56%) of those with no long-term factors were judged preventable homicides.
![]() |
DISCUSSION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Concern for public safety has been taking political precedence over concern for the welfare of those suffering from mental disorders. With this priority, it might seem, at first glance, plausible to argue that at a time of limited resources it is best to focus them on those patients posing the greatest threat to the public. But these findings show that: those showing long-term signs of dangerousness are only a subsection of those who are violent; even those fitting this category do not always show imminent signs that they are about to be violent; and there are a substantial group of people who display none of the accepted indicators of violence before committing homicide.
![]() |
IMPROVING RISK ASSESSMENT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Further empirical research on indicators of risk may help to improve predictive skills, for example, the large-scale MacArthur Risk Assessment Study now being conducted in the USA is studying the association between risk factors and violent behaviour and aims to improve the validity of clinical risk assessment (Steadman et al, 1996). However, improving accuracy is difficult and it is unrealistic to expect risk assessments to achieve high accuracy.
Empirical studies to improve the knowledge base face methodological difficulties, summarised by Monahan & Steadman (1996):
"Four methodological problems have especially plagued actuarial research on the assessment of the potential of the mentally disordered for violence: inadequacy of cues or factors chosen to forecast whether violence will occur, inability to determine the extent of violence within the population studied, limited applicability of research designs used to validate risk factors, and failure to coordinate research efforts in the field." (Monahan & Steadman, 1996)
The MacArthur Risk Assessment Study is designed to minimise these problems but, although its findings may improve knowledge of risk factors, it is unlikely to lead to highly accurate risk predictions because homicides are so rare. The statistics of predicting relatively rare events, such as violence in those with mental disorders, mean that it is extremely difficult to achieve a predictive system with high accuracy. Even a test with high sensitivity and specificity will produce a high number of false positives (Campbell & Machin, 1990; Munro, 1999).
![]() |
ETHICAL PROBLEMS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
The threshold for professional intervention will vary according to the acceptability of each type of error. The TaylorRussell diagram (Taylor & Russell, 1939) helps to illustrate this. The two axes measure the true degree of danger and the professional assessment of risk. If risk assessment were a perfect science, we would expect cases to fall in a straight line, with real and identified risk being the same (Fig. 3). However, because we only have fallible measures, cases will fall in an ellipse (Fig. 4); the less accurate the diagnostic system, the bigger the scatter. Having estimated the probability of violence, professionals then have to make a decision about the threshold for intervention: how high should the risk be before they intervene? If we draw a line to indicate the threshold, the rate of false positives and false negatives becomes apparent. A low threshold for action produces a high rate of false positives (Fig. 5), whereas, conversely, a high threshold leads to a high number of false negatives (Fig. 6).
|
|
|
|
Decisions about the threshold for intervention are separate from estimations of the probability of a particular outcome. The issue is clearly illustrated in the two standards of proof in legal system. Beyond reasonable doubt is a high threshold, minimising the number of innocent people who will be wrongly convicted, whereas on the balance of probabilities is a lower threshold, reducing the number of false negatives.
In mental health work, pressure from the public and politicians to avoid false negatives (violence by people with mental disorders) is pushing the threshold for action down. This, inevitably, has the logical consequence of increasing the number of false positives: patients inaccurately assessed as dangerous. This raises ethical concerns about erosion of patients' civil liberties, especially as the trend appears to be for increasingly coercive measures both in the community and in hospitals.
At a practical level, lowering the threshold for intervention increases the total number of patients assessed as significantly high risk. In a chronically under-funded service, this has the repercussion of reducing the resources available for patients who do not fall into the category of being potentially violent. Ethical concerns can then be raised about those who are ill but are denied services because they are considered to pose no danger to the public.
![]() |
BALANCING WELFARE AND SAFETY IN CHILD CARE |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Faced with public pressure to avoid any type of error, professionals tried to maximise their accuracy by conducting extremely thorough investigations into any allegations of abuse. This focus on investigations has had two highly undesirable outcomes. It has caused considerable distress and damage to the families, abusive or not, who are subject to them (Farmer & Owen, 1995). It has also absorbed so much of the available resources that other parts of the child welfare system are suffering, even services for the children identified by the investigations as at risk (Audit Commission, 1994; Department of Health, 1995).
![]() |
PUBLIC PERCEPTION OF RISK |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Coid (1996) cites evidence that rates of homicide by mentally disordered people are remarkably similar in all countries, in stark contrast to overall rates of homicide, which vary dramatically. He suggests that, because Britain has a relatively low rate of homicide, those by the mentally disordered stand out more than in other countries and so receive more attention. This certainly fits with the priority given in Britain to examining each case in depth, contrasting with the USA, with a substantially higher homicide rate, where the homicides by mentally disordered receive little official study.
Mental health services have a dual commitment to maximise the welfare of patients and to protect the public from harm. The policy of community care has, for most patients, led to improved quality of life although the level of funding has meant that they have not received an optimum level of care and treatment. Treatment in the community rather than in isolated institutions has, however, made people with mental disorders more visible to the general public. Their behaviour can, at times, appear strange and frightening. They can actually be violent, usually to themselves but very occasionally to others. Their victims are usually relatives or professionals known to them, with only 13% being strangers (Taylor & Gunn, 1999).
As the public inquiries show, there are serious obstacles to increasing public safety by improving risk assessment and targeting services on those deemed potentially violent. Mental health professionals have limited ability to predict rare incidents of violence. However, they have considerable skill in diagnosing and treating mental illness. The public would be better protected by having a good standard of care for all patients.
![]() |
Clinical Implications and Limitations |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
LIMITATIONS
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Appleby, L. (1997) National Confidential Inquiry into Suicide and Homicide by People with Mental Illness, Progress Report. London: Department of Health.
Aronson, S. & Dyer, J. (1995) Report of the Enquiry into the Care and Treatment of Philip McFadden. Edinburgh: Mental Welfare Commission for Scotland.
Audit Commission (1994) Seen but not Heard: Co-ordination Community Child Health and Social Services for Children in Need. London: HMSO.
Campbell, M. & Machin, D. (1990) Medical Statistics. Chichester: Wiley.
Chapman, H., Ashman, M., Oyebode, O., et al (1996) Report of the Independent Inquiry into the Treatment and Care of Richard Burton. Leicester: Leicestershire Health Authority.
Coid, J. (1996) Dangerous patients with mental
illness: increased risks warrant new policies, adequate resources and
appropriate legislation. British Medical Journal,
312,
965-969.
Crawford, L., Devaux, M., Ferris, R., et al (1997) Report into the Care and Treatment of Martin Mursell. London: Camden and Islington Health Authority.
Department of Health (1995) Child Protection: Messages from Research. London: HMSO.
Eastman, N. (1997) The Mental Health (Patients in the Community) Act 1995. British Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 492-496.[Medline]
Farmer, E. & Owen, M. (1995) Child Protection Practice: Private Risks and Public Remedies. London: HMSO.
Hammond, K. (1996) Human Judgement and Social Policy, Irreducible Uncertainty, Inevitable Error, Unavoidable Injustice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Keating, D., Collins, P. & Walmsley, S. (1997) Report of the Independent Inquiry into the Treatment and Care of Norman Dunn. Newcastle: Newcastle and North Tyneside Health Authority.
Leff, J. (ed.) (1997) Care in the Community: Illusion or Reality? Chichester: Wiley.
Lingham, R., Candy, J. & Bray, J. (1996) Report of the Inquiry into the Treatment and Care of Raymond Sinclair. Aylesford: West Kent Health Authority.
Menzies, R., Webster, C., McMain, S., et al (1994) The dimensions of dangerousness revisited: assessing forensic predictions about violence. Law and Human Behaviour, 18, 1-28.
Mishcon, J., Dick, D., Welch, N., et al (1995) The Grey Report: Report of the Independent Inquiry Team into the Care and Treatment of Kenneth Grey. London: East London and the City Health Authority.
Monahan, J. & Steadman, H. (1996) Toward a rejuvenation of risk assessment research. In Violence and Mental Disorder (eds J. Monahan & H. Steadman), pp. 1-17. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Munro, E. (1999) Protecting children in an anxious society. Health, Risk and Society, 1, 117-127.
NHS Executive (1994) Health Service Guidelines HSG (94) 27. London: Department of Health.
Richardson, G., Chiswick, D. & Nutting, I. (1997) Report of the Inquiry into the Treatment and Care of Darren Carr. Reading: Berkshire Health Authority.
Ritchie, J., Dick, D. & Lingham, R. (1994) Report of the Inquiry into the Care and Treatment of Christopher Clunis. North East Thames and South East Thames Regional Health Authorities: HMSO.
Steadman, H., Monahan, J., Appelbaum, P., et al (1996) Designing a new generation of risk assessment research. In Violence and Mental Disorder (eds J. Monahan & H. Steadman), pp. 297-318. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.
Taylor, H. & Russell, J. (1939) The relationship of validity coefficients to the practical application of tests of selection. Journal of Applied Psychology, 23, 565-578.
Taylor, P. J. & Gunn, J. (1999) Homicides by people with mental illness: myth and reality. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 9-14.[Abstract]
Received for publication May 20, 1999. Revision received September 9, 1999. Accepted for publication September 10, 1999.