Distinguishing characteristics of subjects with good and poor early outcome in the Edinburgh High-Risk Study*

EVE C. JOHNSTONE, FRCPsych, RICHARD COSWAY, BSc and STEPHEN M. LAWRIE, MRCPsych

University Department of Psychiatry, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, UK

Correspondence: Eve Johnstone, University Department of Psychiatry, Kennedy Tower, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Morningside Park, Edinburgh EH10 5HF, UK

Declaration of interest This study was financed by the Medical Research Council, which supports S.M.L. and R.C.

* Presented in part at the European First Episode Schizophrenia Network Meeting, Whistler BC, Canada, 27 April 2001. Back


   ABSTRACT
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 METHOD
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 Clinical Implications and...
 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
 REFERENCES
 
Background ‘High-risk’ studies of schizophrenia have the potential to clarify the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Here, results of extreme outcome groups in the Edinburgh High-Risk Study are presented.

Aims To compare groups of good and poor outcome from the Edinburgh High-Risk Study and clarify the nature of the change from the state of vulnerability to that of developing psychosis.

Method The recruitment procedure is described. Good and poor outcome are defined. These groups are compared in terms of genetic liability and of baseline and change in neuropsychology and neuroanatomy.

Results Demographic characteristics and genetic liability do not differ between the groups. The good outcome group perform better at baseline in some neuropsychological tests, but there is little neuroanatomical difference. The poor outcome group show consistently impaired memory function and a tendency to reduction in temporal lobe size.

Conclusions In genetically predisposed subjects, the change from vulnerability to developing psychosis may be marked by a reduced size and impaired function of the temporal lobe.


   INTRODUCTION
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 METHOD
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 Clinical Implications and...
 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
 REFERENCES
 
The biological basis of schizophrenia is poorly understood although genetic factors are known to be important. Individuals who develop schizophrenia may have abnormalities of language (Jones et al, 1994), behaviour (Done et al, 1994) and motor development (Walker et al, 1994) in childhood, but whether these features represent a vulnerability to schizophrenia or are precursors of the disorder is unclear. ‘High-risk’ studies of individuals at enhanced risk of developing schizophrenia could potentially clarify this but have mainly concerned individuals identified in infancy as the children of mothers with schizophrenia and thus extend for decades (Asarnow, 1988; Cornblatt & Obuchowski, 1997). The Edinburgh High-Risk Study (Byrne et al, 1999; Hodges et al, 1999; Lawrie et al, 1999, 2001a, b; Cosway et al, 2000; Johnstone et al, 2000; Miller et al, 2001, 2002) differs from others as the subjects have been recruited as young adults who will pass through the period of maximum risk of developing schizophrenia during the planned 10 years of the study. The investigation concerns young people aged between 16 and 25 years at ascertainment (when they were considered well) who have at least two close blood relatives with schizophrenia. A total of 229 such young people were identified and 162 of them have so far provided data. They were compared with 34 age-and gender-matched well young people, with no family history from the same communities (Hodges et al 1999; Johnstone et al, 2000), and 36 age-matched subjects with first episodes of schizophrenia. The numbers in the control and first-episode groups were chosen to reflect the number of high-risk subjects eventually predicted to develop schizophrenia (approximately 30 individuals). The study has now been in progress for more than 6 years and some results have been presented (Byrne et al, 1999; Lawrie et al, 1999, 2001a,b; Miller et al, 2001, 2002). This report compares those individuals from within the high-risk sample who so far have achieved the best and the worst outcomes.


   METHOD
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 METHOD
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 Clinical Implications and...
 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
 REFERENCES
 
Case ascertainment
The methods of the study have been described in detail in earlier papers. Essentially, subjects were assessed, at ascertainment and every 18 months until they develop schizophrenia or reach the age of 30 years, in terms of the following variables: (a) psychopathology as determined by the Present State Examination (PSE; Wing et al, 1974); (b) structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) (Lawrie et al, 1999, 2001a); and (c) an extensive programme of neuropsychological tests (Byrne et al, 1999). In addition, assessments of social function, personality and behaviour and life events were made (Hodges et al, 1999; Miller et al, 2001, 2002).

Definition of outcome categories
As previously described (Johnstone et al, 2000), to simplify consideration of the psychopathology as determined by PSE, a simplified classification was drawn up on the basis of the PSE profiles whereby a score of 4=Catego S+ together with a clinical diagnosis of schizophrenia; 3=fully rated psychotic symptom(s) 55-92 and/or fully rated behavioural item(s) 128, 129, 135, 136, 137; 2=3, but features partially rated or features 49-54 partially or fully rated and/or 108, 109, 118, 125, 126 fully and 133 partially or fully rated; 1=none of the above, but any other items fully rated; 0=none of the above. For the purposes of this study, those with the best outcome were those who have never achieved any fully rated score on any psychopathological item at PSE on any occasion of assessment (i.e. they always scored 0 on the study score), and who, in addition, had a record of sustained employment (or successful study towards employment) at a level higher or at least as high in terms of the Registrar General's ratings (Her Majesty's Stationery Office, 1991) of social class as their parents. Furthermore, at interview they were noted to have no abnormalities of social presentation and gave an account of unimpaired social performance. Within the context of the high-risk study, these individuals are referred to as the ‘perfects’. Those with the worst outcomes have developed schizophrenia, i.e., they achieved a score of 4 on the study score at the last time of assessment and in addition all fulfilled the diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia according to ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 1993).

Comparisons
The ‘perfects’ and the individuals with newly developed schizophrenia were compared in terms of basic demographics, degree of genetic liability, baseline neuropsychology and neuroanatomy, and in those where there were at least two assessments before development of illness, change in neuropsychology and change in neuroanatomy. It will be appreciated that whereas most of the ‘perfects’ provided at least two assessments the numbers of individuals with newly developed schizophrenia were reduced by the fact that some of them became unwell before the second assessment could be carried out.


   RESULTS
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 METHOD
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 Clinical Implications and...
 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
 REFERENCES
 
There are 24 ‘perfects’, i.e. 13 males and 11 females of mean age 21.2 years at ascertainment (range 16-24). Thirteen high-risk subjects have developed schizophrenia (8 males and 5 females) who at ascertainment were of mean age 20.3 years (range 16-23). This difference in age is not significant.

Genetic liability
Genetic liability was assessed categorically in terms of the numbers of relatives of first and second degree known to be affected but this does not, of course, take account of the entire numbers of relatives that the subjects had, and a continuous measure of genetic liability was devised by Professor Pak Sham at the Institute of Psychiatry. It has been described by Lawrie et al (2001a) and takes account of the total number of relatives ill and well of each subject and their degree of relationship to the high-risk individual. On this scale, a higher score indicates a greater degree of genetic liability. The mean score of the ‘perfects’ was 0.25 (range -0.02 to +0.70) and that of those with new schizophrenia 0.16 (-0.01 to +0.40) but this difference is not significant. In the ‘perfects’, 18 had a genetic liability from the maternal side and 6 from the paternal. As far as those with new schizophrenia are concerned, six are known to have maternal genetic liability and five paternal. In the remaining two cases, it is possible that the inheritance is from both sides, but we do not have complete data on both maternal and paternal branches of these families.

Baseline measures
An extensive programme of neuropsychological tests was carried out at baseline on all entrants to the study and these are compared between the ‘perfects’ and those with new schizophrenia. Many of these tests showed no differences between these two groups (Table 1). Differences that were present were always in the direction that those who were destined to develop schizophrenia performed less well (Table 1). Baseline scans were available on 23 of the perfects and 10 of those destined to develop schizophrenia. Reasons for non-availability include pregnancy as well as reluctance to be scanned. The results are shown in Table 2. The significant difference in whole brain relates to the fact that there are more males in the newly developed schizophrenia group and where correction is made for gender and height, this difference disappears.


View this table:
[in this window]
[in a new window]
 
Table 1 Comparison of baseline neuropsychological test results between the ‘perfects’ and the newly developed schizophrenia group
 

View this table:
[in this window]
[in a new window]
 
Table 2 Whole-brain volumes (cm3) and regional proportions (%), at baseline on the ‘perfects’ and the newly developed schizophrenia group
 

Differences between first and second assessments
We then examined the relationship between the first and second neuropsychological assessment and compared this between the ‘perfects’ and those of the newly developed schizophrenia group on whom we had two assessments (eight cases). The significant findings are shown in Table 3. There is consistently poorer performance in memory tests in those who will develop schizophrenia and an improvement in function in the Stroop tests in those patients but not in the ‘perfects’. All other tests were non-significant. Similarly, we compared the difference between the first and second scan in the ‘perfects’ and those with newly developed schizophrenia for whom two scans were available before they became ill. Most comparisons showed no tendency to significance. In particular, the amygdala—hippocampus, which has shown clear-cut findings such that this is smallest in the control schizophrenia group, next in the generality of the high-risk cases and largest in the normal controls (Lawrie et al, 1999, 2001a), showed no tendency to a difference between the ‘perfects’ and those with new schizophrenia. By contrast, there was an apparent difference in the change in temporal lobe size between scans 1 and 2 (see Table 4). This does not achieve significance because of the small numbers and high variance but is of interest.


View this table:
[in this window]
[in a new window]
 
Table 3 Differences (mean (s.d.)) between first and second neuropsychological assessments in the ‘perfects’ and the newly developed schizophrenia group
 

View this table:
[in this window]
[in a new window]
 
Table 4 Differences between first and second scans in the ‘perfects’ and the newly developed schizophrenia group in terms of volume changes in left and right amygdala—hippocampus and temporal lobes
 


   DISCUSSION
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 METHOD
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 Clinical Implications and...
 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
 REFERENCES
 
This paper presents preliminary findings concerning a comparison between two extreme subgroups of a much larger study. The conclusions that can be drawn are, therefore, tentative. None the less, it is clear that in terms of baseline demographic characteristics the two groups are similar and there is no evidence of greater genetic liability in those who will develop schizophrenia. There are neuropsychological differences at baseline between the two groups, such that the good outcome group perform better in terms of memory and some, but not all, measures of IQ. This is redolent of our previous study of treatment-responsive and treatment-resistant schizophrenia (Lawrie et al, 1995). Frontal (Hayling test) and cingulate (Stroop test) tasks did not significantly differ between the two groups. The relatively low National Adult Reading Test (NART; Nelson, 1982) scores are likely to be because of the subjects' youth. At baseline there were essentially no neuroanatomical differences between the two groups and this contrasts with the baseline differences we have established between the high-risk subjects and both normal and schizophrenia controls (Lawrie et al, 1999). This may well be because of the small size of the groups in the current comparison, in that numbers larger than this are generally required to demonstrate differences between patients with schizophrenia and normal controls (Lawrie & Abukmeil, 1998). Where we have had the opportunity to assess the subjects twice before illness develops in comparison to the ‘perfects’, those who will develop schizophrenia show consistently poor memory function (Table 3). They also show a significant improvement in performance on the Stroop test, but this is not easy to interpret as it results from an initially non-significantly poorer performance.

The interest of the impaired memory function that we see before the manifestation of psychosis in those destined to develop schizophrenia is enhanced by the tendency of these subjects to show a reduction in temporal lobe size over the same period because, of course, memory function is most localisable to the temporal lobe. This finding reflects our earlier result (Cosway et al, 2000) of a pre-psychotic decline in memory. We have already shown that the neuropsychological impairments in subjects at enhanced risk of schizophrenia are widespread and affect many more individuals than are likely to develop the condition (Byrne et al, 1999). We suggest that the findings may indicate that the feature that marks the change from vulnerability to developing psychosis is a reduction in size and impairment of function of the temporal lobe. Cognitive change seems to be a precursor and not a consequence of psychosis in people who have schizophrenia.


   Clinical Implications and Limitations
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 METHOD
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 Clinical Implications and...
 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
 REFERENCES
 
CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

LIMITATIONS


   ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 METHOD
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 Clinical Implications and...
 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
 REFERENCES
 
This study was supported and conducted under the auspices of the ethics committees relevant to the districts in which the subjects lived. We are most grateful to Suheib Abukmeil, Majella Byrne, Bobby Clafferty, Elizabeth Grant, Anne Hodges and Jane Morris who recruited the subjects and conducted some of the assessments, and to Norma Brearley who carefully prepared the manuscript. We greatly appreciate the helpfulness of the subjects and their extended families, general practitioners and psychiatrists throughout Scotland.


   REFERENCES
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 METHOD
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 Clinical Implications and...
 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
 REFERENCES
 
Asarnow, J. R. (1988) Children at risk for schizophrenia: converging lines of evidence. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 14, 613-631.[Medline]

Byrne, M., Hodges, A., Grant, E., et al (1999) Neuropsychological assessment of young people at high genetic risk for developing schizophrenia compared with controls: preliminary findings of the Edinburgh High Risk Study (EHRS). Psychological Medicine, 29, 1161-1173.[CrossRef][Medline]

Cornblatt, B. & Obuchowski, M. (1997) Update of high-risk research: 1987-1997. International Review of Psychiatry, 9, 437-447.[CrossRef]

Cosway, R., Byrne, M., Clafferty, R., et al (2000) Neuropsychological change in young people at high risk for schizophrenia: Results from the first two neuropsychological assessments of the Edinburgh High Risk Study. Psychological Medicine, 30, 1111-1121.[CrossRef][Medline]

Done, D. J., Crow, T. J., Johnstone, E. C., et al (1994) Childhood antecedents of schizophrenia and affective illness: social adjustment at ages 7 and 11. BMJ, 309, 699-703.[Abstract/Free Full Text]

Her Majesty's Stationery Office (1991) Classification of Occupations of the Registrar General. London: HMSO.

Hodges, A., Byrne, M., Grant, E., et al (1999) People at risk of schizophrenia. Sample characteristics of the first 100 cases in the Edinburgh High-Risk Study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 547-553.[Abstract]

Johnstone, E. C., Abukmeil, S. S., Byrne, M., et al (2000) Edinburgh High Risk Study — findings after four years. Demographic, attainment and psychopathological issues. Schizophrenia Research, 46, 1-15.[CrossRef][Medline]

Jones, P., Rodgers, B., Murray, R., et al (1994) Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet, 344, 1398-1402.[Medline]

Lawrie, S. M. & Abukmeil, S. S. (1998) Brain abnormality in schizophrenia. A systematic and quantitative review of volumetric magnetic resonance imaging studies. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 110-120.[Abstract]

Lawrie, S. M., Ingle, G. T., Santosh, C. G., et al (1995) Magnetic resonance imaging and single photon emission tomography in treatment-responsive and treatment-resistant schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 202-210.[Abstract]

Lawrie, S. M., Whalley, H., Kestelman, J. N., et al (1999) Magnetic resonance imaging of brain in people at high risk of developing schizophrenia. Lancet, 353, 30-33.[CrossRef][Medline]

Lawrie, S. M., Whalley, H., Abukmeil, S. S., et al (2001a) Brain structure, genetic liability and psychotic symptoms in subjects at high risk of developing schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 811-823.[Medline]

Lawrie, S. M., Byrne, M., Miller, P., et al (2001b) Neurodevelopmental indices and the development of psychotic symptoms in subjects at high risk of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 524-530.[Abstract/Free Full Text]

Miller, P. M., Lawrie, S. M., Hodges, A., et al (2001) Genetic liability, illicit drug use, life stress and psychotic symptoms: preliminary findings from the Edinburgh study of people at high risk for schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 36, 338-342.[CrossRef][Medline]

Miller, P. M., Byrne, M., Hodges, A., et al (2002) Childhood behaviour, psychotic symptoms and psychosis onset in young people at high risk of schizophrenia: early findings from the Edinburgh High-Risk Study. Psychological Medicine 32, 173-179.[Medline]

Nelson, H. E. (1982) The National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual. Windsor: NFER—Nelson.

Walker, E. F., Savoie, T. & Davis, D. (1994) Neuromotor precursors of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20, 441-451.[Medline]

Wing, J. K., Cooper, J. E. & Sartorius, N. (1974) The Description and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms. An Instruction Manual for the PSE and Catego Systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

World Health Organization (1993) ICD—10: The ICD—10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO.