Northern Birmingham Mental Health NHS Trust and University of Birmingham, Birmingham
Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, London
Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Exeter
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK
Correspondence: Max Birchwood, Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Birmingham, Harry Watton House, 97 Church Lane, Aston, Birmingham B6 5UG, UK
Declaration of interest This research was supported by the Mental Health Foundation and Department of Health.
See pp. 522-528, this
issue.
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Aims We chart the ontogeny of depression and psychotic symptoms from the acute psychotic episode over a 12-month period and test the validity of the proposed course patterns.
Method One hundred and five patients with ICD-10 schizophrenia were followed up on five occasions over 12 months following the acute episode, taking measures of depression, positive symptoms, negative symptoms, neuroleptic exposure and side-effects.
Results Depression accompanied acute psychosis in 70% of cases and remitted in line with the psychosis; 36% developed PPD without a concomitant increase in psychotic symptoms.
Conclusions The results provided support for the validity of two of the three course patterns of depression in schizophrenia, including PPD. Post-psychotic depression occurs de novo without concomitant change in positive or negative symptoms.
![]() |
INTRODUCTION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
![]() |
METHOD |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Case identification
The study commenced in 1994 and all patients were recruited from acute
services in northern Birmingham (population 650 000). Inclusion required that
patients fulfilled ICD-10 (World Health
Organization, 1992) criteria for schizophrenia or a related
disorder (F20, F22 and F23), in the absence of organic disorder or a current
diagnosis of mania or bipolar disorder or mood-congruent delusions and
hallucinations. All participants were aged between 18 and 65 years
inclusive.
Daily contact with admission units ensured that all new admissions were screened within 3 days of their arrival by examining medical files for exclusion criteria and current symptoms/diagnosis and through discussion with ward nursing and psychiatric staff. Patients satisfying screening criteria were interviewed within the next 2 days; thus, no more than 5 days elapsed between admission and first interview. The timing of the recovery interview corresponded with each patient's discharge from the ward and employed recovery criteria described by Drury et al (1996). Patients were re-interviewed until recovery had occurred.
Assessments
Each interview was conducted using a battery of measures. Negative and
positive symptoms were measured using the Scale for the Assessment of Negative
Symptoms (SANS; Andreason,
1982) and the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS;
Andreason, 1986) respectively.
Two measures were used to assess depressive pathology: the self-report Beck
Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck,
1967) and the interview-based Calgary Depression Scale for
Schizophrenia (CDSS; Addington et
al, 1993), which also provides sub-scales for hopelessness
and suicidal ideation. The CDSS was developed specifically with the aim of
avoiding phenomenological overlap between depression and negative symptoms.
The presence and severity of pharmacogenic side-effects were assessed using
the Abnormal Involuntary Movements Scale (AIMS;
Wojcik et al, 1980)
and the chlorpromazine equivalent dosages were calculated from the September
1996 British National Formulary
(British Medical Association & Royal
Pharmaceutical Society of Great Britain, 1996). Basic demographic
and clinical data were collected on a standardised form.
Patients' beliefs about their illness (Personal Beliefs about Illness Questionnaire, PBIQ; Birchwood et al, 1993), self-esteem (Crown Self-Esteem Scale; Crown et al, 1977) and insight (Insight Scale; Birchwood et al, 1994) were also assessed. Additional one-off measures were employed during the follow-up phase to evaluate cognitions pertaining to the self, namely the Possible Selves Questionnaire (PosSQ; Marcus & Nurius, 1986) and the Depressive Experiences Questionnaire (DEQ; Blatt et al, 1976). The data from the PBIQ, Crown Self-Esteem Scale, Insight Scale, PosSQ and DEQ will be commented upon in our subsequent companion paper.
Each item pertaining to a symptom on either the SANS and SAPS is assessed on a six-point scale. A score of zero denotes the absence of the symptom whereas a rating of 1-5 denotes the presence of the target symptom in increasing severity. The SANS has five categories: affective blunting and flattening, alogia, avolition-apathy, anhedonia-associality and attention. The SAPS consists of four categories: hallucinations, delusions, bizarre behaviour and positive formal thought disorder. In addition to the symptom items, which comprise each SANS or SAPS category, an additional global rating item for the category as a whole is included.
Reliability of SANS and SAPS ratings
Following a period of training in the instruments, mental state assessments
carried out by the second author (Z.I.) were subject to a reliability check to
prevent drift in accuracy of ratings across the study. A sample (n=6)
from the present study was selected randomly at different points and
re-interviewed by M.B. blind to the original assessment. Sufficient
concordance between the two raters was established for both SANS
(r=0.95, P<0.01) and SAPS (r=0.86,
P<0.05) total scores.
Hypotheses
![]() |
RESULTS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
|
Definition of post-psychotic depression
The ICD-10 definition of post-psychotic depression (PPD, F20.4) requires
that, along with general criteria for schizophrenia during the previous 12
months, the patient must still exhibit persistent hallucinations, thought
disorder or negative symptoms not due to depression or neuroleptic medication.
These criteria make many of the assumptions embodied in the theories outlined
in the introduction, whereas in this study we are making no such assumptions
and indeed we are attempting to discern empirical support for them. Hence, our
definition revolves around depressed mood per se.
Given that we are concerned with depressed mood and not necessarily formal
major depression, the definition of caseness is arbitrary; however, for
reasons outlined in the introduction, we used a definition of at least
moderate depression (BDI score 15). The correlation between the
self-report BDI and the interview-based CDSS approached unity
(r=0.91) and the thresholds proposed by Addington et al
(1993) conformed almost exactly
with the subgroups defined below using the BDI. The BDI criteria will be used
in this and our subsequent companion paper.
Using this definition, 70% of the sample were depressed at onset (n=55): of these, 48 (87%) dipped below the threshold for at least one post-recovery follow-up observation, 26 (54%) of whom became depressed once again and the remaining 22 (46%) stayed below threshold; 5 (9%) were depressed at all follow-up points; and 2 (4%) were depressed at all but the final follow-up point.
Twenty-three subjects were not depressed at onset: thirteen (56%) exceeded the threshold on at least one occasion (post-recovery), nine (39%) remained below threshold and one (4%) became depressed at the recovery observation but remained below threshold at all future follow-up points.
Course types
Using these criteria, all but eight patients were assigned to the following
groups:
Post-psychotic depression (PPD, n=39)
Group I: Depressed at onset, improvement and return of depression (n=26)
Group II: No depression at onset; depressed at one or more follow-up points (n=13)
No post-psychotic depression (non-PPD, n=31)
Group III: Depressed at onset; no depression throughout the follow-up period (n=22)
Group IV: No depression at onset or throughout the follow-up period (n=9)
Five patients did not recover from their acute illness throughout this follow-up period and a further three patients could not be assigned to the above groups. Of the three patients unable to be assigned, two were above threshold for all but the final follow-up point and the third was depressed at the recovery observation.
The potential problem of observer bias was controlled by allocation of subjects to one of the four groups once the collection of data had been completed.
For those patients with PPD, we defined the follow-up point immediately prior to its appearance as pre-PPD: in other words, the point in the follow-up period where the patient manifested no depression prior to the onset of PPD.
The individual subject variation in the timing of PPD, and therefore pre-PPD points, would make any attempt at analysing the data virtually impossible using the original temporal sequence. We therefore examined data at the pre-PPD and PPD stages by realigning individual subject data at the appropriate follow-up points. As the average number of days from the discharge interview to the pre-PPD observation was 167.5 (s.d.=124.9), the default pre-PPD and PPD observations for those subjects who did not develop PPD (n=31) were those at 4 and 8 months after recovery, respectively.
The PPD groups
Figure 1 shows the evolution
of PPD in each of the two PPD groups (groups I and II) juxtaposed with that
for positive symptoms (SAPS total score) and negative symptoms (SANS total
score).
|
The SAPS scores remained unchanged during the PPD episode in both group I (pre-PPD v. PPD: t=1.84, P=0.08) and group II (t=1.34, NS). The SANS scores were also stable in group I (t<1, NS) and group II (t<1, NS).
Although the BDI score in group I reverted to that observed in the acute episode (BDI score=25), the SAPS score rose only slightly (but non-significantly). In group II the BDI score rose to 18.2, which is an unprecedented rise in this particular group, without a concomitant increase in SAPS. It should be noted that the mean level of depression during PPD was moderate for the latter group and severe for the former, using conventional criteria (see Williams, 1992, for a review).
The non-PPD groups
Again, the evolution of depression in patients not manifesting PPD is
juxtaposed with that for SAPS (Fig.
2). In the case of group III, SAPS declines in line with
depression, whereas in group IV, by definition, the decline in SAPS does not
coincide with that for depression.
|
Overall then, in groups I and III depression and positive symptoms follow
the same course (combined n=48) during the acute episode, whereas in
groups II and IV (n=22) depression and positive symptoms are not
synchronous at the acute episode. However, on comparing patients with and
without depression at onset (BDI score 15), there was no significant
difference in SAPS score at onset, confirming that, in general terms, any link
between positive symptoms and depression is confined to a subgroup.
Comparison of PPD with non-PPD groups
Positive symptoms
In order to determine whether the presence of PPD was the result of
variation in positive symptoms, the groups were compared at each follow-up
point. For this analysis groups I and II (PPD) and (III and IV) (non-PPD) were
combined. Table 2 suggests a
small mean rise in SAPS in each of the PPD groups; combining these groups into
a single PPD group (n=39) revealed a modest significant rise in SAPS
from 2.3 (2.6) to 3.6 (3.2) (t=2.3, P<0.05) during PPD;
no overall change in SANS was apparent.
|
This result suggests the presence of a subgroup showing a rise in positive symptoms, beyond the chance level, ranging from a mild increase through to a full relapse. Thus we defined the threshold for a rise in positive symptoms equivalent to at least a mild increase in symptoms; our operational definition was a rise equivalent to at least the margin of measurement error in the SAPS scale. From the interrater reliability data, the maximum discrepancy in SAPS global score ratings by M.B. and Z.I. was 3, which is equivalent to a single standard deviation. By defining subgroups of the PPD subjects using this discrepancy as a cut-off, 28 out of 39 (72%) did not display an increase in positive symptoms (i.e. a change of <3 in the SAPS global score) whereas SAPS scores for 11 subjects (28%) increased at PPD by at least the maximum interrater discrepancy. The mean SAPS global score for these 11 subjects at PPD was not significantly different from their SAPS score at onset (t=2.03, NS), suggesting that the rise in depression and positive symptoms in this subgroup was concordant with symptoms indicative of a psychotic relapse, which indeed conformed with our clinical impression (e.g. some were hospitalised). We conclude, therefore, that the remaining 28 subjects (36% of the whole sample) developed PPD without any marked increase in positive symptoms and were hence not displaying signs of psychotic relapse. This now becomes the new pure strain PPD sample.
Negative symptoms
A similar investigation of the depressed and non-depressed subjects' extent
of negative symptoms during the pre-PPD and PPD stages was also conducted. No
overall change in SANS was apparent (Table
2). These results clearly indicate the stability of negative
symptoms during the pre-PPD and PPD stages as subjects become depressed,
irrespective of changes in positive symptoms.
Drugs and side-effects
Information on prescribed neuroleptics was obtained at each of the
follow-up points and converted to chlorpromazine equivalents (British
National Formulary, 1996). In accordance with pharmacogenic theories of
depression in schizophrenia, the two groups were compared at each follow-up
point to determine whether the presence of PPD was the result of neuroleptic
exposure. No differences were observed at any of the follow-up points
(F<1 for all).
Similarly, the two groups were compared for side-effects using the AIMS scale. At no point in the follow-up period did the PPD group differ in severity of side-effects using either the global or composite item measures.
Is PPD just an early sign of relapse?
It can be argued that the rise in depression during the PPD phase may be an
early sign of a further psychotic episode, because dysphoria is a known
precursor of psychotic relapse (Birchwood
et al, 1989), rather than PPD per se. One way of
addressing this issue is to establish the presence of psychotic symptoms
following PPD. A comparison of positive symptoms after the PPD stage
could be investigated only for those subjects who developed PPD at the third
or fourth observation points, where follow-up data would therefore be
available (i.e. at the fourth or fifth observation, respectively). Hence,
there would be no follow-up data for subjects developing PPD at the fifth (and
final) observation. Sixty-four per cent of subjects (n=18) who
developed PPD had follow-up data after the PPD stage. For these cases, the
mean SAPS global score was not higher at the subsequent observation than that
observed at PPD (2.52 (2.50) v. 2.76 (2.66); t=0.38, NS) and
thus does not support the argument that the rise in depression at PPD is a
precursor to the onset of relapse.
Who develops PPD?
Of the 28 patients developing PPD, 68% were also depressed at onset. A
total of 22/48 (46%) with depression at onset did not develop PPD and
conversely 9/28 (32%) with PPD were not depressed at onset. Overall, the
presence of depression at onset did not raise the risk of PPD
(2=0.63, NS). There were no differences between the groups
with (n=28) and without PPD (n=31) for: age at onset,
duration of illness or total number of admissions; gender, marital status or
ethnicity; or being under a section of the Mental Health Act.
Depression during acute psychosis
Definition
The wide variation in depression during the acute episode was not
anticipated and here we distinguish those individuals with and without
depression at onset using the same criteria (BDI score 15). This yields a
group of patients with acute psychosis who are not depressed: 39/105 (37%) for
the whole cohort and 22/78 (28%) for the sample completing all follow-up
points. The mean BDI score for the depressed group was 27.7 (8.5) and for the
non-depressed was 7.4 (4.4), indicating that the depressed group were, on
average, severely depressed. Contrary to expectation, there was
no difference in SAPS global score between these depression groups:
depressed=8.2 (3.7); not depressed=7.8 (3.1). Similarly, there was no
difference between these groups in SANS global score: depressed=11.8 (4.7);
not depressed=11.2 (4.9).
Who develops depression at onset?
People with depression at onset were younger at their first episode (30
v. 25 years; F=6.1, P<0.01) and younger at
inclusion to the study (40 v. 32; F=10.8,
P<0.001). There were no differences between the groups in
neuroleptic dose or drug side-effects.
Hopelessness and suicidal thinking
A total of 54% of non-relapsing depressed patients at the PPD stage also
reported suicidal thinking or worse on the CDSS, including 18% who had made
suicidal plans and 4% who had undertaken a suicide attempt designed to end in
death. Similarly, 36% of those with PPD expressed a concurrent persistent
sense of hopelessness about the future (P<0.01 for both).
One-year prevalence
In order to gauge the level of occurrence of these features using this
unique longitudinal perspective, we calculated the prevalence of at least
moderate severity levels of depression or hopelessness or
suicidal thinking across the three follow-up points over the 12-month
envelope. Excluding the acute psychotic and recovery points, the 12-month
prevalence for subjects with moderate levels of depression and
hopelessness amounted to one-third (33.3%) of the sample, whereas that for
subjects who were moderately depressed and suicidal was 20.5%.
Finally, 61% during the 12 months after discharge from hospital experienced at
least moderate levels of depression or suicidal ideation or
hopelessness at a follow-up observation.
![]() |
DISCUSSION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
On the basis of our definition of PPD, 36% (28/78) of patients in the study developed depressive symptoms of at least moderate intensity during the 12 months following an acute psychotic episode. It is important to point out that at the follow-up point immediately preceding the onset of PPD (i.e. pre-PPD) all these subjects were not depressed. The average time to PPD after recovery was 248 days or about 8 months, whereas the average number of days to PPD from the onset of psychosis was 304. Earlier studies have observed prevalence rates for PPD at 38% (Van Putten & May, 1978), 39% (Roy, 1981) and 45% (Leff et al, 1988). Our sample consists of a high proportion of chronic and multiple-relapse cases within a socio-economically depressed inner-city area, more so than in previous studies, although these similar rates of PPD suggest that chronicity or relapse history may not alone be predictive of PPD and its origin may lie elsewhere. This is underlined by recent observations of a high rate of depression in first-episode subjects (Addington et al, 1998), and in the present study 50% of first-episode subjects developed PPD compared with 32% for those with multiple relapses (t=3.1, P<0.01). Overall, then, the sample's bias towards youth and the differential rate of PPD in the first-episode group need to be kept in mind when considering the generalisability of the data.
Analyses reveal that, for the patients developing pure strain PPD, no significant changes are observed in positive symptoms, negative symptoms, the severity of side-effects or drug dose during the follow-up period.
The results clearly indicate the stability of negative and positive symptoms during the pre-PPD and PPD stages as subjects become depressed. To our surprise, correlational analyses between SANS global score and sub-scales and BDI for depressed subjects at the PPD phase indicate no significant associations between PPD and negative symptoms (all r<0.16, NS). We support Siris's (1995) position that the major difference between depression and negative symptoms is blue mood, a valuable indicator of depression that differentiates it from symptoms such as anhedonia, alogia, affective flattening and apathy, providing a clear rationale for distinguishing negative symptoms from depression. Finally, the risk of PPD was unaffected by the presence of depression during the acute psychotic stage. This suggests that the processes underlying PPD and depression at onset are different, and in line with the theories outlined in the introduction.
An unexpected finding was that about one-third of patients in acute psychosis were not depressed and the remaining two-thirds displayed mostly severe depression. Furthermore, there were no significant differences between the positive symptoms, drug dosage or severity of side-effects between these two groups at onset. Finally, there is substantial evidence that suicidal thinking and hopelessness accompany depressive pathology in the period following remission of acute psychosis. In the context of our 1-year prevalence rates for suicidal ideation, parasuicide and suicide of 23%, which are in line with previous results of 25% (Roy, 1986) and 10% (Mortensen & Juel, 1993), there is a considerable need to establish a theoretical framework and preventive therapy for this most serious of problems, which shows a clear linkage with PPD.
The course patterns
We find evidence to support two of the course patterns outlined by Leff
(1990). Depression can follow
the same course as positive symptoms (during acute psychosis and relapse) and
present de novo during follow-up without a change in positive
symptoms (i.e. PPD). We found no evidence that depression emerged immediately
following the remission of positive symptoms. Pharmacogenic theories are not
supported either during acute psychosis or in the following 12 months.
Similarly, negative symptoms remained stable whether PPD was observed or not.
In addition, the study attempted to employ stringent criteria at recruitment
and to define clearly the nature of PPD, and therefore we do not believe that
diagnostic variation provides an explanation for these results.
Methodological considerations
In order to control for interviewer bias the BDI (self-report) and CDSS
(observerrated), two highly correlated and virtually interchangeable
depression inventories (Addington et
al, 1993), were utilised. These measures were highly
intercorrelated (r=0.91). Interrater reliability for the SANS and
SAPS was also established at various junctures throughout the study, and shown
to be satisfactory.
We conclude that the experiment embodied little measurement error, and that insufficient to distort the main results.
What is PPD ?
The ICD-10 definition of PPD (F20.4) requires that, alongside a general
criterion for schizophrenia during the previous 12 months, the patient must
still exhibit persistent hallucinations in any modality, symptoms of thought
disorder or certain negative symptoms that are not due to depression or
neuroleptic medication. Based upon these criteria, none of our PPD subjects
would conform to ICD PPD but would be simply depressed. We suggest that there
is a strong case for further investigation of the diagnostic criteria for PPD.
The ICD-10 criteria seem to be rooted in the assumption that depression in
schizophrenia is an endemic feature of the psychotic process.
We have argued that although PPD is embedded in the realities of a psychotic illness, it is the beliefs or appraisals about psychosis made by those who experience it that are important (Birchwood & Iqbal, 1998; Rooke & Birchwood, 1998). In our subsequent companion paper this hypothesis is put to the test.
![]() |
Clinical Implications and Limitations |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
LIMITATIONS
![]() |
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
Addington, D., Addington, J. & Patten, S. (1998) Depression in people with first-episode schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172 (supp. 33), 90-92.[Abstract]
Andreason, N. C. (1982) Negative symptoms of schizophrenia: definition and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 784-788.[Abstract]
Andreason, N. C. (1986) The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City: University of Iowa.
Beck, A. T. (1967) Depression: Clinical, Experimental and Theoretical Aspects. New York: Harper and Row.
Birchwood, M. J. & Iqbal, Z. (1998) Depression and suicidal thinking in psychosis: a cognitive approach. In Outcomes and Innovation in Psychological Management of Schizophrenia (T. Wykes, N. Tarrier & S. Lewis), pp. 81-100. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Birchwood, M. J., Smith, J., MacMillan, J. F., et al (1989) Predicting relapse in schizophrenia: the development of an early signs monitoring system using patients and families as observers. Psychological Medicine, 19, 649-656.[Medline]
Birchwood, M. J., Mason, R., MacMillan, F., et al (1993) Depression, demoralisation and control over psychotic illness: a comparison of depressed and non-depressed patients with a chronic psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 23, 387-395.[Medline]
Birchwood, M. J., Smith, J., Drury, V., et al (1994) A self-report insight scale for psychosis: reliability, validity and sensitivity to change. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 89, 62-67.[Medline]
Birchwood, M. J., Spencer, E. & McGovern, D.
(2000a) Schizophrenia: early warning signs.
Advances in Psychiatric Treatment,
6, 93-101.
Birchwood, M. J., Iqbal, Z., Chadwick, P., et al
(2000b) Cognitive approach to depression and
suicidal thinking in psychosis. I. Ontogeny of post-psychotic depression.
British Journal of Psychiatry,
177,
516-521.
Blatt, S. J., D'Afflitti, P. & Quinlan, D. M. (1976) Experience of depression in normal young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 383-389.[CrossRef][Medline]
Bleuler, E. (1950) Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias, 1911 (English translation: J. Zinkin). New York: International Universities Press.
British Medical Association & Royal Pharmaceutical Society of Great Britain (1996) British National Formulary. London: BMA & Pharmaceutical Press.
Brown, G. W., Bifulco, A. & Andrews, B. (1990) Selfesteem and depression. IV. Effect on course and recovery. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 25, 244-249.[Medline]
Chadwick, P. D., Birchwood, M. & Trower, P. (1996) Cognitive Therapy for Delusions, Voices and Paranoia. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Crown, S., Lucas, C. J., Stringer, P., et al (1977) Personality correlates of study difficulty and academic performance in university students. II: Conscience and selfesteem. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 50, 275-281.[Medline]
Davidson, I. & Strauss, J. S. (1992) Sense of self in recovery from severe mental illness. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 65, 131-145.[Medline]
Drake, R. E., Gates, C. & Cotton, T. (1986) Suicide among schizophrenics: a comparison of attempters and completed suicides. British Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 784-787.[Abstract]
Drury, V., Birchwood, M., Cochrane, R., et al (1996) Cognitive therapy and recovery from acute psychosis: a controlled trial. I. Impact on psychotic symptoms. British Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 593-601.[Abstract]
Gilbert, P. (1992) Depression: The Evolution of Powerlessness. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gilbert, P. (1995) Power, social rank and depression: comments on Price and Gardner. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 68, 211-215.
Hirsch, S. R. & Jolley, A. G. (1989) The dysphoric syndrome in schizophrenia and its implications for relapse. British Journal of Psychiatry, 155 (suppl. 5), 46-50.
Lazarus, R. & Folkman, J. (1984) Stress, Appraisal and Coping. New York: Springer.
Leff, J. (1990) Depressive symptoms in the course of schizophrenia. In Depression in Schizophrenia (ed. L. E. DeLisi), pp. 3-23. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Leff, J., Tress, A. & Edwards, B. (1988) The clinical course of depressive symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 1, 25-30.[CrossRef][Medline]
Marcus, H. & Nurius, P. (1986) Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954-969.[CrossRef]
Marcus, H. & Wurf, E. (1987) The dynamic self-concept: a social psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 299-337.[CrossRef]
Mortensen, P. B. & Juel, K. (1993) Mortality and causes of death in first admitted schizophrenic patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 183-189.[Abstract]
Ogilvie, D. M. (1987) The undesired self: a neglected variable in personality research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 379-385.[CrossRef]
Rooke, O. & Birchwood, M. (1998) Loss, humiliation and entrapment as appraisals of schizophrenic illness: a prospective study of depressed and non-depressed patients. British Journal of Psychology, 37, 259-268.
Rosenberg, M. (1965) Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Roy, A. (1981) Depression in the course of chronic undifferentiated schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 296-297.[Abstract]
Roy, A. (1986) Suicide in schizophrenia. In Suicide (ed. A. Roy), pp 97-112. Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins.
Siris, S. G. (1995) Depression and schizophrenia. In Schizophrenia (eds S. R. Hirsch & D. R. Weinberger), pp. 128-145. Oxford: Blackwell.
Van Putten, T. & May, P. (1978) Akinetic depression in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 1101-1107.[Abstract]
Williams, J. M. G. (1992) The Psychological Treatment of Depression. London: Routledge.
Wojcik, J. D., Gelenberg, A. J., LaBrie, R. A., et al (1980) Prevalence of tardive dyskinesia in an outpatient population. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 21, 370-380.[Medline]
World Health Organization (1992) The Tenth Revision of the International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD-10). Geneva: WHO.
Zuroff, D. C., Moskowitz, D. S. & Cote, S. (1999) Dependency, self-criticism, interpersonal behaviour and affect: evolutionary perspectives. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 231-250.[Medline]
Received for publication December 15, 1999. Revision received June 8, 2000. Accepted for publication June 9, 2000.