Department of Psychiatry, University of California and the Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System, San Diego, CA, USA
(Received 18 February 2003; accepted 18 June 2003)
* Author to who correspondence should be addressed at: Department of Psychiatry (116A), University of California, 3350 La Jolla Village Drive, San Diego, CA 921612002, USA. Tel.: 858 552 8585 ext. 7978; Fax: 858 552 7424. E-mail: mschuckit{at}ucsd.edu
![]() |
ABSTRACT |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
![]() |
INTRODUCTION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
A third model focuses on deviance proneness, and posits that a family history (FH) of disinhibited behaviours (including substance use disorders) relates to cognitive difficulties in the offspring which might produce symptoms of sadness and of disinhibited, acting out, or externalizing behaviours (Jessor and Jessor, 1977; Hesselbrock et al., 1991
; Sher, 1991
, 1997
; Pihl and Bruce, 1995
; Zucker et al., 1996
; Finn et al., 1997
; Bauer and Hesselbrock, 1999
; Giancola and Parker, 2001
; Tarter, 2002
). Eventually, these externalizing symptoms and associated phenomena may contribute to the selection of deviant or substance-using peers, and to the early onset of substance use and an enhanced risk for alcohol and drug problems. High levels of externalizing behaviours have been noted in adolescents who use substances, might characterize children of alcoholics, and have been reported to predict future substance-related problems (Sher, 1991
; Cadoret et al., 1995
; Crowley et al., 1998
; Puttler et al., 1998
; Jacob and Windle, 2000
; Zucker et al., 2000
; Tarter, 2002
). While much of the relationship of externalizing symptoms to substance use might be explained by extreme conditions such as conduct disorder or antisocial personality disorder (ASPD), some aspects of disinhibited or impulsive behaviour may contribute to substance use even after the impact of these conditions is controlled (Brown et al., 1996
; Biederman et al., 1998
; Giancola and Parker, 2001
).
The ideal approach to evaluate models of risk would involve a longitudinal study where data were obtained regarding biological background and multiple domains of functioning (Caspi et al., 1996; Newcomb, 1997
; Chassin et al., 1999
; Ferdinand et al., 2001
). This technique may be especially useful if observations begin early in life and domains of interest are measured repeatedly, without over-reliance on long-term retrospective data. Prospective research offers the opportunity to evaluate intermediate phenotypes as they develop and subsequently contribute to the problematic use of substances such as alcohol.
Just as there is no single, perfect model or set of domains for study in the development of AUD (Carpenter and Hasin, 1999; Labouvie and Bates, 2002
; Tarter, 2002
), there are also several different, but complementary, ways to select and evaluate populations appropriate for such longitudinal research. Some investigations focus on children already demonstrating extreme forms of major intermediate phenotypes relevant to one or more of the models, including young subjects with relatively severe depressions or anxiety disorders, and those already demonstrating intense externalizing symptoms (Merikangas et al., 1996
; Tarter, 2002
). The latter has involved identification of young persons who already meet criteria for conduct disorder, or who are selected because of involvement with the juvenile justice system (Bauer and Hesselbrock, 1999
; Tarter, 2002
). Such subjects have many assets for enhancing understanding of the approximately 20% of alcohol-dependent individuals who have ASPD, and for analyzing the impact of such extreme externalizing conditions on the risk for early-onset AUD associated with additional substance dependencies and criminality (e.g. Types 2 and B alcoholism) (Cloninger, 1987
; Hesselbrock et al., 1991
; Babor et al., 1992
; Slutske et al., 1998
; Bauer and Hesselbrock, 1999
; Schuckit, 2000
). However, the focus on extreme cases, which are likely to represent a minority of alcoholics, might produce a situation where the high proportion of the variance explained by the externalizing symptoms in these subjects could diminish the magnitude of effect of additional influences that contribute to the risk for AUD in the less extreme case.
In the mid 1970s, our research group adopted a strategy of identifying subjects at high risk for future AUD who were felt to represent the more usual, relatively functional alcoholic (Schuckit, 1998; Schuckit and Smith, 2000
; Schuckit et al., 2002
). We excluded individuals in whom extreme levels of disinhibition were likely to operate by selecting subjects who, by the average age of 20 years, had not developed ASPD, and limited the potential extreme effects of two major psychiatric conditions known to be associated with subsequent substance use disorders (SUD) by excluding subjects with manic depressive disease or schizophrenia in themselves or their parents (Schuckit, 2002
). Our face-to-face follow-ups of over 97% of these 453 men, 10 and 15 years after their initial evaluation, revealed the expected high rate of AUD (Schuckit and Smith, 1996
, 2000
). Approximately 45% of the FH positive subjects developed alcohol abuse or dependence, compared with 20% of the FH negatives, figures that included 32 vs 14% with alcohol dependence.
While not studied until late adolescence or early adulthood, the data on these 453 men generated information about several additional domains of functioning applicable to the three models described above (Schuckit and Smith, 2000). Most directly germane to the current discussion is the fact that a measure of externalizing symptoms, behavioural undercontrol (Sher, 1991
), was related to an alcoholic outcome, although did not link to the FH of alcoholism in these families. Behavioural undercontrol operated as a predictor of AUD in a path model, with the influence of this variable appearing to flow primarily through expectations of the effects of alcohol, ways of coping with stress, and association with heavy-drinking peers (Schuckit and Smith, 2000
).
Recently, this prospective investigation has turned to an emphasis on gathering information on the offspring of our original subjects. For these children, data have been generated from the mother regarding pregnancy and birth complications (PBC), a phenomenon reported to relate to cognitive problems, externalizing behaviours, and internalizing symptoms in children (Pihl and Bruce, 1995; van Os et al., 1997
; Tapert et al., 1999
; Hill et al., 2000
; Giancola and Parker, 2001
; Tarter, 2002
). The mother also supplied information on a host of additional activities and symptoms among these offspring beginning at age 4 years through the Child Behavioral Checklist (CBCL), along with a standardized evaluation of life functioning and psychiatric symptoms through the parent report version regarding children for the Semi Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (C-SSAGA-P) at age seven (Achenbach, 1991
; Kuperman et al., 2001
). This prospective process offers the opportunity to evaluate domains of life functioning repeatedly as these children mature, including externalizing and internalizing symptoms which are directly relevant to several of the models of substance-related problems. Data from these analyses can then be interpreted in the light of intensely studied families without the liabilities encountered in long-term retrospective analyses spanning a decade or more.
Information on 444 children had been gathered during the recently completed 15-year evaluation, with 503 offspring identified in the ongoing 20-year stage. As recently reported (Barnow et al., 2002; Preuss et al., 2002
), 151 of these offspring were between ages 7 and 18, with complete data available on 146. These were analyzed to determine the relationship between the FH of AUD and internalizing as well as externalizing symptoms. Here, externalizing problems were not related to the presence or absence of a FH of AUD overall, but were higher in children with high family densities of alcoholism. Internalizing symptoms had a relatively weak relationship to a FH of AUD, but were robustly tied to the FH of non-substance-related (i.e. independent) mood and anxiety disorders in those families.
Thus, the studies to date on these offspring have evaluated how a FH of AUD relates to a single area of functioning. The current analyses explore the interrelationships among several characteristics of the children from these intensely studied families, and their link to an intermediate phenotype likely to be related to the future risk for AUD, externalizing symptoms. We have focused on aspects of the deviance-prone model, limiting the number of domains to those appropriate to 146 subjects by selecting those characteristics we projected might be most relevant to this blue-collar and white-collar set of families.
![]() |
SUBJECTS AND METHODS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
The original subjects were followed-up with face-to-face structured interviews approximately 10 years after initial testing (T10), at which time all 453 had been located, and all but three (99.3%) agreed to an evaluation with themselves and an additional informant, usually the spouse (Schuckit, 1998). These interviews focused on changes in demography, FH, and SUD or psychiatric conditions for the subject during the interval since initial testing.
Five years later (i.e. 15 years after the initial evaluation or at T15), subjects were located once again, and complete information was obtained on the 440 men among the 449 who were still living (Schuckit and Smith, 2000). At T15, for all subjects who had married, the spouse was interviewed regarding her demography and personal as well as FH of SUD and psychiatric conditions using an interview similar to that originally given to the subjects and to the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA) instrument (Bucholz et al., 1994
; Hesselbrock et al., 1999
). Spouses were queried regarding pregnancies, and information gathered on gestation, parturition, and the neonatal period for each child. Mothers of the 151 offspring who were ages 7 through 17 years (out of the total of all ages of 444 children) were asked to fill out the CBCL (Achenbach, 1991
). This instrument consists of 20 questions about the child's activities, participation in organizations, completion of chores, interactions with siblings and peers, as well as school performance, along with 120 items on behavioural and emotional problems, with the latter scored on a three-point scale from not true to very true. The CBCL can be used to generate standardized scores, including those that summarize acting out or problematic behaviours (externalizing symptoms), as well as a score for depressive and anxiety-type symptoms (internalizing symptoms) (Puttler et al., 1998
; Barnow et al., 2002
; Preuss et al., 2002
). Major scores on the CBCL have been reported to have a three-month retest reliability as high as 0.84, agreement among parents of 0.62, and an adequate external validity when compared to clinical evaluations (Bird et al., 1991
; Puttler et al., 1998
; Johnson et al., 2000
).
Parents of children age seven and above were also given to a face-to-face SSAGA interview about the children (Kuperman et al., 2001; Barnow et al., 2002
; Preuss et al., 2002
), selecting that minimum age as most appropriate for both gathering information from the major interview and questionnaire used, and slightly before the youngest age at which children might be expected to be exposed to alcohol (Reich, 1988
; Reich et al., 1993
; Johnston et al., 2001
). The basic instrument, with good to excellent validity when compared to another standardized interview (Hesselbrock et al., 1999
), reviews 17 Axis I psychiatric conditions and the ASPD, and has been reported to have good to excellent across interviewer and retest reliabilities, with kappas in excess of 0.60 for most diagnoses, including alcohol dependence (Bucholz et al., 1994
). The parent version reporting on children (C-SSAGA-P) was developed as an alternative form to facilitate evaluation of demography and psychiatric symptoms and syndromes in children through age 17 (Kuperman et al., 2001
).
Data from these instruments and from interviews of the original subjects and spouses were used to generate a series of scores in domains. The ideal evaluation would require personal interviews and testing of all children, a step which was considered too intrusive for these parents to accept for young offspring. Thus, the construction of relevant domains was guided by a parent's perception of the child's functioning in areas that often parallel domains currently being studied in the original subjects. Additional areas relate to domains hypothesized in our model to be relevant for young children in families of alcoholics and as predictors of future alcohol involvement and associated problems.
For these analyses, the lifetime FH of DSM-III-R alcohol abuse or dependence in biological grandparents (GRPARalc) was noted as the number of the four such individuals with an AUD, while a similar approach was used regarding a score of zero to two for the biological parents (PARalc) (American Psychiatric Association, 1987). The lifetime FH of affective and anxiety disorders reflected a similar score for grandparents (GRPARaff) and parents (PARaff) for DSM-III-R independent (i.e. not including substance-induced) major mood syndromes (major depressive or bipolar disorders) or major anxiety disorders (panic, generalized anxiety, or phobic conditions; American Psychiatric Association, 1987
; Schuckit et al., 1997
). As explained in more detail elsewhere (American Psychiatric Association, 1994
; Schuckit et al., 1997
), an alcohol-induced disorder is diagnosed if the syndrome only occurred in the context of a relevant substance dependence, while an independent condition either predated the dependence or occurred during a several month or more subsequent period of abstinence. Evidence of PBC was determined from the z-score transformation of three items gathered from the interview of the mother including a gestational period less than nine months, having to be placed in an incubator after birth, and staying in the hospital longer than the mother at birth (
= 0.78). Symptoms of externalization (EXT) were measured by the sum of z-score transformed values from the CBCL T-score (i.e. sex- and age-corrected) externalizing, as well as a count of the number of externalizing symptoms endorsed from among the 42 possible appropriate problematic behaviours queried on the C-SSAGA-P (
= 0.82) (Barnow et al., 2002
). The internalizing score (INT) (
= 0.73) was a combination of the CBCL internalizing T-score and a symptom count of the 39 questions relevant to anxiety and mood symptoms in the C-SSAGA-P (Preuss et al., 2002
). The stability of the home environment (HOME) during the 5 years leading up to the interview was measured on a 3-point scale relating to the continuous presence in the home of the biological father and/or mother. For all domains, higher scores indicated a more problematical condition.
Based on the literature cited above, these domains were hypothesized to relate together in the manner described in the Introduction and presented as a modified deviance-prone model in Fig. 1. Briefly, both FH measures of AUD were predicted to relate to EXT, as well as to each other. AUD in the parents were predicted to relate to PBC, as well as to the measure of HOME, while both PBC and HOME were felt to be likely to relate to EXT. Because internalizing symptoms (INT) play a role in the hypothesized deviance prone model, the FH of affective and anxiety disorders were included, with projections that such disorders in parents and grandparents would relate to EXT, these two FH measures would relate to each other and to internalizing symptoms, while parental problems in this domain would relate to PBC. INT and EXT were both considered potential concurrent end points at this stage of the model, and, therefore, were hypothesized to have a non-recursive relationship.
|
![]() |
RESULTS |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
A major goal in the current paper with these young subjects is to describe how potential predictors of future substance use and problems related to the intermediate phenotype of externalizing symptoms as well as to each other at this early age. Regarding these domains, reflecting the selection criteria for the original families, 80.7% of the offspring had at least one parent or grandparent with alcohol misuse or dependence, including 51.0% with one such relative, 22.1% with two, 4.8% with three, and 2.8% with four. One in three offspring (33.1%) had at least one relative with an independent mood or anxiety disorder, including 25.5% who had one relevant parent or grandparent, 3.4% with two, and the remainder with three such relatives. For HOME, 11.0% of the offspring had at least one parent not living with them. The mean z-scores on the additional domains were 0.003 (± 0.83) for PBC, 0.009 (± 1.62) for EXT, 0.006 (± 1.55) for INT.
Table 1 presents the zero-order correlations among the seven variables. All but one domain significantly related to externalizing symptoms, with the exception being the parental FH of AUD, where the FH relationship was carried solely by AUD in the extended family. The two alcohol FH variables correlated positively with each other, and each related to the absence of one or more of the biological parents in the home. FHaff measures were related, each correlated with all domains except for the two FH of AUD and HOME, and demonstrated their highest correlations with INT, with the latter variable correlating with PBC.
|
|
The generalizability of the empirical model was then examined by evaluating sex and age. First, regarding sex, at the zero-order level this variable was not related significantly to any of the eight domains. Not unexpectedly, therefore, when sex was added to the model, the R2 only increased to 0.27, and the major relationships reported in Fig. 2 were unchanged. Additionally, when all paths except for that from HOME to INT were constrained to be equivalent across sex, this two-group model fits well (2 = 51.10; d.f. = 44; P = 0.22; with a
2 to d.f. ratio of 1.16; GFI = 0.92; NNFI 0.93; CFI = 0.95; and RMSEA = 0.034), suggesting that this model fits the data for boys and girls equally. Regarding the path from HOME to INT, the beta weight was 0.36 (P < 0.001) for boys while it was 0.05 (= 0.62) for the girls. Second, regarding age, while this variable correlated with HOME (0.39; P < 0.001), INT (0.21; P < 0.02), and EXT (0.20; P < 0.02) on the zero-order level, when age was added to the model in Fig. 2, the relationships of age to INT and EXT were no longer significant, and the overall R2 remained at 0.26. Also, selecting the usual age break used for the CBCL, when all paths were constrained to be equivalent across two age groups (11 or younger versus 12 or older), this two-group model fits well (
2 = 48.35; d.f. = 46; P = 0.34, with a
2 to d.f. ratio of 1.07; GFI = 0.92; NNFI = 0.96; CFI = 0.97; and RMSEA = 0.23), suggesting this model fits the data from the younger and older subjects equally.
It is important to note that, in light of the young age of the sample, information regarding most domains was obtained only from the parents. An effort was made to estimate the probable accuracy of the parents' reports through an evaluation of data from subjects where information had already been gathered from the next stage, T20 evaluations. Here, 63 children age 13 and above have been subsequently interviewed to date with the adolescent version of the SSAGA (C-SSAGA-A), while the parents reported on similar items through the C-SSAGA-P. In that subset, the correlation between the number of conduct-related symptoms reported by parent and child was 0.90 (P < 0.001), and, reflecting few subjects with more than one mood or anxiety problem noted, the correlation for the presence or absence of such difficulties was 0.81 (P < 0.001). In each instance when a discrepancy was seen, the child was more likely to report an item than the parent.
![]() |
DISCUSSION |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
The hypothesized model worked well with excellent goodness of fit indicators for the hypothesized and empirical pathways, and the latter explaining 26% of the variance. The empirical model appeared to work similarly in both sexes and across ages, although with sex, the path from HOME to INT appeared to be important only for boys. The latter might reflect a special vulnerability for boys when a father is absent from the home, as fathers were more often the absent parent. The impacts of age and sex were also considered by using adjusted T-scores from the CBCL for internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Future follow-ups of this population will determine whether the model is stable over time, and if the current domains predict future substance-related problems.
The dependent variable, externalizing symptoms, proved to be important to this model despite the exclusion of families where extreme externalizing conditions such as ASPD were prominent. These results support the usefulness of externalizing-type behaviours even in the more usual family from which alcoholics come, as only 20% or so of alcohol-dependent men carry an ASPD label (Bauer and Hesselbrock, 1999; Schuckit, 2000
). At the same time, the complex nature of EXT is supported by the links to FH of both AUD and mood/anxiety disorders, as well as to internalizing symptoms and to the number of biological parents in the home.
The report of the child's mood and anxiety symptoms (INT) was included here because it is often incorporated in evaluations of deviance-prone models. Our prior analyses of this domain demonstrated the expected close relationship between INT and a FH of similar disorders, and thus, the FH domain was also incorporated. Both INT and the FH of affect and anxiety-related variables worked well within the model, with INT in a non-recursive relationship with EXT. This might reflect a tendency of relatively young children with acting out behaviours to develop sadness and anxiety symptoms as they face subsequent problems, and/or the probability that internalizing disturbances can at times present as behavioural difficulties. As reported in prior evaluations (Schuckit et al., 1997; Schuckit and Smith, 2000
; Preuss et al., 2002
), neither FH of alcoholism measure related significantly to INT on either a zero-order level or within the model.
The association between an AUD in a grandparent (but not in a parent) and the externalizing symptoms in these offspring is worthy of note. Regarding the parents, our original subjects were only included if they were relatively highly functional. Thus, the grandparents, having no such restrictions, might have a wider range of externalizing phenomena, and might be better predictors of a predisposition toward such behaviours than the parents themselves. Perhaps similar factors operate regarding the FH of mood and anxiety disorders. The current results are consistent with our prior work with the fathers of these children where another measure of externalizing-type behaviours, behavioural undercontrol, related to the future risk for AUD, but was not directly tied to the FH of alcoholism (Schuckit and Smith, 1996). Similarly, the current findings are consistent with our prior analysis where no significant tie was found for externalizing symptoms and the presence or absence of a FH of AUD in a subset of these children (Schuckit et al., 2000
). The relationship of externalizing symptoms to an FH of alcoholism in parents may be more prominent in other samples, especially those where the original subjects include persons with ASPD.
A second unexpected finding was the limited role for a history of PBC. The one correlation to the FH of mood and anxiety disorders in parents might reflect difficulties during pregnancy or the early neonatal period when mothers were depressed, and such conditions are more prevalent among women than are AUD (Regier et al., 1988; Kessler et al., 1996
). Perhaps PBC might operate more robustly in the model among less highly functional families or those where parents are not as highly educated as our current group. The results might also reflect the limited scope of the PBC measure used here, and direct evaluations of cognitive performance in the children will be incorporated in the future as offspring over age 12 are directly tested. The current findings, however, may demonstrate how the impact of any single domain in any model or research paradigm is likely to depend on the context (e.g. other domains) within which the variable is measured.
While there are a number of relatively unique aspects to this study, the results must be evaluated in light of the specific methods used. First, all evaluations were carried out cross-sectionally in a modest sized sample, with the latter dictating that a conservative eight domains could be used in the SEM. Second, although the young age of the subjects facilitates evaluation of phenomena at an appropriate age while avoiding the problems of long-term recall, at this stage it is inappropriate to directly measure our eventual dependent variable, alcohol use and related problems. Thus, the emphasis here is on what is felt to be an important intermediate phenotype related to the AUD risk. Third, our results evaluate part of a single model, and future analyses will turn to additional models and a wider range of components of the deviance-prone hypothesis as the number of offspring in an appropriate age range grows. Fourth, reflecting the young age of this sample, all data were obtained from a parent. Future evaluations of offspring over age 12 will incorporate additional domains relevant to the deviance-prone, socialization, and affect-related models including the direct evaluation of cognitive functioning, the child's perceptions of deviance and substance use in peers, alcohol expectancies, ways of coping with stress, along with an item felt to be important to a socialization model, the level of response to alcohol. Fifth, some of the domains (e.g. HOME) were evaluated through use of a single variable, and, because the modest size of the sample, when multiple items were used, the manifest score was incorporated in the evaluation of the model. Latent variables will be used in the next stage of the work when we estimate 350 subjects will be age 12 or older. Also, a shorthand was used to label each of the domains, with the result that the reader is encouraged to carefully consider the actual sources of information while interpreting the data. Finally, while the population used here is likely to represent a substantial proportion of people in the USA (i.e. more functional blue- and white-collar Caucasian and Hispanic families), the original decision to exclude individuals with several severe psychiatric conditions and to emphasize more highly functional families is also likely to impact on the results.
![]() |
Acknowledgements |
---|
![]() |
REFERENCES |
---|
![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() ![]() |
---|
American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III-R), 3rd edn, revised. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Fourth Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV). American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
Arbuckle, J. L. and Wothke, W. (1999) Amos 4.0 User's Guide. Small Waters Corp., Chicago, IL.
Babor, T. F., Hofmann, M., DelBoca, F. K., Hesselbrock, V., Meyer, R. E., Dolinsky, Z. S. and Rounsaville. B. (1992) Types of Alcoholics, I: Evidence for an empirically derived typology based on indicators of vulnerability and severity. Archives of General Psychiatry 49, 599608.[Abstract]
Barnow, S., Schuckit, M. A., Smith, T. L., Preuss, U. and Danko, G. (2002) The relationship between the family density of alcoholism and externalizing symptoms among 146 children. Alcohol and Alcoholism 37, 383387.
Bauer, L. and Hesselbrock, V. (1999) P300 decrements in teenagers with conduct problems: implications for substance abuse risk and brain development. Biological Psychiatry 46, 263272.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Bentler, P. M. (1990) Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin 107, 238246.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Bentler, P. M. and Chou, C. (1987) Practical issues in structural modeling. Sociological Methods and Research 16, 78117.
Bentler, P. M. and Chou, C. (1995) Estimates and tests in structural equation modeling. In Structural Equation Modeling: Concepts, Issues, and Applications, Hoyle R. H. ed., pp. 3755. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.
Biederman, J., Wilens, T. E., Mick, E., Faraone, S. V. and Spencer, T. (1998) Does attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder impact the developmental course of drug and alcohol abuse and dependence? Biological Psychiatry 44, 269273.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Bird, H., Gould, M., Rubio-Stipee, M., Staghezza, B. and Canino, G. (1991) Screening for childhood psychopathology in the community using the Child Behavior Checklist. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 30, 116123.[ISI][Medline]
Boomsma, A. (1987) The robustness of maximum likelihood estimation in structural equation models. In Structural Modeling by Example: Applications in Educational, Sociological and Behavioral Research, Cuttance, P. and Ecob, R. eds, pp. 160198. Cambridge University Press.
Brown, S. A., Gleghorn, A. A., Schuckit, M. A., Myers, M. G. and Mott, A. (1996) Conduct disorder among adolescent alcohol and drug abusers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 57, 314324.[ISI][Medline]
Browne, M. W. and Cudeck, R. (1993) Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In Testing Structural Equation Models, Bollen, K. A. and Long, J. S. eds, pp. 136162. Sage, Newbury Park, CA.
Bucholz, K. K., Cadoret, R., Cloninger, C. R., Dinwiddie, S. H., Hesselbrock, V. M., Nurnberger, J. I., Jr, Reich, T., Schmidt, I. and Schuckit, M. A. (1994) A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: a report on the reliability of the SSAGA. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 55, 149158.[ISI][Medline]
Cadoret, R. J., Yates, W. R., Troughton, E., Woodworth, G. and Stewart, M. A. (1995) Adoption study demonstrating two genetic pathways to drug abuse. Archives of General Psychiatry 52, 4252.[Abstract]
Carmines, E. G. and McIver, J. P. (1981) Analyzing models with unobserved variables. In Social Measurement: Current Issues, Bohrnstedt, G. W. and Borgatta, E. F. eds, Sage, Beverly Hills, CA.
Carpenter, K. M. and Hasin, D. (1998) A prospective evaluation of the relationships between reasons for drinking and DSM-IV alcohol-use disorders. Addictive Behavior 23, 4146.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Carpenter, K. M. and Hasin, D. S. (1999) Drinking to cope with negative affect and DSM-IV AUD: A test of three alternative explanations. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 60, 694704.[ISI][Medline]
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T., Newman, D. and Silva, P. (1996) Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 53, 10331039.[Abstract]
Chassin, L., Curran, P. J., Hussong, A. M. and Colder, C. R. (1996) The relation of parent alcoholism to adolescent substance use: A longitudinal follow-up study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 105, 7080.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Chassin, L., Pitts, S. C., DeLucia, C. and Todd, M. (1999) A longitudinal study of children of alcoholics: predicting young adult substance use disorders, anxiety, and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 108, 106119.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Cloninger, C. R. (1987) Neurogenetic adaptive mechanisms in alcoholism. Science 236, 410416.[ISI][Medline]
Cohen, J. (1988) Statistical Power Analyses for the Behavioral Sciences. Lawrence Erbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.
Crowley, T. J., MacDonald, M. J., Whitmore, E. A. and Mikulich, S. K. (1998) Cannabis dependence, withdrawal, and reinforcing effects among adolescents with conduct symptoms and substance use disorders. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 50, 2737.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Ding, L., Velicer, W. F. and Harlow, L. L. (1995) Effects of estimation methods, number of indicators per factor, and improper solutions on structural equation modeling fit indices. Structural Equation Modeling 2, 119143.
Ferdinand, R. F., Blum, M. and Verhulst, F. C. (2001) Psychopathology in adolescence predicts substance use in young adulthood. Addiction 96, 861870.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Finn, P., Sharkansky, E., Viken, R., West, R., Sandy, J. and Befferd, G. (1997) Heterogeneity in the families of sons of alcoholics: the impact of familial vulnerability type on offspring characteristics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 106, 2636.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Giancola, P. R. and Parker, A. M. (2001) A six-year prospective study of pathways toward drug use in adolescent boys with and without a family history of a substance use disorder. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 62, 166178.[ISI][Medline]
Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. F. and Miller, J. Y. (1992) Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early adulthood: implications for substance abuse prevention. Psychological Bulletin 112, 64105.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Heath, A. C., Madden, P. A., Bucholz, K. K., Dinwiddie, S. H., Slutske, W. S., Bierut, L. J., Rohrbaugh, J. W., Statham, D. J., Dunne, M. P., Whitfield, J. B. and Martin, N. G. (1999) Genetic differences in alcohol sensitivity and the inheritance of alcoholism risk. Psychological Medicine 29, 10691081.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Hesselbrock, V., Bauer, L., Hesselbrock, M. and Gillen, R. (1991) Neuropsychological factors in individuals at high risk for alcoholism. In Recent Developments in Alcoholism, Vol. 9, Galanter, M. ed., pp. 2140. Plenum Press, New York.
Hesselbrock, M., Easton, C., Bucholz, K. K., Schuckit, M. and Hesselbrock, V. (1999) A validity study of the SSAGAa comparison with the SCAN. Addiction 94, 13611370.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Hill, S. Y., Lowers, L., Locke-Wellman, J. and Shen, S. (2000) Maternal smoking and drinking during pregnancy and the risk for child and adolescent psychiatric disorders. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 61, 661668.[ISI][Medline]
Hu, L. and Bentler, P. M. (1998) Fit indices in covariance structure analysis: Sensitivity to underparametersized model misspecification. Psychological Methods 4, 321.[ISI]
Jacob, T. and Leonard, K. (1994) Family and peer influences in the development of adolescent alcohol abuse. In Development of Alcohol Problems: Exploring the Biopsychosocial Matrix of Risk (NIAAA Monograph No. 26), Zucker, R., Boyd, G. and Howard, J. eds, pp. 123156, NIAAA, Bethesda, MD.
Jacob, T. and Windle, M. (2000) Young adult children of alcoholic, depressed and nondistressed parents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 61, 836844.[ISI][Medline]
Jessor, R. and Jessor, S. L. (1977) Problem Behavior and Psychosocial Development: A Longitudinal Study of Youth, San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Johnson, J. L., Rolf, J. E., Tiegel, S. and McDuff, D. (2000) Developmental assessment of children of alcoholics. In Children of Alcoholics: Selected Readings, Vol. II, Abbott, S. ed., pp. 265287. National Association of Children of Alcoholics, Rockville, MD.
Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M. and Backman, J. G. (2001) Monitoring the Future 19752000. Vol. 1: Secondary School Students. National Institutes of Health Publication No. 014927. National Institute on Drug Abuse, Bethesda, MD.
Jöreskog, K. G. (1993) Testing structural equation models. In Testing Structural Equation Models, Bollen, K. A. and Long, J. S. eds, pp. 294316. Sage Publications, Newbury Park, CA.
Jöreskog, K. G. and Sörbom, D. (1989) LISREL 7 User's Reference Guide. Scientific Software, Mooresville, IN.
Kessler, R. C., Nelson, C. B., McGonalke, K. A., Eclund, M. J., Frank, R. G. and Leaf, P. J. (1996) The epidemiology of co-occurring addictive and mental disorders: Implications for prevention and service utilization. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 66, 1731.[ISI][Medline]
Kuperman, S., Schlosser, S. S., Kramer, J. R., Bucholz, K., Hesselbrock, V., Reich, T. and Reich, W. (2001) Risk domains associated with an adolescent alcohol dependence diagnosis. Addiction 96, 629636.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Labouvie, E. and Bates, M. (2002) Reasons for alcohol use in young adulthood: Validation of a three-dimensional measure. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 63, 145155.[ISI][Medline]
McGue, M. (1997) A behavioral-genetic perspective on children of alcoholics. Alcohol Health and Research World 21, 210217.
McGue, M., Slutske, W., Taylor, J. and Iacono, W. G. (1997) Personality and substance use disorders: I. Effects of gender and alcoholism subtype. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 21, 513520.[ISI][Medline]
Merikangas, K. R., Stevens, D. and Fenton, B. (1996) Comorbidity of alcoholism and anxiety disorders: the role of family studies. Alcohol Health and Research World 20, 100106.
Newcomb, M. (1997) Psychosocial predictors and consequences of drug use: a developmental perspective within a prospective study. Journal of Addictive Diseases 16, 5189.
Pandina, R. J., Johnson, R. and Labouvie, E. W. (1992) Affectivity: A central mechanism in the development of drug dependence, in Vulnerability to Drug Abuse, edited by Glantz, M. and Pickens, R., pp 179209. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
Pihl, R. and Bruce, K. (1995) Cognitive impairment in children of alcoholics. Alcohol Health and Research World 19, 142147.
Prescott, C. A. and Kendler, K. S. (1999) Genetic and environmental contributions to alcohol abuse and dependence in a population-based sample of male twins. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 3440.
Preuss, U. W., Schuckit, M. A., Smith, T. L., Barnow, S. and Danko, G. P. (2002) Mood and anxiety symptoms among 140 children from alcoholic and control families. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 67, 235242.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Puttler, L. I., Zucker, R. A., Fitzgerald, H. E. and Bingham, C. R. (1998) Behavioral outcomes among children of alcoholics during the early and middle childhood years: familial subtype variations. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 22, 19621972.[ISI][Medline]
Regier, D. A., Boyd, J. H., Burke, J. D., Jr, Rae, D. S., Myers, J. K., Kramer, M., Robins, L. N., George, L. K., Karno, M. and Locke, B. Z. (1988) One-month prevalence of mental disorders in the United States. Archives of General Psychiatry 45, 977986.[Abstract]
Reich, W. (1988) Home Environment Interview for Children (HEIC). Washington University School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry, St Louis, MO.
Reich, W., Earls, F., Frankel, O. and Shayka, J. J. (1993) Psychopathology in children of alcoholics. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 32, 9951002.[ISI][Medline]
Schuckit, M. A. (1998) Biological, psychological and environmental predictors of the alcoholism risk: A longitudinal study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 59, 485494.[ISI][Medline]
Schuckit, M. A. (2000) Drug and Alcohol Abuse: A Clinical Guide to Diagnosis and Treatment, 5th edn, Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, New York.
Schuckit, M. A. (2002) Vulnerability factors for alcoholism. In Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress, Davis, K. ed., pp. 13991411. American College of Neuropsychopharmacology and Lippincott/Williams & Wilkins, Philadelphia.
Schuckit, M. A. and Smith, T. L. (1996) An 8-year follow-up of 450 sons of alcoholic and control subjects. Archives of General Psychiatry 53, 202210.[Abstract]
Schuckit, M. A. and Smith, T. L. (2000) The relationships of a family history of alcohol dependence, a low level of response to alcohol and six domains of life functioning to the development of AUD. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 61, 827835.[ISI][Medline]
Schuckit, M. A., Smith, T. L., Radziminski, S. and Heyneman, E. K. (2000) Behavioral symptoms and psychiatric diagnoses among 162 children in nonalcoholic or alcoholic families. American Journal of Psychiatry 1157, 18811883.[CrossRef]
Schuckit, M. A., Smith, T. L., Danko, G. P., Reich, T., Bucholz, K. K. and Bierut, L. J. (2002) Similarities in the clinical characteristics related to alcohol dependence in two populations. American Journal on Addictions 11, 19.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Schuckit, M. A., Tipp, J. E., Bergman, M., Reich, W., Hesselbrock, V. M. and Smith, T. L. (1997) Comparison of induced and independent major depressive disorders in 2,945 alcoholics. American Journal of Psychiatry 154, 949957.
Schumacker, R. E. and Lomax, R. G. (1996) A Beginner's Guide to Structural Equation Modeling. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.
Sher, K. J. (1991) Children of Alcoholics: A Critical Appraisal of Theory and Research. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Sher, K. J. (1997) Psychological characteristics of children of alcoholics. Alcohol Health and Research World 21, 247254.
Slutske, W. S., Heath, A. C., Dinwiddie, S. H., Madden, P. A. F., Bucholz, K. K., Dunne, M. P., Statham, D. J. and Martin, N. G. (1998) Common genetic risk factors for conduct disorder and alcohol dependence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 107, 363374.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Spitzer, R. L. and Endicott, J. (1977) Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia. New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York.
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., Gibbon, M. and First, M. D. (1992) The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID). Archives of General Psychiatry 49, 624629.[Abstract]
Steiger, J. H. (1990) Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research 25, 173180.[ISI]
Tapert, S. F., Brown, S. A., Myers, M. G. and Granholm, E. (1999) The role of neurocognitive abilities in coping with adolescent relapse to alcohol and drug use. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 60, 500508.[ISI][Medline]
Tarter, R. E. (2002) Etiology of adolescent substance abuse: A developmental perspective. American Journal of Addictions 11, 171191.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Tarter, R. E. and Vanyukov, M. (1994) Alcoholism: A developmental disorder. Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychology 62, 10961107.[CrossRef][ISI][Medline]
Twitchell, G. R., Hanna, G. L., Cook, E. H., Fitzgerald, H. E., Little, K. Y. and Zucker, R. A. (1998) Overt behavior problems and serotonergic function in middle childhood among male and female offspring of alcoholic fathers. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 22, 13401348.[ISI][Medline]
van Os, J., Jones, P., Lewis, G., Wadsworth, M. and Murray, R. (1997) Developmental precursors of affective illness in a general population birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry 54, 625631.[Abstract]
Windle, M. (2000) Parental, sibling, and peer influences on adolescent substance use and alcohol problems. Applied Developmental Science 4, 98110.
Zucker, R. A., Ellis, D. A., Bingham, C. R. and Fitzgerald, H. E. (1996) The development of alcoholic subtypes: risk variation among alcoholic families during the early childhood years. Alcohol Health and Research World 20, 4654.
Zucker, R. A., Ellis, D., Bingham, C. R. and Fitzgerald, H. E. (2000) The development of alcoholic subtypes risk variation among alcoholic families during the early childhood years. In Children of Alcoholics: Selected Readings, Vol. II, Abbott, S. ed., pp. 243264. National Association of Children of Alcoholics, Rockville, MD.