SUBJECTIVE RATINGS OF PROSPECTIVE MEMORY DEFICITS IN CHRONIC HEAVY ALCOHOL USERS

Thomas M. Heffernan,*, Mark Moss and Jonathon Ling1

Human Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, Division of Psychology, University of Northumbria, Newcastle upon Tyne NE1 8ST and
1 Psychology Section, University of Teesside, Middlesborough TS1 3BA, UK

Received 30 July 2001; in revised form 28 September 2001; accepted 20 November 2001


    ABSTRACT
 TOP
 FOOTNOTES
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 SUBJECTS AND METHODS
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 REFERENCES
 
— Chronic alcohol abuse has a detrimental effect on retrospective memory. Less is known about its putative effects on everyday memory. This study looked at self-ratings of prospective memory (PM) (memory for future events). After controlling for other drug and strategy use, chronic heavy alcohol users showed global impairments in PM, when compared to matched controls. The underlying mechanisms are discussed.


    INTRODUCTION
 TOP
 FOOTNOTES
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 SUBJECTS AND METHODS
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 REFERENCES
 
Chronic heavy alcohol consumption leads to a number of neuropsychological impairments, including memory deficits. For example, studies have revealed cognitive impairments in problem-solving abilities and decision making (Leckliter and Matarazzo, 1989Go; Selby and Azrin, 1998Go), as well as in a range of memorial functioning. In terms of memory, chronic heavy alcohol users and alcohol-dependent persons show impaired performance on a range of memory tasks, for example learning word lists (Grant, 1987Go; Bachara et al., 2001Go), short- and long-term logical memory (Selby and Azrin, 1998Go), general working memory (Ambrose et al., 2001Go) and executive function (Wendt and Risberg, 2001Go). Such research into memory dysfunction has tended to focus on laboratory and/or field tests of retrospective memory — encompassing the learning, consolidation, retention and retrieval of previously presented target material. However, few investigations have examined the extent to which these impairments impact upon memory functioning in an everyday context.

One important aspect of day-to-day memory function is prospective memory (PM), which refers to the process of remembering to do things at some future point in time (Brandimonte et al., 1996Go). For example, remembering to attend a particular function such as a party, or to carry out a particular task, such as remembering to pay a bill on time. PM has only recently been subject to systematic empirical research (e.g. Brandimonte et al., 1996Go; Ellis et al., 1999Go). The Prospective Memory Questionnaire (PMQ), developed by Hannon et al. (1995) is a self-rating scale that requires participants to record the number of times their prospective memory has failed them within a period of time. Three sub-scales provide self-reported measures of short-term habitual, long-term episodic, and internally-cued prospective memory. In addition, the PMQ gauges the number of strategies used to aid remembering via the Techniques to Remember Scale. The scale has proved to be a useful tool in estimating the effectiveness of PM in a number of settings, including assessing the impact of personality differences (Heffernan and Ling, 2001Go), age-related differences (Heffernan and Elmirghani, 2000Go), as a neuropsychological instrument in the study of brain damaged patients (Hannon et al., 1995Go), and has been used to explore self-rated prospective memory impairments in regular ecstasy users (Heffernan et al., 2001Go).

There is some evidence to suggest that chronic heavy alcohol users show detriments in remembering within an everyday context (Knight and Godfrey, 1985Go). Given this and the evidence that retrospective memory is impaired in this group, one might expect that they would report more impairments in prospective memory when compared to a sample of low-dose/non-alcohol users. The present study aimed to extend our knowledge on potential memory deficits resulting from heavy alcohol use, focusing here on self-rated errors of prospective memory. If chronic heavy alcohol consumption does have an adverse effect on prospective memory, then one would expect this group to report significantly greater errors in their prospective memory functioning when compared to a low dose/alcohol-free group


    SUBJECTS AND METHODS
 TOP
 FOOTNOTES
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 SUBJECTS AND METHODS
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 REFERENCES
 
The current Government recommended number of units of alcohol consumption per week is 28 units for males and 21 for females (Institute of Alcohol Consumption, 2001). Thirty chronic heavy alcohol users (16 females and 14 males; mean age: 23.3 ± 4.5 years), defined as ingesting above the recommended weekly number of units in the UK over a period of 5 years or more and thirty low-dose/alcohol-free controls (19 females and 11 males; mean age 21.1 ± 7.7 years), defined as ingesting below the recommended weekly number of units over 5 years or more (with 4 of these participants being non-drinkers) were compared. The range of alcohol consumption for the chronic heavy alcohol user group was 30–90 units per week and the range of alcohol consumption for the low dose/ non-alcohol group was 0–20 units per week. The participants were undergraduate students studying in the North East of England. The drinking participants reported that they: (1) were social drinkers; (2) had been drinking alcohol for at least 5 years; (3) had been completely drug free for at least 48 h; (4) had not been diagnosed as alcohol dependent; (5) were not suffering from any form of amnesia. Other drug use was assessed by a questionnaire gauging the number of times ecstasy, LSD, marijuana and cocaine were consumed per week. None of the participants used ecstasy, cocaine or LSD, but some used marijuana (n = 8 in the chronic heavy alcohol group and n = 5 in the low dose/no-alcohol group).

Prospective memory (PM) was assessed using the Prospective Memory Questionnaire (PMQ), which is a valid and reliable self-report measure (Hannon et al., 1995Go). The PMQ provides measures of three aspects of PM on a series of nine-point scales. Fourteen questions measure short-term habitual PM, e.g. ‘I forgot to turn my alarm clock off when I got up this morning’). Fourteen items measure long-term episodic PM, e.g. ‘I forgot to pass on a message to someone’. Ten questions measure internally cued PM, e.g. ‘I forgot what I wanted to say in the middle of a sentence’. The PMQ provides a measure of self-reported errors in the previous week, or month or year, depending upon the specific questionnaire item. The scale ranges from 1 (where least forgetting is evident) to 9 (where there is a great deal of forgetting); the greater the score, the more faulty one's prospective memory. Additionally, 14 questions make up the ‘techniques to remember’ scale, providing a measure of the number of strategies used to aid remembering. The Techniques to Remember Scale ranges from 1 (few strategies used) to 9 (a high number of strategies used). On this latter scale, the greater the score, the more memory aids used. Completion of the PMQ preceded the drug-use questionnaire.


    RESULTS
 TOP
 FOOTNOTES
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 SUBJECTS AND METHODS
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 REFERENCES
 
The results of the study are summarized in Table 1Go. Two one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) showed that there were no significant differences between the groups in terms of their ages, nor in terms of the number of strategies used. A one-way ANOVA confirmed that the heavy alcohol group consumed significantly greater amounts of alcohol than the low-dose/no alcohol control group. A final one-way ANOVA showed that the ‘heavy’ alcohol group consumed significantly greater amounts of marijuana than the low alcohol control group.


View this table:
[in this window]
[in a new window]
 
Table 1. Comparison of measures between chronic heavy alcohol users and low-dose/no alcohol controls
 
Amounts of marijuana used per week were incorporated into analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) applied to the data from each sub-scale. The analysis of covariance was used because it allowed for statistical control of the other drug usage by entering the data for each of the other drugs used as a covariate, a method used in previous studies on substance use and cognition (Heffernan et al., 2001Go). These revealed a significantly greater number of impairments reported by the heavy alcohol group, when compared to the low/no alcohol control group, in terms of their long-term episodic PM, their short-term habitual PM, and their internally cued PM (Table 1Go). It is concluded from these results that heavy alcohol users report global impairments in their everyday prospective memory when compared to a matched control group.


    DISCUSSION
 TOP
 FOOTNOTES
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 SUBJECTS AND METHODS
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 REFERENCES
 
The findings from the present study demonstrate that, compared to the low-dose/non-alcohol group, chronic heavy alcohol users reported global impairments in prospective memory. Specifically, the chronic heavy alcohol group reported significantly greater levels of prospective forgetting for long-term, short-term, and internally cued prospective memory. The self-reported deficits in this group persisted, even after controlling for the use of other drugs and the number of strategies used to aid remembering. These findings are novel and add weight to the growing body of evidence that suggests that chronic heavy alcohol use impairs cognitive function, and in particular memory (e.g. Grant, 1987Go; Selby and Azrin, 1998Go; Ambrose et al., 2001Go; Bachara et al., 2001Go; Wendt and Risberg, 2001Go). These results suggest that prospective memory — an important aspect of everyday cognitive functioning — should be included in the list of neuropsychological sequelae resulting from a history of heavy alcohol consumption. The statistical similarity between the two groups in terms of their use of strategies to aid remembering suggests that the heavy chronic alcohol users are either unaware or are not compensating for their memory deficits.

The mechanisms underlying the range of cognitive impairments associated with chronic heavy alcohol use are not fully understood at present. It is known that alcohol causes brain shrinkage, particularly in alcohol-dependent people, where the damage induced may be permanent (Kril and Halliday, 1998Go). Alcohol has been found to reduce the number of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain leading to reduced hippocampal function — a structure heavily implicated in memory consolidation (Garcia-Moreno et al., 2001Go). Alcohol also appears to inhibit prefrontal lobe functioning (Wendt and Risberg, 2001Go). Prospective memory may also be under the influence of pre-frontal and frontal cortical control (Okuda et al., 1998Go; McDaniel et al., 1999Go), and prospective memory performance is strongly correlated with frontal lobe executive processes (Shapiro et al., 1998Go). It seems possible, therefore, that damage in the pre-frontal and frontal regions of the brain may be responsible for the self-reported deficits observed in the present study. Although this is feasible, alternative explanations cannot be ruled out, such as the role of the hippocampus in prospective memory and/or the putative depletion of specific neurotransmitter substances known to impact upon mnemonic processes, such as serotonin (Spoont, 1992Go; Hunter, 2000Go). It seems quite possible that a complex interaction exists between the effects of excessive alcohol use, regional brain functioning and neurotransmitter depletion.

Although the use of other drugs did not statistically affect heavy alcohol-related prospective memory impairments, one cannot rule out the possibility that biological interactions between alcohol and other drugs may contribute to the effects seen here. One way to address this issue would be to include a ‘heavy alcohol-only ’ group who were otherwise drug free, in addition to a matched control group. It is also necessary to consider the use of self-report measures in studies of this kind. The reliance on self-reports of remembered errors in a group where memorial deficits are already known raises the possibility of a ‘memory paradox’, in which heavy alcohol users may forget their memory lapses. But, given the direction and strength of the group differences found in the present study it could be argued that, if anything, this possibility adds strength to the present findings. It is likely that alcohol users may well have underestimated their memory deficits.

These findings have implications on the potentially harmful effects of chronic heavy alcohol use. Future research may wish to employ more objective methods for assessing prospective memory, such as laboratory based prospective memory tasks, or perhaps, video simulations which assess prospective remembering (Titov and Knight, 2001Go). It is clear that further research is needed to clarify the relationship between chronic heavy alcohol use, impairments in prospective memory and the neuropsychological basis for such impairments, such as the cortical and sub-cortical regions involved in these processes.


    FOOTNOTES
 TOP
 FOOTNOTES
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 SUBJECTS AND METHODS
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 REFERENCES
 
* Author to whom correspondence should be addressed. Back


    REFERENCES
 TOP
 FOOTNOTES
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 SUBJECTS AND METHODS
 RESULTS
 DISCUSSION
 REFERENCES
 
Ambrose, M. L., Bowden, S. C. and Whelan, G. (2001) Working memory impairments in alcohol-dependent participants without clinical amnesia. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 25, 185–191.[ISI][Medline]

Bachara, A., Dolan, S., Denburg, N., Hindes, A., Anderson, S. W. and Nathan, P. E. (2001) Decision-making deficits, linked to dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia 39, 376–388.[ISI][Medline]

Brandimonte, M., Einstein, G. O. and McDaniel, M. A. (1996) Prospective Memory: Theory and Applications. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, New Jersey.

Ellis, J., Kvavilashvili, L. and Milne, A. (1999) Experimental tests of prospective remembering: the influence of cue-event frequency on performance. British Journal of Psychology 90, 9–23.[ISI][Medline]

Garcia-Moreno, L. M., Conejo, N. M., Pardo, H. G., Gomez, M., Martin, F. R., Alonso, M. J. and Arias, J. L. (2001) Hippocampal AgNOR activity after chronic alcohol consumption and alcohol deprivation in rats. Physiology and Behaviour 72, 115–221.[ISI][Medline]

Grant, I. (1987) Alcohol and the brain: neuropsychological correlates. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 55, 310–324.[ISI][Medline]

Hannon, R., Adams, P., Harrington, S., Fries-Dias, C. and Gibson, M. T. (1995) Effects of brain injury and age on prospective memory self-rating and performance. Rehabilitation Psychology 40, 289–297.[ISI]

Heffernan, T. M. and Elmirghani, M. (2000) Individual differences in prospective memory: the impact of personality and age. International Journal of Psychology 35, p. 432.

Heffernan, T. M. and Ling, J. (2001) The impact of Eysenck's Extraversion–Introversion personality dimension on prospective memory. Scandanavian Journal of Psychology 42, 321–325.

Heffernan, T. M., Ling, J. and Scholey, A. (2001) Subjective ratings of prospective memory in ecstasy users. Human Psychopharmacology 16, 339–344.[ISI][Medline]

Hunter, A. (2000) Serotonergic involvement in learning and memory. Biochemical Society Transactions 17, 79–81.

Institute of Alcohol Consumption (Downloaded 16 July, 2001) UK alcohol consumption. http://www.eurocare.org/profiles/uk/ukconsumption.htm

Knight, R. G. and Godfrey, H. P. (1985) The assessment of memory impairment: the relationship between different methods of evaluating dysmnesic deficits. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology 24, 25–31.[ISI][Medline]

Kril, J. J. and Halliday, G. M. (1998) Brain shrinkage in alcoholics: a decade on and what have we learned? Progress in Neurobiology 58, 381–387.[ISI]

Leckliter, I. N. and Matarazzo, J. D. (1989) The influence of age, education, IQ, gender, and alcohol abuse on Halstead–Reitan neuropsychological test battery performance. Journal of Clinical Psychology 45, 485–512.

McDaniel, M. A., Glisky, E. L., Guynn, M. J. and Rotherieaux, B. C. (1999) Prospective memory: a neuropsychological study. Neuropsychology 13, 103–110.[ISI][Medline]

Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Yamadori, A., Kawashima, R., Tsukiura, T., Fukatsu, R., Suzuki, K., Ito, M. and Fukuda, H. (1998) Participation of the prefrontal cortices in prospective memory: evidence from a PET study in humans. Neuroscience Letters 253, 127–130.[ISI][Medline]

Selby, M. J. and Azrin, R. L. (1998) Neuropsychological functioning in drug abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 50, 39–45.[ISI][Medline]

Shapiro, J. K., Shapiro, L. R., Russell, S. and Alper, J. S. (1998) A role of executive processes in prospective remembering. Brain and Cognition 39, 1–3.[ISI]

Spoont, M. R. (1992) Modulatory role of serotonin in neural information processing: implications for human psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin 112, 330–350.[ISI][Medline]

Titov, N. and Knight, R. G. (2001) A video-based procedure for the assessment of prospective memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology 15, 61–83.[ISI]

Wendt, P. E. and Risberg, J. (2001) Ethanol reduces cCFB activation of left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during a verbal fluency task. Brain and Language 77, 197–215.[ISI][Medline]





This Article
Abstract
FREE Full Text (PDF)
Alert me when this article is cited
Alert me if a correction is posted
Services
Email this article to a friend
Similar articles in this journal
Similar articles in ISI Web of Science
Similar articles in PubMed
Alert me to new issues of the journal
Add to My Personal Archive
Download to citation manager
Search for citing articles in:
ISI Web of Science (2)
Request Permissions
Google Scholar
Articles by Heffernan, T. M.
Articles by Ling, J.
PubMed
PubMed Citation
Articles by Heffernan, T. M.
Articles by Ling, J.